We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Chapter seven addresses the question of why some Rwandans killed and others not. Drawing on original data, it provides new estimates of the number of individuals killed and the number of individuals who participated in the violence. It finds that an astonishing one in five Hutu men committed an act of violence during the genocide. However, this still means that four in five did not. Drawing on survey data of 294 Rwandans, the chapter then systematically compares perpetrators against non-perpetrators. Consistent with the consensus on 'ordinary men', it finds few statistically significance differences between them. However, using other evidence, the chapter shows that important dispositional differences nonetheless existed that the perpetrator sample, drawn exclusively from within the prison system, did not capture. It distinguishes between extremists, opportunists, conformists, and pacifists. Perpetrator heterogeneity existed. Differential selection into the violence was not solely dispositional, however. The chapter shows that situational and relational forces also mattered. It maps the household locations of 3,246 residents of a single Rwandan community and finds that where you lived in part determined whether you would be drawn into the violence. Spatial proximity increased the opportunity for ‘social influence’ - the induction of individuals, through coercion or persuasion, into the violence. The chapter also maps the social networks of 130 Rwandans. It finds that who you knew also mattered.
Recommend this
Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this to your organisation's collection.