We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
We have seen that in normal cases children create their mental grammar from input that has been called “poor,” but that language fails to emerge when input is totally absent. Chapter 12 shows that when the input is of “extra-poor” quality, children nevertheless manage to arrive at mental grammars of apparently sufficient complexity. The relevant situation can lead to the emergence of whole new languages. The key claim is that children, when exposed to a so-called pidgin (a relatively primitive system of communication created by adults who do not share a common language), transform such an impoverished language into a full-fledged language, which we call a creole. How can children do this? Nativists have argued that children can do this because they have an innate “manual” that dictates the construction of a mental grammar, no matter how impoverished the input. This leads to the creolization argument in support of the Innateness Hypothesis. This chapter also surveys various kinds of artificially constructed languages that are typically created by adults who either want to design a universal language or to provide groups of people in fictional stories with their own language.
This last chapter deals with three remaining issues, each with a dedicated section: macro families, sign languages, and constructed languages. Several major proposals that combine well-established language families into larger macro families are outlined, including the famous Nostratic hypothesis, as well as some lesser-known hypotheses such as the Dene-Yeniseian and Ural-Altaic hypotheses. The second section discusses sign languages and argues that they are like oral languages in that they have grammatical patterns of their own. The final section gives a brief overview of artificially constructed languages and how they differ from natural human languages, to which the majority of this book is dedicated.
Recommend this
Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this to your organisation's collection.