Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T16:44:50.030Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Aggressive behaviours associated with MDMA and psychedelics: a narrative review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2024

Negar Sayrafizadeh
Affiliation:
Centre for Complex Interventions, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health (CAMH), Toronto, ON, Canada
Nicole Ledwos
Affiliation:
Centre for Complex Interventions, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health (CAMH), Toronto, ON, Canada
M. Ishrat Husain
Affiliation:
Campbell Family Mental Health Research Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, ON, Canada Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
David J. Castle*
Affiliation:
Centre for Complex Interventions, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health (CAMH), Toronto, ON, Canada Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
*
Corresponding author: David J. Castle; Email: david.castle@ths.tas.gov.au
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Monoamine neurotransmitters play a role in aggression, especially when altered by illicit substances. However, some literature suggests that not all illicit substances may lead to aggression, notably psychedelics. This narrative review investigates the associations between serotonergic psychedelics and MDMA on aggressive behaviour.

Methods:

PubMed and PsycINFO were searched for original, peer-reviewed articles evaluating the effects of serotonergic psychedelics and 3,4-methyl enedioxy methamphetamine (MDMA) on violent and aggressive behaviour using Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines.

Results:

After removing duplicates, a total of 555 articles were screened, with 16 meeting the inclusion criteria. One additional article was obtained through reference screening bringing the total to 17 articles. Of these 17 articles, 14 studies focused on MDMA and three on serotonergic psychedelics. Findings were mixed, with some results demonstrating increased aggression following psychedelics and others suggesting protective effects. Limitations in the current literature include varied definitions of psychedelics, lack of standardised objective outcome measures and failure to control for confounding.

Conclusion:

As psychedelic research continues to expand, further assessment on the effects of serotonergic psychedelics and MDMA on aggressive behaviour is required.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Scandinavian College of Neuropsychopharmacology

Significant outcomes

  • A brief history of psychedelic drugs, their different classes and their mechanism of action on aggressive behaviour are presented.

  • There is a limited number of studies that investigate associations between psychedelic use and aggressive behaviour.

  • The included studies share similar shortcomings that must be addressed in future research to improve understanding on associations between psychedelic use and aggressive behaviour.

Limitations

  • This review included contemporary studies on associations between psychedelic use and aggression and does not include all available literature.

  • This review excludes literature with indirect outcome variables related to violence or aggression.

Introduction

Psychedelic drugs are most notoriously associated with the counterculture movement of the mid-twentieth century when reports of lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) and ‘magic mushrooms’ (psilocybin) made their way into mainstream media. Plant-based psychedelics have been used for hundreds of years in indigenous communities for religious and ceremonial purposes (Garcia-Romeu et al., Reference Garcia-Romeu, Kersgaard and Addy2016). From the 1950s to the 1970s, some patients were treated using psychedelic drugs through clinical research trials (Garcia-Romeu et al., Reference Garcia-Romeu, Kersgaard and Addy2016; Rucke et al., Reference Rucker, Iliff and Nutt2018). Results from these early studies indicated that psychedelics might enhance traditional psychotherapy, with some participants demonstrating clinical improvement (Rucker et al., Reference Rucker, Iliff and Nutt2018). However, in the United States (US) in the 1970s, the Controlled Substances Act signed by the Nixon administration categorised psychedelics as Schedule 1 substances, halting most research (Carhart-Harris and Goodwin, Reference Carhart-Harris and Goodwin2017). In the late 1970s, 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) started to gain popularity among psychiatrists due to the belief that patient insight and communication improved as a result of combining MDMA with therapy (Grinspoon and Bakalar, Reference Grinspoon and Bakalar1986); MDMA also started to become a popular recreational drug (Parrott, Reference Parrott2001; Passie, Reference Passie2018). In 1985, the US Drug Enforcement Administration banned MDMA and placed it among Schedule 1 drugs (DEA 2018).

‘Psychedelic’ is an umbrella term for several drugs traditionally categorised by their neurobiological mechanism of action and chemical structure. So-called ‘classic’ psychedelics include serotonin receptor agonists such as LSD, psilocybin, peyote, ayahuasca and N,N-dimethyltryptamine (DMT) (Nichols, Reference Nichols1986; Garcia-Romeu et al., Reference Garcia-Romeu, Kersgaard and Addy2016) However, other pathways and receptors have been implicated in the mechanism of action for some of the drugs including mescaline and DMT. Evidence suggests that mescaline influences dopaminergic activity and 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) levels in the brain (Dinis-Oliveira et al., Reference Dinis-Oliveira, Pereira and da Silva2019). DMT has been found to have an affinity for mGluR2 glutamate, sigma-1, the α1- and α2-adrenergic receptors, as well as the dopamine D1 receptor (Hamill et al., Reference Hamill, Hallak, Dursun and Baker2019). Substances such as 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) that combine the catecholaminergic effects of methamphetamine with the serotonergic effects of psychedelics have been categorised as empathogens. Empathogens typically produce psychedelic-like effects without hallucinations (Nichols, Reference Nichols1986).

Classic psychedelics, by definition, are 5-HT2A receptor agonists that act primarily as full or partial agonists of the serotonin (5-HT) 2A receptors (Nichols, Reference Nichols2016). Several studies suggest that classic psychedelics increase communication between multiple disparate brain regions while decreasing activity in the default mode network (DMN) (Nichols, Reference Nichols2016). This pattern may explain the sense of unity and ego dissolution often associated with the psychedelic experience (Nichols, Reference Nichols2016). MDMA, on the other hand, acts through increasing release of serotonin, noradrenaline, and dopamine and is also associated with release of oxytocin, which might explain some of its empathogenic effect (Liechti et al., Reference Liechti, Baumann, Gamma and Vollenweider2000; Dunlap et al., Reference Dunlap, Andrews and Olson2018). MDMA alters blood flow to brain regions involved in emotion formation, processing and behavioural learning and may be particularly important in regulating fear-based behaviours (Vollenweider, Reference Vollenweider2022). Taken together, these drugs impact several areas of the brain that play central roles in human behaviour and emotional processing. However, there are mixed findings regarding the association between psychedelic use and aggression/violence. The most frequent explanation for violence and aggression is related to low serotonin levels and, as mentioned earlier, this is not found to be a neurobiological effect of classical psychedelics (Boles and Miotto, Reference Boles and Miotto2003). Instead, feelings of invincibility and ego dissolution can be part of the psychedelic experience (Nichols, Reference Nichols2016). These feelings of invincibility may explain some users’ aggressive or violent outbursts following use. On the other hand, there is some evidence to suggest that, although hallucinogenic substances do not usually trigger violent behaviours per se, they may exacerbate the effects of underlying psychopathologies, which can lead to aggression; this has been described in studies of LSD, for example (Reiss and Roth, Reference Reiss and Roth1993). On a similar note, there is evidence to suggest that paranoid ideation can increase violent behaviour (Mojtabai, Reference Mojtabai2006; Coid et al., Reference Coid, Ullrich, Bebbington, Fazel and Keers2016). Given that paranoia is found to be one of the side effects of psychedelic use (Schlag et al., Reference Schlag, Aday, Salam, Neill and Nutt2022), it can be inferred that this side effect could possibly result in violent behaviour in users.

As research into the therapeutic use of psychedelics expands, the association between these compounds and violent or aggressive behaviour is important to consider especially given the mixed findings mentioned above. Substance use is frequently involved in violent behaviour, with a substantial proportion of incarcerated violent offenders being substance-involved (Boles and Miotto, Reference Boles and Miotto2003). Alcohol, cocaine and phencyclidine (PCP) may trigger violent behaviour (Boles and Miotto, Reference Boles and Miotto2003). Alcohol, cocaine and psychedelics, among others, impact the monoamine neurotransmitters (serotonin, dopamine, norepinephrine) involved in behaviour regulation. However, the link between psychoactive substances and behaviour differ by drug type, the amount and pattern of use, as well as individual characteristics such as personality. For instance, chronic use of illicit substances has been found to alter the normal functioning of the nervous system, effects that can impact social communication and emotional processing; such factors could underpin aggressive behaviours (Boles and Miotto, Reference Boles and Miotto2003).

Furthermore, pre-existing aggressive conditions in childhood, prior to the onset of substance use, may also explain why some individuals’ aggressive and violent tendencies are exacerbated following substance use (Boles and Miotto, Reference Boles and Miotto2003). The prediction of violent behaviour in the context of substance use is highly complex, given the multiple contexts (e.g. environmental, social, situational and cultural) that impact violent outcomes as well as inter-individual differences in physiology, psychology, personality, personal history, sex and gender (Boles and Miotto, Reference Boles and Miotto2003).

There are also many methodological constraints in studies of substance use and aggression/violence. For one, illicit substances are often grouped together and/or used together, making it difficult to draw conclusions about the effects specific substances (Boles and Miotto, Reference Boles and Miotto2003). Furthermore, various stages of substance use, such as acute intoxication, withdrawal and drug-seeking behaviour, may lead to different types of violence and aggression (Boles and Miotto, Reference Boles and Miotto2003).

The primary aim of this narrative review is to investigate the current literature on the associations between psychedelics and violent and aggressive behaviours and to explore potential mechanisms for any associations. We included all classic psychedelics and MDMA, as these are drugs of current interest in psychiatry (Nutt and Castle Reference Nutt and Castle2022).

Methods

Search strategy

As this is a narrative review, only PubMed and PsycINFO databases were searched. We included peer-reviewed, original studies written in English published between the years 2000 through to the end of July 2022. This period was chosen because it covers the ‘modern’ era of research involving psychedelic agents. The following search terms were employed: ‘psychedelic*’ OR ‘MDMA’ OR ‘3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine’ OR ‘Psilocybin’ OR ‘Psiloci’ OR ‘LSD’ OR ‘Lysergic acid diethylamide’ OR ‘Mescaline’ OR ‘DMT’ OR ‘N,N-Dimethyltryptamine’ OR ‘5-MeO-DMT’ OR ‘Ayahuasca’ AND ‘Aggressive behavior?r’ OR ‘Violent behavior?r’ OR ‘Criminal behavior?r’ OR ‘Aggress*’ OR ‘Violen*’. These keywords were searched along with relevant search terms adapted to each database. Reference lists of included articles were checked for any publications that might have been missed by our search strategy.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Following the PICO framework, the criteria for selecting the studies were as follows: population (P): Participants 16 years of age or older with a history of lifetime psychedelic use; Intervention (I): Psychedelic use; Comparison (C): No comparison or studies including groups of participants with other substance use disorders or a control group which includes participants with no substance use disorder; Outcomes (O): Any form of aggressive or violent behaviour (e,g., physical, verbal, sexual) as either the primary or secondary outcome, established using validated measures (e.g. Conflicts Tactics Scale) or incarceration records.

The exclusion criteria were: 1) studies that did not include a group of participants with lifetime psychedelic use or were individuals<16 years of age; 2) abstracts for ongoing trials, conference abstracts, dissertations, opinions and commentaries. There were no restrictions on setting or language, and efforts were made to locate an English version of articles published in a different language.

Data extraction

All studies generated from the search were uploaded into the Covidence software (Veritas Health Innovation, 2022), where duplicate studies were automatically removed. Two independent reviewers (N.S. & N.L.) screened the titles and abstracts for all studies against the pre-defined inclusion and exclusion criteria. Both reviewers obtained and reviewed full texts for all selected studies (N.S. & N.L.). Any conflicts at the title/abstract screening stage and the full-text review stage were resolved by a third independent reviewer (DC). Data extraction was conducted by one reviewer (NS) and included the following information: 1) author and publication year; 2) study design; 3) sample size and demographic information; 4) type of psychedelic; 5) outcome measures; 6) study results. This review was registered on the Open Science Framework (DOI 10.17605/OSF.IO/HRNG7) database.

Results

The search identified a total of 848 studies. Following the removal of 293 duplicates, 555 studies progressed to the title and abstract review phase (See Fig. 1). Of the 848 articles, 53 were selected for full-text screening and 16 met our eligibility criteria. By reviewing the citation list of one relevant study (Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ampollini, Giusti, Delsignore, Raggi, Laviola, Macchia and Brambilla2001), one additional study was found to be eligible, bringing the total number of included studies to 17. A complete summary of each trial is outlined in Table 1.

Figure 1. PRISMA 2020 flow diagram for new systematic reviews which included searches of databases and registers only. From: Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al., The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021;372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71. For more information, visit: http://www.prisma-statement.org/.

Table 1. Summary of trials

The sample sizes of included studies ranged from 30 to 19,067 participants. A majority of the studies (N = 10) were conducted in Europe. Several studies included only males (N = 9) and did not report on ethnicity (N = 10). Sixteen studies focused on ages 16–50 (Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ferri, Zambelli, Timpano, Neri, Marzocchi, Delsignore and Brambilla2000; Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ampollini, Giusti, Delsignore, Raggi, Laviola, Macchia and Brambilla2001; Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Hadfield, Calin and Curran2002; Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Maidment and Curran2003; Bond et al., Reference Bond, Verheyden, Wingrove and Curran2004; Curran et al., Reference Curran, Rees, Hoare, Hoshi and Bond2004; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Bisla and Curran2004; Hendrickson and Gerstein, Reference Hendrickson and Gerstein2005; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Pratt, Mehta, Bond and Curran2006; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Cohen, Lemanski, Piccini, Bond and Curran2007; Reid et al., Reference Reid, Elifson and Sterk2007; Feingold et al., Reference Feingold, Kerr and Capaldi2008; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Hides, Allen and Lubman2013; Vaughn et al., Reference Vaughn, Salas-Wright, DeLisi, Perron and Cordova2015; Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Hendricks, Smith, Kosson, Thiessen, Lucas and Swogger2016; Romero-Martínez et al., Reference Romero-Martínez, Lila and Moya-Albiol2019). However, one study included participants over the age of 50 (Thiessen et al., Reference Thiessen, Walsh, Bird and Lafrance2018). Of the 17 studies, 14 focused on MDMA (Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ferri, Zambelli, Timpano, Neri, Marzocchi, Delsignore and Brambilla2000; Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ampollini, Giusti, Delsignore, Raggi, Laviola, Macchia and Brambilla2001; Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Hadfield, Calin and Curran2002; Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Maidment and Curran2003; Bond et al., Reference Bond, Verheyden, Wingrove and Curran2004; Curran et al., Reference Curran, Rees, Hoare, Hoshi and Bond2004; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Bisla and Curran2004; Hendrickson and Gerstein, Reference Hendrickson and Gerstein2005; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Pratt, Mehta, Bond and Curran2006; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Cohen, Lemanski, Piccini, Bond and Curran2007; Reid et al., Reference Reid, Elifson and Sterk2007; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Hides, Allen and Lubman2013; Vaughn et al., Reference Vaughn, Salas-Wright, DeLisi, Perron and Cordova2015; Romero-Martínez et al., Reference Romero-Martínez, Lila and Moya-Albiol2019). Out of the three non-MDMA studies, only one study (Thiessen et al., Reference Thiessen, Walsh, Bird and Lafrance2018) assessed classic psychedelics as the main substance of research focus whereas the other two grouped hallucinogens into a broad category encompassing both classic and non-classic psychedelics (Feingold et al., Reference Feingold, Kerr and Capaldi2008; Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Hendricks, Smith, Kosson, Thiessen, Lucas and Swogger2016). Most studies ascertained aggression or intimate partner violence (IPV) as outcomes, followed by criminal behaviour and aggressive cognitive bias. There is substantial heterogeneity concerning the instruments employed to measure these outcomes, as outlined below.

Included studies were grouped into three categories based on their outcome measures and results: the relationship between classic psychedelics and aggression, MDMA and criminality, and lastly, MDMA and aggression. A summary of the results can be found in Table 2.

Table 2. Summary of results

* IPV, Intimate Partner Violence.

Psychedelic/MDMA use (Independent Variable).

Anger and/or Aggression, Aggressive cognitive bias, IPV (Dependent Variable).

Classic psychedelics

One of the important predictors of IPV is reported to be substance use (Foran and O’Leary Reference Foran and O’Leary2008), but the association between substance use and violence varies across each class of drugs. In a previous study, inmates who met criteria for lifetime hallucinogen use disorder (HUD) (n = 22) were less than half as likely to be arrested for perpetrating IPV compared to those without lifetime hallucinogen use disorder (n = 280) (Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Hendricks, Smith, Kosson, Thiessen, Lucas and Swogger2016). These findings suggest that hallucinogenic drugs may be protective against IPV. The use of hallucinogens also predicted reduced arrest for IPV independently (β = −0.54, SE = 0.20, χ2 = 7.19, exp(B) = 0.58, p < 0.01) as well as when controlling for covariates (β = −0.48, SE = 0.23, χ2 = 4.44, exp(B) = 0.62, p < 0.05) (Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Hendricks, Smith, Kosson, Thiessen, Lucas and Swogger2016). Contrary to these findings, a study of an at-risk community sample of men (n = 150) found that hallucinogen use had the largest effect on IPV (η 2 = .09) compared to other hard drugs, even after controlling for antisocial personality (Feingold et al., Reference Feingold, Kerr and Capaldi2008).

Thus, published studies suggest that hallucinogens potentially have some protective effects against aggressive behaviours. However, two of the three studies combined classic psychedelics with other hallucinogens making it challenging to attribute this potential protective effect to one drug class. The only study (Thiessen et al., Reference Thiessen, Walsh, Bird and Lafrance2018) that reported specifically on classic psychedelics found that a lifetime history of classic psychedelic use (psilocybin and LSD) was inversely related to IPV perpetration in males (odds ratio = 0.42, p < 0.05). Interestingly, male psychedelic users reported better emotion regulation, which mediated the relationship between psychedelic use and IPV.

Gender differences

No concrete conclusions can be drawn regarding gender differences in relation to psychedelic use and aggression. Two of the three hallucinogen studies included only males in their sample (Feingold et al., Reference Feingold, Kerr and Capaldi2008; Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Hendricks, Smith, Kosson, Thiessen, Lucas and Swogger2016). Nevertheless, one study found that lifetime history of classic psychedelic use was inversely related to IPV perpetration in males (odds ratio = 0.42, p < 0.05) but not in females (odds ratio = 1.11; p > 0.05) (Thiessen et al., Reference Thiessen, Walsh, Bird and Lafrance2018).

Instruments of measurement

Variability in IPV data collection methods may influence the definition of IPV and its related components. One study used various methods (e.g. self-reports, partner reports, interviewer ratings, behavioural assessment) to gather information on injuries and physical and psychological aspects of IPV (Feingold et al., Reference Feingold, Kerr and Capaldi2008). In contrast, another used the revised short-form Conflicts Tactics Scale to measure couples psychological and physical aggression and pro-social negotiation tactics (Thiessen et al., Reference Thiessen, Walsh, Bird and Lafrance2018).

MDMA

MDMA and criminality

We identified four studies (Hendrickson and Gerstein, Reference Hendrickson and Gerstein2005; Reid et al., Reference Reid, Elifson and Sterk2007; Vaughn et al., Reference Vaughn, Salas-Wright, DeLisi, Perron and Cordova2015; Romero-Martínez et al., Reference Romero-Martínez, Lila and Moya-Albiol2019) investigating potential associations between MDMA use and IPV, criminality and violent behaviour. The first study had a sample of 17,558 subjects consisting of arrestees and a matched sample of men who had not been arrested. This team found that among arrestees, ecstasy users compared to non-ecstasy users, were less likely to be charged with assault (OR = 0.682, p < 0.10) or burglary (OR = 0.498, p < 0.05) but more likely to be charged with drug-related crimes (OR = 1.29, p < 0.05) (Hendrickson and Gerstein, Reference Hendrickson and Gerstein2005). The second study (n = 102) reported that among male perpetrators of IPV against women, MDMA use was related to the existence of a previous criminal record without violence (Romero-Martínez et al., Reference Romero-Martínez, Lila and Moya-Albiol2019). The third study (N = 19,067) found that, compared to non-MDMA users, MDMA users participated significantly more in criminal and violent behaviour, even after controlling for covariates such as lifetime substance use, sociodemographic factors and parental antisocial behaviours and substance use characteristics (Vaughn et al., Reference Vaughn, Salas-Wright, DeLisi, Perron and Cordova2015).Lastly, data collected from 260 ecstasy users showed higher levels of violent behaviour among users with high levels of lifetime ecstasy use (Reid et al., Reference Reid, Elifson and Sterk2007).

MDMA and aggression: Longevity of effects

Acute MDMA use is commonly associated with elevated mood, pro-social behaviour and enhanced empathy (Vollenweider et al., Reference Vollenweider, Gamma, Liechti and Huber1998). However, the rapid release of serotonin combined with the inhibition of tryptophan hydroxylase (TPH) can result in serotonin depletion in the long term (Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Wu and Lovenberg1986; McKenna and Peroutka, Reference McKenna and Peroutka1990). Low serotonin levels have been associated with depression and can modulate human aggression (da Cunha-Bang and Knudsen, Reference da Cunha-Bang and Knudsen2021). Several studies have investigated the impact MDMA has on aggression, both immediately and in the days following consumption. Three of the reviewed studies found no significant difference between MDMA users and control groups on trait aggression as measured by the Aggression Questionnaire (A.Q) (Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Hadfield, Calin and Curran2002; Curran et al., Reference Curran, Rees, Hoare, Hoshi and Bond2004; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Bisla and Curran2004). However, four studies (including the three previously discussed), did find that aggression, as measured by self-report, increased on day four post-use (Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Hadfield, Calin and Curran2002; Curran et al., Reference Curran, Rees, Hoare, Hoshi and Bond2004; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Bisla and Curran2004; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Pratt, Mehta, Bond and Curran2006). This is further supported by objective measures of aggression where MDMA users were faster at responding (Curran et al., Reference Curran, Rees, Hoare, Hoshi and Bond2004) and recognising (Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Pratt, Mehta, Bond and Curran2006) aggressive sentences compared to controls on day four post-use.

There is contradictory evidence regarding whether increased aggression following MDMA use is transient or persists. For example, Curran and colleagues (Curran et al., Reference Curran, Rees, Hoare, Hoshi and Bond2004) reported that self-rated aggression in ecstasy users had reverted to the same level as controls by day seven post-use. Similarly, Hoshi and colleagues (Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Cohen, Lemanski, Piccini, Bond and Curran2007), found that habitual ecstasy users abstinent for an average of two weeks did not report increased aggression compared to current users, polydrug users and drug-naïve controls. Furthermore, Gerra et al. (Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ferri, Zambelli, Timpano, Neri, Marzocchi, Delsignore and Brambilla2000) showed that ‘direct aggressiveness’ scores on the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory (BDHI) decreased significantly in ecstasy users abstinent for 12 months, compared to their three weeks of abstinence scores (df = 1;28; F = 9.74; p < .05). However, in another study, scores from the Direct Aggression and Irritability scales of the BDHI (t = 3.87, df = 38, P < .001; t = 3.51, df = 38, P < .001) and aggressive responses to a laboratory task (F = 20.74, df = 76, P < .001) were significantly higher in ecstasy users who had been abstinent for three weeks compared to controls (Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ampollini, Giusti, Delsignore, Raggi, Laviola, Macchia and Brambilla2001). One caveat to consider regarding this study is that ecstasy users were recruited from a drug addiction service and some reported personality disorders and dysphoria. Thus, aggressive responding may be linked to other psychological problems that may have predated ecstasy use, and given that the control group was not matched on these factors, it is difficult to draw concrete conclusions.

Further investigations into the longevity of effects of MDMA were explored in data from 110 ex-MDMA users which were divided into two groups based on their reason for quitting: 1.mental health or 2. circumstantial reasons (Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Maidment and Curran2003). There was a relationship between years of MDMA use and anger and aggression. In the mental health group, years of use correlated with the following scale scores: the Multidimensional Anger Inventory’s (MAI) frequency of anger (r = 0.55, p < 0.005) and aggression (total score) (r = 0.68, p < 0.001), and the Aggression Questionnaire’s (A.Q) anger (r = 0.51, p < 0.01) and physical aggression (r = 0.53, p < 0.005). In the circumstantial group, physical aggression scores on the A.Q had a positive correlation with frequency (r = 0.60, p < 0.01) and amount of MDMA use (r = 0.64, p < 0.01) (Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Maidment and Curran2003). It is important to note that the frequency (U = 124.00, p < 0.005) and duration (U = 89.00, p < 0.01) of LSD use were higher among participants in the mental health group compared to the circumstantial group. Furthermore, data collected from 260 ecstasy users in the United States, found individuals with high levels of lifetime ecstasy use had higher levels of violent and aggressive behaviour (Reid et al., Reference Reid, Elifson and Sterk2007). Based on this finding, the authors concluded that there is a relationship between high levels of aggressive and violent behaviour and high prevalence of lifetime ecstasy use. Additionally, high levels of lifetime ecstasy use was found to impact individuals with high self-control more than those with low self-control (i.e. more aggression was observed in individuals with high self-control) (Reid et al., Reference Reid, Elifson and Sterk2007).

In a correlational study (Scott et al., Reference Scott, Hides, Allen and Lubman2013) with a sample of 56 participants (35 ecstasy users and 21 abstained ecstasy users), quality and hours of sleep were associated with lower mood and increased psychological distress. Ecstasy use was not found to be related to self-reported mood symptoms or clinician-rated subacute symptoms of depression, anxiety, or aggression in the days following use.

Finally, Bond et al. (Reference Bond, Verheyden, Wingrove and Curran2004) employed a tryptophan depletion/enhancement strategy to investigate the effects of 5-HT on ‘angry responding’ which was based on the participants’ reaction towards a story-based task. Trait anger and aggression were measured by administering the A.Q (Buss and Perry, Reference Buss and Perry1992) and the MAI (Siegel, Reference Siegel1986) in current MDMA users who abstained for three weeks, ex-users abstinent for longer than one year and non-MDMA substance users. All participants processed angry sentences faster than non-angry ones, and this relationship was strongest among current MDMA users (r = 0.40, P = 0.028). Furthermore, ex-MDMA users had higher scores than controls on aggression, as seen from their total scores on the A.Q (F2,91 = 3.72, P < 0.03) (Bond et al., Reference Bond, Verheyden, Wingrove and Curran2004).

Gender differences

Half of the MDMA studies included only men (Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ferri, Zambelli, Timpano, Neri, Marzocchi, Delsignore and Brambilla2000; Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ampollini, Giusti, Delsignore, Raggi, Laviola, Macchia and Brambilla2001; Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Maidment and Curran2003; Bond et al., Reference Bond, Verheyden, Wingrove and Curran2004; Hendrickson and Gerstein, Reference Hendrickson and Gerstein2005; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Cohen, Lemanski, Piccini, Bond and Curran2007; Romero-Martínez et al., Reference Romero-Martínez, Lila and Moya-Albiol2019) and the few studies that included both genders were underpowered to complete gender-based analyses (Curran et al., Reference Curran, Rees, Hoare, Hoshi and Bond2004; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Bisla and Curran2004).

Four studies analysed the data between male and female MDMA users (Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Hadfield, Calin and Curran2002; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Pratt, Mehta, Bond and Curran2006; Reid et al., Reference Reid, Elifson and Sterk2007; Vaughn et al., Reference Vaughn, Salas-Wright, DeLisi, Perron and Cordova2015). In one study, self-rated trait aggression scores on day 0 and day 4 post-MDMA use did not differ between males and females (Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Hadfield, Calin and Curran2002). Additionally, there were some indications of gender differences in residual responses to MDMA, where the amount of MDMA consumed correlated positively with increased aggression scores in males at mid-week (r = 0.66, P < 0.005), but not in females. Based on the analysis of MDMA use patterns (i.e. dose, frequency of use and years of use), men had taken more MDMA tablets over their lifetime than women (t 22= –2.76, P < 0.05)(Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Hadfield, Calin and Curran2002). These results are similar to those reported by Vaughn et al. (Reference Vaughn, Salas-Wright, DeLisi, Perron and Cordova2015) who found that male, but not female, MDMA users were significantly more prone to commit acts of violence (AOR = 1.73, 95% CI = 1.51–2.00; AOR = 0.77, 95% CI = 0.63–0.94). Additionally, compared to female non-users, female MDMA users were significantly less likely to commit acts of violence and compared to male non-users, had a higher prevalence of engaging in criminal and violent acts (except injuring someone in a fight), even after controlling for confounding variables (Vaughn et al., Reference Vaughn, Salas-Wright, DeLisi, Perron and Cordova2015). Additionally, Reid et al. (Reference Reid, Elifson and Sterk2007) found that males exhibited less aggressive behaviour compared to females when looking at the impact of lifetime ecstasy use on aggressive behaviour among users with low self-control. In contrast, Hoshi et al. (Reference Hoshi, Pratt, Mehta, Bond and Curran2006) found no differences in aggressive interpretative bias and self-rated aggression in male and female MDMA users.

Instruments of measurement

Among MDMA studies, half of the studies (N = 7/14) administered the Aggression Rating Scale (ARS) (Bond and Lader, Reference Bond and Lader1986) and/or the A.Q (Buss and Perry, Reference Buss and Perry1992) to investigate participant aggression/anger (Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Hadfield, Calin and Curran2002; Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Maidment and Curran2003; Bond et al., Reference Bond, Verheyden, Wingrove and Curran2004; Curran et al., Reference Curran, Rees, Hoare, Hoshi and Bond2004; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Bisla and Curran2004; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Pratt, Mehta, Bond and Curran2006; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Cohen, Lemanski, Piccini, Bond and Curran2007). Two studies (Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ferri, Zambelli, Timpano, Neri, Marzocchi, Delsignore and Brambilla2000; Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ampollini, Giusti, Delsignore, Raggi, Laviola, Macchia and Brambilla2001) utilised the BDHI (Buss and Durkee, Reference Buss and Durkee1957) to quantify and characterise aggressiveness (direct, indirect, verbal; irritability; negativism; resentment; suspiciousness; guilt). Vaughan and team (Vaughn et al., Reference Vaughn, Salas-Wright, DeLisi, Perron and Cordova2015) administered the antisocial personality disorder module from the Alcohol Use Disorder and Associated Disabilities Interview Schedule (Grant et al., Reference Grant, Dawson, Stinson, Chou, Kay and Pickering2003) to gather data on violent behaviour in addition to using dichotomous questions to investigate involvement in criminal behaviour. One study used the MAI (Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Maidment and Curran2003), and Reid et al. (Reference Reid, Elifson and Sterk2007) created an additive scale that investigated aggressive behaviours. Lastly, Scott et al. (Reference Scott, Hides, Allen and Lubman2013) used Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) (Ventura et al., Reference Ventura, Green, Shaner and Liberman1993) items to measure aggression.

Discussion

The current narrative review investigated the association between psychedelic use and violent/aggressive behaviour among lifetime psychedelic users. Overall, it is difficult to draw any firm conclusions from the published data because of several shortcomings including lack of standardised measures, non-specific drug categorisation and failure to control for potential confounders or to provide details on gender differences, among other issues. These are outlined below.

Gender differences across studies

As highlighted in the results, it is difficult to draw any firm conclusions regarding gender differences in violent/aggressive behaviour amongst psychedelic users. Several reviewed studies included only males or were underpowered to detect gender differences. A small portion of the MDMA studies did investigate gender differences, but the findings were variable. This variation may be due to the diversity in measurements utilised to assess aggression across the various studies making comparisons difficult. For example, a meta-analysis found that in comparison to naturalistic studies, experimental studies were less likely to find increased aggression in men and gender differences were greater for certain forms of aggression (e.g. physical vs. verbal aggression) (Knight et al., Reference Knight, Guthrie, Page and Fabes2002). Men tend to express physical, overt and direct aggression, while relational and indirect aggression is more often found in women (Im et al., Reference Im, Jin, Jeong, Yeom, Jekal, Lee, Cho, Lee, Lee, Kim, Bae, Heo, Moon and Lee2018). The aggression scales used in seven of the fourteen MDMA studies investigated trait aggression which is generally more common in men. Administering aggression scales that assess various types of aggression would provide a more comprehensive understanding of gender differences and help to establish a greater degree of accuracy.

Polysubstance use

An important confounding factor across many of the reviewed studies is the use of other substances. In 13 of the 14 MDMA studies (Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ferri, Zambelli, Timpano, Neri, Marzocchi, Delsignore and Brambilla2000; Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ampollini, Giusti, Delsignore, Raggi, Laviola, Macchia and Brambilla2001; Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Hadfield, Calin and Curran2002; Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Maidment and Curran2003; Bond et al., Reference Bond, Verheyden, Wingrove and Curran2004; Curran et al., Reference Curran, Rees, Hoare, Hoshi and Bond2004; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Bisla and Curran2004; Hendrickson and Gerstein, Reference Hendrickson and Gerstein2005; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Pratt, Mehta, Bond and Curran2006; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Cohen, Lemanski, Piccini, Bond and Curran2007; Reid et al., Reference Reid, Elifson and Sterk2007; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Hides, Allen and Lubman2013; Romero-Martínez et al., Reference Romero-Martínez, Lila and Moya-Albiol2019) and two of the three hallucinogenic/classic psychedelic studies (Feingold et al., Reference Feingold, Kerr and Capaldi2008; Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Hendricks, Smith, Kosson, Thiessen, Lucas and Swogger2016), most, if not all, participants were episodic or frequent users of alcohol and other substances of abuse. Some studies tried to mitigate this by asking participants to abstain. For example, Bond et al. (Reference Bond, Verheyden, Wingrove and Curran2004) requested abstaining from cannabis; however, abstinence from habitual cannabis can be associated with withdrawal symptoms, which might exacerbate aggressive tendencies. Similarly, participants in another study met the criteria for other substance use disorders, including alcohol use disorder, which is known to influence aggression (Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Hendricks, Smith, Kosson, Thiessen, Lucas and Swogger2016). In some of the MDMA studies, the researchers mitigated the effects of polysubstance use by excluding participants who had dependencies on other substances (Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ferri, Zambelli, Timpano, Neri, Marzocchi, Delsignore and Brambilla2000; Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ampollini, Giusti, Delsignore, Raggi, Laviola, Macchia and Brambilla2001), or by controlling for confounding variables in their analyses as seen in two MDMA studies (Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Cohen, Lemanski, Piccini, Bond and Curran2007; Vaughn et al., Reference Vaughn, Salas-Wright, DeLisi, Perron and Cordova2015) and the classic psychedelic study (Thiessen et al., Reference Thiessen, Walsh, Bird and Lafrance2018). However, these strategies are not adequate to fully delineate the effects of psychedelics on aggression/violence and criminality from the impact of other drugs. Polysubstance users vary in the number and category of substances they use and the frequency, dose and duration of use. The level of impact of each substance on violent and/or aggressive behaviour varies across categories of substances (Boles and Miotto, Reference Boles and Miotto2003). For example, more studies suggest that cocaine use increases risk of aggression compared to the mixed findings associated with cannabis use (Boles and Miotto, Reference Boles and Miotto2003; Zhong et al., Reference Zhong, Yu and Fazel2020). Variability in usage behaviour, in addition to the variability in the magnitude of impact from each substance on aggressive/violent behaviour, makes it challenging to draw any conclusions from a polysubstance sample. Moreover, some polysubstance users may have started to use various substances to intentionally control the cognitive, emotional or behavioural side effects experienced from a single substance (Kataja et al., Reference Kataja, Tigerstedt and Hakkarainen2018; Foundation, Reference Foundation2021). For instance, an individual may be experiencing aggressive behaviour due to their MDMA use and may start to use cannabis in an attempt to ameliorate this effect. It is also important to note that a study of a university student sample found that polysubstance users reported higher physical and verbal aggression than mono-substance users (Steele and Peralta, Reference Steele and Peralta2020). The presence of multiple substances and the interaction between these substances may be the reason for this finding. Generally, interaction effects and developmental, psychosocial and personality characteristics associated with substance use and aggression make such disaggregation challenging. Furthermore, some psychedelics may directly affect the use and dependence of other drugs. For example, ayahuasca has been found to be effective in treating substance dependence (Frecska et al., Reference Frecska, Bokor and Winkelman2016).

Mental health

Further complexities in interpreting the reviewed literature lie in associations between previous aggressive behaviour and emotion regulation (Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ampollini, Giusti, Delsignore, Raggi, Laviola, Macchia and Brambilla2001; Thiessen et al., Reference Thiessen, Walsh, Bird and Lafrance2018). For example, the decreased odds of reporting IPV among male psychedelic users compared to males with no psychedelic history may be attributed –in part - to emotion regulation (Thiessen et al., Reference Thiessen, Walsh, Bird and Lafrance2018). When controlling for emotion regulation, the negative association between psychedelic use and IPV was reduced (b = −0.09, SE = 0.04, p > 0.05). These results suggest that emotion regulation influences the relationship between lower perpetration of IPV and psychedelic use. In contrast, another study reported that hallucinogen use (both classical and non-classical psychedelics) had the largest effect on IPV compared to other hard drugs, even after controlling for antisocial personality (Feingold et al., Reference Feingold, Kerr and Capaldi2008).

Looking at MDMA, pre-existing personality traits that influence an individual’s aggressive behaviour may be a factor in mediating aggression in the context of MDMA use (Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ampollini, Giusti, Delsignore, Raggi, Laviola, Macchia and Brambilla2001). Research also suggests that the likelihood of substance use may be related to pre-existing anger levels: those with high anger levels are more likely to use drugs (Serafini et al., Reference Serafini, Toohey, Kiluk and Carroll2016). This theory is supported by Bond et al. (Reference Bond, Verheyden, Wingrove and Curran2004) who found no difference in processing time of angry versus non-angry sentences between current MDMA users abstinent for three weeks, ex-users abstinent for longer than one year and non-MDMA substance users. All participants processed angry sentences faster than non-angry ones. These results suggest that individuals with high pre-existing anger levels may be more prone to use MDMA thus, reports of increased anger following MDMA use may not directly be related to the use of MDMA (Bond et al., Reference Bond, Verheyden, Wingrove and Curran2004). Furthermore, Reid et al. (Reference Reid, Elifson and Sterk2007) found that self-control may also mitigate the influence that ecstasy use has on aggression. Those with high self-control were more aggressive at high levels of lifetime ecstasy use. In contrast, those with low self-control were found to be less affected by high levels of lifetime ecstasy use, with little change in aggressive behaviour (Reid et al., Reference Reid, Elifson and Sterk2007).

Challenges with assessment measures

As outlined in the results, there is heterogeneity among studies regarding the assessment measures used to evaluate aggression. This makes it challenging to compare observations across studies. In addition, a majority (N = 14/17) of the reviewed studies relied on self-report when collecting substance use history and did not employ confirmatory, objective lab tests such as urine or hair analysis (Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Hadfield, Calin and Curran2002; Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Maidment and Curran2003; Bond et al., Reference Bond, Verheyden, Wingrove and Curran2004; Curran et al., Reference Curran, Rees, Hoare, Hoshi and Bond2004; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Bisla and Curran2004; Hendrickson and Gerstein, Reference Hendrickson and Gerstein2005; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Pratt, Mehta, Bond and Curran2006; Reid et al., Reference Reid, Elifson and Sterk2007; Feingold et al., Reference Feingold, Kerr and Capaldi2008; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Hides, Allen and Lubman2013; Vaughn et al., Reference Vaughn, Salas-Wright, DeLisi, Perron and Cordova2015; Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Hendricks, Smith, Kosson, Thiessen, Lucas and Swogger2016; Thiessen et al., Reference Thiessen, Walsh, Bird and Lafrance2018; Romero-Martínez et al., Reference Romero-Martínez, Lila and Moya-Albiol2019). Data collected through self-report may be biased due to social desirability or recall bias which can jeopardise data accuracy and may explain the mixed findings observed (Althubaiti, Reference Althubaiti2016).

Cohesion across studies on which instruments are used would enhance comparability between results. Moreover, each instrument has certain limitations and strengths which need to be considered before being employed in the study. For instance, the BDHI can measure the level of aggression influenced by the environment, life events and pharmacological use (Buss and Durkee, Reference Buss and Durkee1957), but misses out on assessing areas of aggression such as physical, verbal, anger and hostility, which are directly measured by the Aggression Questionnaire (Buss and Perry, Reference Buss and Perry1992).

Variability in duration, frequency and dose

There was substantial variability between studies regarding the duration and frequency of use for the drugs that were assessed. For example, in one study MDMA users were defined as having taken MDMA on at least 20 occasions during the previous year (Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Hadfield, Calin and Curran2002) while in another study, MDMA users had to have taken MDMA at least once a month with a minimum of 25 uses (Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Cohen, Lemanski, Piccini, Bond and Curran2007).

Additionally, a prevalent issue in clinical practice regarding substance use treatment and clinical effects following drug use is drug impurity (Shesser et al., Reference Shesser, Jotte and Olshaker1991). It is difficult to determine the actual dosage, level of purity and chemical make up of street drugs which is only further complicated by the fact that most participant samples consist of individuals who used multiple substances. There is heterogeneity in the make up of drugs and the actual dosage of pure substance used by each individual. In one study, participants reported using 1–2 pills, only on weekend nights (Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ferri, Zambelli, Timpano, Neri, Marzocchi, Delsignore and Brambilla2000). In contrast, the mean number of tablets per session of use was found to be 2.71 among MDMA participants of another study (Curran et al., Reference Curran, Rees, Hoare, Hoshi and Bond2004). This phenomenon may explain the mixed findings across MDMA studies given that other amphetamine analogues and other compounds, such as caffeine, have been found in ecstasy tablets (Wolff et al., Reference Wolff, Hay, Sherlock and Conner1995).

Lastly, further research should explore the extent to which the duration and frequency of psychedelic use impacts aggressive and violent behaviour in MDMA users. The ability to recognise fearful faces on the fourth day following use has been demonstrated to have a negative relationship between duration (r = −0.54, P = 0.048) and amount (r = −0.58, P = 0.031) of ecstasy use (Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Bisla and Curran2004). This means that more ecstasy use at baseline and an extended history of use was associated with worse fear recognition accuracy score on day four. Reid and colleagues (Reid et al., Reference Reid, Elifson and Sterk2007) also found a linear relationship between the prevalence of committing violent acts and the number of ecstasy pills ever used.

Limitations and future directions

There is abundant room for further progress in exploring associations between psychedelic use and aggressive/violent behaviour. This research is especially important given the growing interest from the research community and the general public regarding psychedelics as potential treatments for mental health and addictions. There are several limitations regarding the reviewed literature. First, in several of the reviewed studies, ‘hallucinogen’ was used as a broad category to include several serotonergic psychedelics. Early research investigating the use of classic psychedelics for therapeutic benefits among incarcerated individuals demonstrated positive personality changes (Arendsen-Hein, Reference Arendsen-Hein1963; Leary, Reference Leary1969) and enhanced empathy, communication and insight (Tenenbaum, Reference Tenenbaum1961). However, rigorous scientific methodology was not employed in these studies. Of the three non-MDMA studies reviewed (Feingold et al., Reference Feingold, Kerr and Capaldi2008; Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Hendricks, Smith, Kosson, Thiessen, Lucas and Swogger2016; Thiessen et al., Reference Thiessen, Walsh, Bird and Lafrance2018), only one distinguished classic serotonergic psychedelics (i.e. LSD, psilocybin) from non-classic psychedelics (i.e. PCP, MDMA) (Thiessen et al., Reference Thiessen, Walsh, Bird and Lafrance2018). As discussed in the introduction, the mechanism of action of classic psychedelics and empathogens differ (Nichols, Reference Nichols1986; Garcia-Romeu et al., Reference Garcia-Romeu, Kersgaard and Addy2016). Grouping these drugs into one category limits our understanding of the impact that each one may have on aggressive/violent behaviour and precludes specific conclusions from being drawn. Studies that compare the effects of using classic versus non-classic psychedelics are needed to understand the extent to which agents from each class impacts aggressive behaviours. Future studies should differentiate individual substances and their relation to aggression/violence.

Second, several studies included participants who were polysubstance users (Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ferri, Zambelli, Timpano, Neri, Marzocchi, Delsignore and Brambilla2000; Gerra et al., Reference Gerra, Zaimovic, Ampollini, Giusti, Delsignore, Raggi, Laviola, Macchia and Brambilla2001; Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Hadfield, Calin and Curran2002; Verheyden et al., Reference Verheyden, Maidment and Curran2003; Bond et al., Reference Bond, Verheyden, Wingrove and Curran2004; Curran et al., Reference Curran, Rees, Hoare, Hoshi and Bond2004; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Bisla and Curran2004; Hendrickson and Gerstein, Reference Hendrickson and Gerstein2005; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Pratt, Mehta, Bond and Curran2006; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Cohen, Lemanski, Piccini, Bond and Curran2007; Reid et al., Reference Reid, Elifson and Sterk2007; Feingold et al., Reference Feingold, Kerr and Capaldi2008; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Hides, Allen and Lubman2013; Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Hendricks, Smith, Kosson, Thiessen, Lucas and Swogger2016; Romero-Martínez et al., Reference Romero-Martínez, Lila and Moya-Albiol2019). Future research projects should focus on controlling for confounding variables such as psychiatric disorders, polysubstance use, personality characteristics and pre-existing behaviours to explore the association between psychedelic use and aggressive/violent behaviour. Third, most of the studies relied on self-report regarding the use of recreational drugs. Without proper laboratory analysis, we cannot be certain about the purity or dose of the substances being consumed. Fourth, the type and extent of aggression/IPV varied substantially across studies, with some research focusing on violent criminal activity and others focusing on objective measures (e.g. interpretative bias tasks). While it could be argued that aggressive interpretative bias may underlie violence, it is difficult to conclude the role psychedelics play in this relationship and it has yet to be rigorously investigated. Fifth, there was variation in measurements of aggression, with few studies employing objective and subjective measures (Bond et al., Reference Bond, Verheyden, Wingrove and Curran2004; Curran et al., Reference Curran, Rees, Hoare, Hoshi and Bond2004; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Bisla and Curran2004; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Pratt, Mehta, Bond and Curran2006; Hoshi et al., Reference Hoshi, Cohen, Lemanski, Piccini, Bond and Curran2007). To develop a complete picture on the extent of influence psychedelic use has on aggression, additional studies that include multidimensional and validated measures of aggression are needed. Lastly, more longitudinal research is needed to understand the relationship between trait aggression and prolonged cessation of use and whether increases in aggression following use is transient or long-lasting.

A number of studies from our initial search were excluded as they had not employed standardised questionnaires or because the outcome variables did not directly relate to violence or aggression (Wu et al., Reference Wu, Schlenger and Galvin2006; Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Bellis, Calafat, Juan, Schnitzer and Anderson2008; Håkansson and Berglund, Reference Håkansson and Berglund2012; Raznahan et al., Reference Raznahan, Hassanzadeh, Houshmand, Kashani, Tabrizi and Akhondzadeh2013; Carbonaro et al., Reference Carbonaro, Bradstreet, Barrett, MacLean, Jesse, Johnson and Griffiths2016; Hendricks et al., Reference Hendricks, Crawford, Cropsey, Copes, Sweat, Walsh and Pavela2018; Jones and Nock, Reference Jones and Nock2022). One of these studies found that psychedelic use (Ayahuasca, DMT, LSD, mescaline, peyote, psilocybin) was associated with lower odds of property crime, theft, assault and violent crime compared to other illicit substance use (Hendricks et al., Reference Hendricks, Crawford, Cropsey, Copes, Sweat, Walsh and Pavela2018). Building on this, another study reported significantly lowered odds of seven crime arrest outcomes (larceny, burglary, robbery, simple assault/battery, serious violence, driving under the influence and miscellaneous crimes) among participants with a lifetime history of psilocybin use (Jones and Nock, Reference Jones and Nock2022). Although these studies are informative, they were excluded because the data was collected from The National Survey on Drug Use and Health and standardised anger/aggression measures were not utilised. Most psychedelic drugs are presumed to demonstrate relatively good physical and psychological safety profiles, in addition to therapeutic potential for mental health and substance use disorders, leading to a renewed interest in their potential therapeutic use (Perkins et al., Reference Perkins, Sarris, Rossell, Bonomo, Forbes, Davey, Hoyer, Loo, Murray, Hood, Schubert, Galvão-Coelho, O’Donnell, Carter, Liknaitzky, Williams, Siskind, Penington, Berk and Castle2021).

It is important to consider the difference between ‘hostility’ and ‘aggression’. Violence is often seen as an extreme form of aggression (Allen and Anderson, Reference Allen and Anderson2017). In reviewing the literature, there is strong evidence to suggest an increased risk of aggressive behaviour and violent crimes is associated with the use of many other drugs of abuse (Pihl and Peterson, Reference Pihl and Peterson1995; Boles and Miotto, Reference Boles and Miotto2003). We were interested in the overt and observable anger and not hostility which can be defined as an attitude (Eckhardt et al., Reference Eckhardt, Norlander and Deffenbacher2004). Finally, our study was limited to research articles published between 2000 and 2022. There are numerous studies conducted before the early 2000s that investigated relationships between psychedelics and aggression. However, we were interested in more recent trials and thus excluded studies done before the year 2000.

Conclusions

Findings from studies on associations between MDMA use and aggressive/violent behaviour are conflicting. The data on classic psychedelic use and aggression is limited, and no definitive conclusions can be drawn. Thus far, classic psychedelic use has not been shown to be associated with increased violent offences such as IPV. Variability in sample characteristics, instruments of measurement and confounding factors may explain some of the inconsistencies in the current literature. Researchers should ensure they have robust and diverse sample sizes and consider implementing more rigorous methods of analysis into future studies.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported in part by an Academic Scholars Award to MIH from the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto.

Author contribution

NS, MIH and DJC conceived the idea for the narrative review. NS and NL drafted the manuscript, conducted the literature review and analysed the data. NS extracted the data and created the summary table and summary of the results table. DJC resolved any conflicts at the title/abstract screening and full-text review stage. DJC and MIH helped with drafting the manuscript. All authors reviewed and approved the final manuscript.

References

Allen, JJ and Anderson, CA (2017) Aggression and violence: definitions and distinctions. In The Wiley Handbook of Violence and Aggression pp. 114.Google Scholar
Althubaiti, A (2016) Information bias in health research: definition, pitfalls, and adjustment methods. Journal of Multidisciplinary Healthcare 9, 211217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arendsen-Hein, G (1963) LSD in the treatment of criminal psychopaths. In Hallucinogenic Drugs and Their Psychotherapeutic Use . London: HK Lewis & Co.: Ltd, pp. 101106.Google Scholar
Boles, SM and Miotto, K (2003) Substance abuse and violence. Aggression and Violent Behavior 8(2), 155174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bond, A and Lader, M (1986) A method to elicit aggressive feelings and behaviour via provocation. Biological Psychology 22(1), 6979.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bond, AJ, Verheyden, SL, Wingrove, J and Curran, HV (2004) Angry cognitive bias, trait aggression and impulsivity in substance users. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 171(3), 331339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, AH and Durkee, A (1957) An inventory for assessing different kinds of hostility. Journal of Consulting Psychology 21(4), 343349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, AH and Perry, M (1992) The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 63(3), 452459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carbonaro, TM, Bradstreet, MP, Barrett, FS, MacLean, KA, Jesse, R, Johnson, MW and Griffiths, RR (2016) Survey study of challenging experiences after ingesting psilocybin mushrooms: acute and enduring positive and negative consequences. Journal of Psychopharmacology 30(12), 12681278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carhart-Harris, RL and Goodwin, GM (2017) The therapeutic potential of psychedelic drugs: past, present, and future. Neuropsychopharmacology 42(11), 21052113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coid, JW, Ullrich, S, Bebbington, P, Fazel, S and Keers, R (2016) Paranoid ideation and violence: meta-analysis of individual subject data of 7 population surveys. Schizophrenia Bulletin 42(4), 907915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curran, HV, Rees, H, Hoare, T, Hoshi, R and Bond, A (2004) Empathy and aggression: two faces of ecstasy? A study of interpretative cognitive bias and mood change in ecstasy users. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 173(3-4), 425433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
da Cunha-Bang, S and Knudsen, GM (2021) The modulatory role of serotonin on human impulsive aggression. Biological Psychiatry 90(7), 447457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DEA (2018) Lists of: scheduling actions, controlled substances, regulated chemicals . US Dep. Justice-Drug Enforcement AdministrationDiversion Control Division-Drug & Chemical Evaluation Section. Retrieved Oct, 2023, from https://www.deadiversion.usdoj.gov/schedules/orangebook/orangebook.pdf.Google Scholar
Dinis-Oliveira, RJ, Pereira, CL and da Silva, DD (2019) Pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic aspects of peyote and mescaline: clinical and forensic repercussions. Current Molecular Pharmacology 12(3), 184194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunlap, LE, Andrews, AM and Olson, DE (2018) Dark classics in chemical neuroscience: 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine. ACS Chemical Neuroscience 9(10), 24082427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckhardt, C, Norlander, B and Deffenbacher, J (2004) The assessment of anger and hostility: a critical review. Aggression and Violent Behavior 9(1), 1743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feingold, A, Kerr, DCR and Capaldi, DM (2008) Associations of substance use problems with intimate partner violence for at-risk men in long-term relationships. Journal of Family Psychology 22(3), 429438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foran, HM and O’Leary, KD (2008) Alcohol and intimate partner violence: a meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review 28(7), 12221234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foundation, AD (2021) Polydrug use. Available at https://adf.org.au/reducing-risk/polydrug-use/.Google Scholar
Frecska, E, Bokor, P and Winkelman, M (2016) The therapeutic potentials of ayahuasca: possible effects against various diseases of civilization. Frontiers in Pharmacology 7, 35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia-Romeu, A, Kersgaard, B and Addy, PH (2016) Clinical applications of hallucinogens: a review. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 24(4), 229268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerra, G, Zaimovic, A, Ampollini, R, Giusti, F, Delsignore, R, Raggi, MA, Laviola, G, Macchia, T and Brambilla, F (2001) Experimentally induced aggressive behavior in subjects with 3,4-methylenedioxy-methamphetamine (“Ecstasy”) use history: psychobiological correlates. Journal of Substance Abuse 13(4), 471491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerra, G, Zaimovic, A, Ferri, M, Zambelli, U, Timpano, M, Neri, E, Marzocchi, GF, Delsignore, R and Brambilla, F (2000) Long-lasting effects of (±)3,4-methylene-dioxymethamphetamine (Ecstasy) on serotonin system function in humans. Biological Psychiatry 47(2), 127136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grant, BF, Dawson, DA, Stinson, FS, Chou, PS, Kay, W and Pickering, R (2003) The Alcohol Use Disorder and Associated Disabilities Interview Schedule-IV (AUDADIS-IV): reliability of alcohol consumption, tobacco use, family history of depression and psychiatric diagnostic modules in a general population sample. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 71(1), 716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grinspoon, L and Bakalar, JB (1986) Can drugs be used to enhance the psychotherapeutic process? American Journal of Psychotherapy 40(3), 393404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Håkansson, A and Berglund, M (2012) Risk factors for criminal recidivism - a prospective follow-up study in prisoners with substance abuse. BMC Psychiatry 12(1), 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamill, J, Hallak, J, Dursun, SM and Baker, G (2019) Ayahuasca: psychological and physiologic effects, pharmacology and potential uses in addiction and mental illness. Current Neuropharmacology 17(2), 108128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hendricks, PS, Crawford, MS, Cropsey, KL, Copes, H, Sweat, NW, Walsh, Z and Pavela, G (2018) The relationships of classic psychedelic use with criminal behavior in the United States adult population. Journal of Psychopharmacology 32(1), 3748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hendrickson, JC and Gerstein, DR (2005) Criminal involvement among young male ecstasy users. Substance Use & Misuse 40(9-10), 15571575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoshi, R, Bisla, J and Curran, HV (2004) The acute and sub-acute effects of ‘ecstasy’ (MDMA) on processing of facial expressions: preliminary findings. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 76(3), 297304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoshi, R, Cohen, L, Lemanski, L, Piccini, P, Bond, A and Curran, HV (2007) Ecstasy (MDMA) does not have long-term effects on aggressive interpretative bias: a study comparing current and ex-ecstasy users with polydrug and drug-naive controls. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 15(4), 351358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoshi, R, Pratt, H, Mehta, S, Bond, AJ and Curran, HV (2006) An investigation into the sub-acute effects of ecstasy on aggressive interpretative bias and aggressive mood - are there gender differences? Journal of Psychopharmacology 20(2), 291301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, K, Bellis, MA, Calafat, A, Juan, M, Schnitzer, S and Anderson, Z (2008) Predictors of violence in young tourists: a comparative study of British, German and Spanish holidaymakers. European Journal of Public Health 18(6), 569574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Im, S, Jin, G, Jeong, J, Yeom, J, Jekal, J, Lee, SI, Cho, JA, Lee, S, Lee, Y, Kim, DH, Bae, M, Heo, J, Moon, C and Lee, CH (2018) Gender differences in aggression-related responses on EEG and ECG. Experimental Neurobiology 27(6), 526538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, GM and Nock, MK (2022) Psilocybin use is associated with lowered odds of crime arrests in US adults: a replication and extension. Journal of Psychopharmacology 36(1), 6673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kataja, K, Tigerstedt, C and Hakkarainen, P (2018) More social research into polydrug use. Nordic Studies on Alcohol and Drugs 35(6), 399403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knight, G, Guthrie, I, Page, M and Fabes, R (2002) Emotional arousal and gender differences in aggression: a meta-analysis. Aggressive Behavior 28(5), 366393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leary, T (1969) The effects of consciousness-expanding drugs on prisoner rehabilitation. Psychedelic Review 10, 2945.Google Scholar
Liechti, ME, Baumann, C, Gamma, A and Vollenweider, FX (2000) Acute psychological effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, “Ecstasy”) are attenuated by the serotonin uptake inhibitor citalopram. Neuropsychopharmacology 22(5), 513521.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKenna, DJ and Peroutka, SJ (1990) Neurochemistry and neurotoxicity of 3, 4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA,“ecstasy”). Journal of Neurochemistry 54(1), 1422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mojtabai, R (2006) Psychotic-like experiences and interpersonal violence in the general population. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 41(3), 183190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nichols, DE (1986) Differences between the mechanism of action of MDMA, MBDB, and the classic hallucinogens. Identification of a new therapeutic class: entactogens. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 18(4), 305313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nichols, DE (2016) Psychedelics. Pharmacological Reviews 68(2), 264355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nutt, D & Castle, D (2022) Psychedelics as Psychiatric Medicines Oxford . Oxford University Press Oxford.Google Scholar
Parrott, AC (2001) Human psychopharmacology of Ecstasy (MDMA): a review of 15 years of empirical research. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental 16(8), 557577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Passie, T (2018) The early use of MDMA (‘Ecstasy’) in psychotherapy (1977-1985). Drug Science, Policy and Law 4, 2050324518767442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perkins, D, Sarris, J, Rossell, S, Bonomo, Y, Forbes, D, Davey, C, Hoyer, D, Loo, C, Murray, G, Hood, S, Schubert, V, Galvão-Coelho, NL, O’Donnell, M, Carter, O, Liknaitzky, P, Williams, M, Siskind, D, Penington, D, Berk, M and Castle, D (2021) Medicinal psychedelics for mental health and addiction: advancing research of an emerging paradigm. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 55(12), 11271133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pihl, RO and Peterson, J (1995) Drugs and aggression: correlations, crime and human manipulative studies and some proposed mechanisms. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience: JPN 20(2), 141149.Google ScholarPubMed
Raznahan, M, Hassanzadeh, E, Houshmand, A, Kashani, L, Tabrizi, M and Akhondzadeh, S (2013) Change in frequency of acute and subacute effects of ecstasy in a group of novice users after 6 months of regular use. Psychiatria Danubina 25(2), 175178.Google Scholar
Reid, LW, Elifson, KW and Sterk, CE (2007) Hug drug or thug drug? Ecstasy use and aggressive behavior. Violence and Victims 22(1), 104119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reiss, AJ and Roth, JA (1993) Alcohol, other psychoactive drugs and violence. Understanding and Preventing Violence 1, 182220.Google Scholar
Romero-Martínez, Á., Lila, M and Moya-Albiol, L (2019) Long-term drug misuse increases the risk of cognitive dysfunctions in intimate partner violence perpetrators: key intervention targets for reducing dropout and reoffending. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 16(20), 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rucker, JJH, Iliff, J and Nutt, DJ (2018) Psychiatry & the psychedelic drugs. Past, present & future. Neuropharmacology 142, 200218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schlag, AK, Aday, J, Salam, I, Neill, JC and Nutt, DJ (2022) Adverse effects of psychedelics: from anecdotes and misinformation to systematic science. Journal of Psychopharmacology 36(3), 258272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, CJ, Wu, L and Lovenberg, W (1986) Methylenedioxymethamphetamine: a potentially neurotoxic amphetamine analogue. European Journal of Pharmacology 124(1-2), 175178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, RM, Hides, L, Allen, JS and Lubman, DI (2013) Subacute effects of ecstasy on mood: an exploration of associated risk factors. Journal of Psychopharmacology 27(1), 5361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Serafini, K, Toohey, MJ, Kiluk, BD and Carroll, KM (2016) Anger and its association with substance use treatment outcomes in a sample of adolescents. Journal of Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse 25(5), 391398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shesser, R, Jotte, R and Olshaker, J (1991) The contribution of impurities to the acute morbidity of illegal drug use. American Journal of Emergency Medicine 9(4), 336342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, JM (1986) The multidimensional anger inventory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51(1), 191200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steele, JL and Peralta, RL (2020) Are polydrug users more physically and verbally aggressive? An assessment of aggression among mono- versus polydrug users in a university sample. Journal of Interpersonal Violence 35(21-22), 44444467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tenenbaum, B (1961) Group therapy with LSD-25. (A preliminary report). Diseases of the Nervous System 22, 459462.Google ScholarPubMed
Thiessen, MS, Walsh, Z, Bird, BM and Lafrance, A (2018) Psychedelic use and intimate partner violence: the role of emotion regulation. Journal of Psychopharmacology 32(7), 749755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaughn, MG, Salas-Wright, CP, DeLisi, M, Perron, BE and Cordova, D (2015) Crime and violence among MDMA users in the United States. AIMS Public Health 2(1), 6473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ventura, J, Green, MF, Shaner, A and Liberman, RP (1993) Training and quality assurance with the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale: “the drift busters“. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 3, 221244.Google Scholar
Verheyden, SL, Hadfield, J, Calin, T and Curran, HV (2002) Sub-acute effects of MDMA (+/−3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine, “ecstasy“) on mood: evidence of gender differences. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 161(1), 2331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verheyden, SL, Maidment, R and Curran, HV (2003) Quitting ecstasy: an investigation of why people stop taking the drug and their subsequent mental health. Journal of Psychopharmacology 17(4), 371378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veritas Health Innovation (2022) Covidence Systematic Review Software . Melbourne, Australia: Veritas Health Innovation.Google Scholar
Vollenweider, FX (2022) Brain mechanisms of hallucinogens and entactogens. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 3, 265279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vollenweider, FX, Gamma, A, Liechti, M and Huber, T (1998) Psychological and cardiovascular effects and short-term sequelae of MDMA (“ecstasy”) in MDMA-naïve healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology 19(4), 241251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walsh, Z, Hendricks, PS, Smith, S, Kosson, DS, Thiessen, MS, Lucas, P and Swogger, MT (2016) Hallucinogen use and intimate partner violence: prospective evidence consistent with protective effects among men with histories of problematic substance use. Journal of Psychopharmacology 30(7), 601607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolff, K, Hay, AW, Sherlock, K and Conner, M (1995) Contents of “ecstasy”. The Lancet 346(8982), 11001101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, LT, Schlenger, WE and Galvin, DM (2006) Concurrent use of methamphetamine, MDMA, LSD, ketamine, GHB, and flunitrazepam among American youths. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 84(1), 102113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhong, S, Yu, R and Fazel, S (2020) Drug use disorders and violence: associations with individual drug categories. Epidemiologic Reviews 42(1), 103116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. PRISMA 2020 flow diagram for new systematic reviews which included searches of databases and registers only. From: Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al., The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021;372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71. For more information, visit: http://www.prisma-statement.org/.

Figure 1

Table 1. Summary of trials

Figure 2

Table 2. Summary of results