Hostname: page-component-6bb9c88b65-spzww Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-07-19T13:57:49.209Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neuromodulation of the right inferior frontal gyrus in bipolar disorder to target response inhibition: a proof-of-concept study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 June 2025

Karling Luciani
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Institute of Mental Health, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Laura Schmid
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Institute of Mental Health, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada https://ror.org/01jvd8304 BC Mental Health & Substance Use Services Research Institute , PHSA, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Bazyl Carroll
Affiliation:
Non-Invasive Neurostimulation Therapies Laboratory, Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Alexandra Sebastian
Affiliation:
University Medical Center, https://ror.org/00q1fsf04 Johannes Gutenberg University , Mainz, Germany https://ror.org/00q5t0010 Leibniz Institute for Resilience Research , Mainz, Germany
Fidel Vila-Rodriguez
Affiliation:
Non-Invasive Neurostimulation Therapies Laboratory, Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada School of Biomedical Engineering, https://ror.org/03rmrcq20 University of British Columbia , Vancouver, BC, Canada Spanish Consortium of Biomedical Research in Epidemiology and Public Health (CIBERESP), Madrid, Spain Health Technology Assessment in Primary Care and Mental Health Research Group (PRISMA), Barcelona, Spain
Christian G. Schütz*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Institute of Mental Health, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada https://ror.org/01jvd8304 BC Mental Health & Substance Use Services Research Institute , PHSA, Vancouver, BC, Canada
*
Corresponding author: Christian G. Schütz; Email: christian.schutz@ubc.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

Bipolar disorder (BD) affects over 1% of the population and is characterized by deficits in response inhibition. Response inhibition, a crucial component of executive functions, involves the ability to suppress or withhold a planned or ongoing response that is no longer required or appropriate in a given context. Response inhibition may be dissociated into three subcomponents: interference inhibition, action withholding, and action cancellation. These subcomponents are assessed using the hybrid response inhibition (HRI) task. Previous research has shown that inhibitory control is strongly lateralized to the right hemisphere. Specifically, the right inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG) is a key node underpinning response inhibition and might be amenable to neuromodulation using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS). This proof-of-concept study aimed to investigate the effects of rTMS targeting the rIFG on response inhibition in individuals with BD and controls.

Methods

We investigated HRI performance scores in individuals with BD (n = 12) and sex-/age-matched controls (n = 12) immediately before and after intermittent theta-burst stimulation (iTBS) and continuous TBS to modulate cortical excitability of the rIFG.

Results

The response inhibition subcomponent “action withholding” was significantly improved in the HRI task following iTBS in the BD group. No other significant effects were observed in the results.

Conclusions

Our study is the first to show that iTBS to the rIFG neuromodulated a specific subcomponent of response inhibition in BD. Further research investigating the potential therapeutic effect of neuromodulation of the rIFG in BD is warranted.

Information

Type
Original Research
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Bipolar disorder (BD) is a mental disorder characterized by shifts in mood and energy, with a lifetime prevalence of more than 1% in the global population.Reference Smith, Whitham and Ghaemi1, Reference Moreira, Van Meter, Genzlinger and Youngstrom2 BD has a substantial impact on an individual’s everyday functioning and has the highest rate of suicide of all psychiatric conditions.Reference Miller and Black3 Despite this, current treatment strategies for BD face substantial limitations.Reference Baldessarini, Tondo and Vazquez4 While pharmaceutical treatments, often combined with psychological interventions, are the most commonly utilized approaches for managing BD, their efficacy can be limited, and these treatments can induce substantial and sometimes serious adverse effects.Reference Baldessarini, Tondo and Vazquez4, Reference Dean, Gliddon and Van Rheenen5 Impulsivity, a prominent trait in BD, is associated with increased suicidality.Reference Johnson, Carver and Tharp6 Existing pharmaceutical treatments for BD have not been effective in reducing impulsivity,Reference Ajmi, Najjar and Masmoudi7 and no other treatment approaches prioritize impulsivity as a primary focus.

A central process that affects changes in brain function associated with BD is cognitive control, the ability to manage thoughts and behaviors in accordance with one’s goals.Reference Miller and Cohen8 One component of cognitive control is being able to reverse decisions after they are made but before they are implemented, otherwise known as response inhibition.Reference Jacobson, Javitt and Lavidor9 Individuals with BD have been found to be more impulsive because of deficits in response inhibition co-occurring with strong impulses, which lead to poor decision-making and functioning.Reference Chan, Alter and Hazlett10 Response inhibition may be dissociated into three subcomponents: (i) interference inhibition, (ii) action withholding, and (iii) action cancellation.Reference Sebastian, Pohl and Kloppel11 All subcomponents share a common neural network in the prefrontal cortex but differ in the degree of regional involvement.Reference Sebastian, Pohl and Kloppel11 Thus, they all constitute different subprocesses of response inhibition that should be considered when investigating cognitive control in different populations. These subcomponents have not yet been investigated in a BD population; research has primarily focused on response inhibition as a whole.

The regions of the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex involved in inhibitory control are strongly lateralized to the right hemisphere.Reference Garavan, Ross and Stein12 In particular, the right inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG) plays a large role in stopping impulsive behaviors and is thus a potential target for augmentation of response inhibition and cognitive control.Reference Chan, Alter and Hazlett10 Reduced activation of the rIFG during response inhibition has been discovered in individuals with BD during both the euthymic (stable mood) and manic states compared to controls.Reference Hajek, Alda, Hajek and Ivanoff13 Past studies have found cortical hyperactivations in BD in other brain regions, such as the adjacent prefrontal cortex and superior temporal gyrus, which may, in part, represent compensatory activity for the cognitive changes caused by the rIFG hypoactivations.Reference Chan, Alter and Hazlett10, Reference Hajek, Alda, Hajek and Ivanoff13 Thus, treatments focused on stimulation of the rIFG may reduce the need for compensatory overactivity during response inhibition and improve behavioral outcomes.Reference Chan, Alter and Hazlett10

Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) is a noninvasive brain stimulation technique that induces electrical stimuli to the cortex using strong, rapidly changing magnetic fields to target specific brain regions.Reference Klomjai, Katz and Lackmy-Vallee14 Theta-burst stimulation (TBS) is a patterned form of rTMS shown to produce strong and lasting neurophysiological effects with a reduced administration time.Reference Huang, Edwards, Rounis, Bhatia and Rothwell15 The two different patterns of TBS commonly used are intermittent TBS (iTBS), where pulses are delivered intermittently to increase cortical excitability, and continuous TBS (cTBS), where pulses are delivered continuously to reduce cortical excitability.Reference Huang, Edwards, Rounis, Bhatia and Rothwell15 rTMS is currently being investigated for mental disorders involving impulse control deficits, such as post-traumatic stress disorder and generalized anxiety disorder.Reference Baeken, Brem and Arns16, Reference Kozel17 In addition, a study investigating the effects of TBS of the rIFG on inhibitory control in individuals with a substance use disorder showed a significant improvement following iTBS.Reference Newman-Norlund, Gibson, McConnell and Froeliger18 However, current investigations on the application of rTMS in BD remain limited,Reference Konstantinou, Hui and Ortiz19 and no study to date has looked at the stimulation of the rIFG in BD to assess response inhibition.

The current proof-of-concept study explored neuromodulation of the rIFG and its effect on response inhibition in individuals with BD and controls. The three subcomponents of response inhibition were investigated and compared using a validated computerized task before and after TBS. The primary aim of the study was to assess the effect of iTBS and cTBS on response inhibition performance scores for individuals with BD. The secondary aim of the study was to identify any score differences between individuals with BD and controls for the effect of iTBS and cTBS on response inhibition.

Methods

Participants

The study included 12 euthymic adults aged 19–45 years with BD (I or II) and 12 age- and sex-matched controls. Before study enrollment, a phone screener was performed to prescreen eligibility. Participants were eligible to participate if they were right-handed, fluent in English, and had no history of any substance use disorder, gambling disorder, or any other mental disorder. Individuals were excluded if they did not meet the criteria for both the TBS and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) safety screenings. BD participants required a confirmed diagnosis via the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders psychiatric disorders)Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier and Sheehan20 and had to be stable on psychotropic medications for at least 2 weeks before screening, while remaining in the euthymic state during this time. Several BD participants reported taking antipsychotic medications (quetiapine, n = 5; aripiprazole, n = 1), antidepressants (escitalopram, n = 3; bupropion, n = 2; trazodone, n = 1, for sleep), and mood stabilizers (lamotrigine, n = 3; divalproex, n = 2; lithium, n = 1). One participant also reported gidazepam, a benzodiazepine. No controls reported taking any regular medications. To be able to participate, participants were screened for substance use including alcohol at the start of each session by completing a urine toxicology test (One Step Multi-Line Screen Test Device, Nova Century Scientific, Burlington, ON) and an alcohol breathalyzer test (BACtrack S80, KHN Solutions Inc., San Francisco, CA). Nicotine and tobacco use were also assessed, and participants confirmed they had not used any nicotine or tobacco products for at least 12 hours before each session.

The study protocol was approved by the University of British Columbia Clinical Research Ethics Board and the Vancouver Coastal Health Research Institute. All participants provided written informed consent before study enrollment and ongoing verbal consent at the start of each study session.

Procedures

All participants completed one baseline session to obtain demographic and clinical characteristics, one MRI session, and two TBS sessions. During the baseline session, participants completed demographic questionnaires and a brief training session on the computerized hybrid response inhibition (HRI) task. During Session 2, MRI brain scans were performed on the participants using a single Philips Achieva 3.0 Tesla MRI scanner to obtain an individual anatomical 3DT1 image for TBS targeting of the rIFG. Structural scans were acquired with a voxel size of 1 mm3, and functional MRI scans were collected during the HRI task (TR (repetition time) = 2000 ms, TE (echo time) = 30 ms, voxel size = 3 mm3). The participants received iTBS during Session 3 and cTBS during Session 4. The two TBS sessions were required to be a minimum of 24 hours and a maximum of 9 days apart. In Session 3, the researchers determined participants’ motor threshold in accordance with standard clinical practice.Reference McClintock, Reti and Carpenter21 During both Sessions 3 and 4, participants completed the state anxiety section of the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory and the HRI task before receiving TBS (pre-TBS). Immediately following TBS (post-TBS), participants completed the HRI task again (within 5 min) and then completed a comfort-rating questionnaire to report any immediate adverse effects. The morning after each TBS session, participants completed a follow-up questionnaire to document any next-day side effects.

TBS was administered using the MagVenture MagPro X100 stimulator equipped with the MagVenture Cool-B65 coil (MagVenture, Farum, Denmark). The TBS dose was calculated as 90% of a participant’s resting motor threshold, falling within the range of 35–65% intensity. TBS stimulation began at 20–25% intensity to acclimate participants to the sensation and was increased to the participant’s dose throughout stimulation at each session. Stimulation parameters for the iTBS consisted of bursts of pulses in triplets at 50 Hz, repeated at 5 Hz, with inter-train pauses of 8 s for 20 trains to deliver a total of 600 pulses over 189 s, aimed at increasing cortical excitability.Reference Huang, Edwards, Rounis, Bhatia and Rothwell15 Stimulation parameters for the cTBS consisted of pulses in triplets, each with a frequency of 50 Hz and delivered every 0.2 s, applied continuously for 40 s, resulting in a total of 600 pulses, aimed at decreasing cortical excitability.Reference Huang, Edwards, Rounis, Bhatia and Rothwell15

The method of neuronavigation for locating each participant’s rIFG was based on their neuroanatomical 3DT1 brain scan obtained in the MRI session. A frameless stereotaxic system to target the specific area of interest was used. Neuronavigation proceeded using the system Localite (Version 3, www.localite.de) to position the coil for maximal field strength at the rIFG for each participant. The rIFG was specified by reverse coregistration from a stereotaxic coordinate on the standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI-152) template brain onto each participant’s anatomical MRI. The MNI coordinates for the rIFG subregion pars opercularis were x, y, z = +52, +13, +8, drawn from a prior study reporting a functional subdivision of Broca’s area and confirmed with the Yale BioImage MNI to Talairach Converter Tool.Reference Gough, Nobre and Devlin22, Reference Papademetris, Jackowski and Rajeevan23 For all participants, the coil handle was oriented posteriorly (pointing backwards) and positioned tangentially to the scalp, perpendicular to the sagittal plane (ie, to an imaginary line running from the top of the head to the feet), to optimize stimulation perpendicular to the rIFG.

Measures

The HRI task is a newly developed, validated behavioral paradigm that assesses the three subcomponents of response inhibition.Reference Sebastian, Pohl and Kloppel11 In our study, the HRI task was conducted by replicating the design created by Sebastian et al.,Reference Sebastian, Pohl and Kloppel11 and programmed in the software Presentation (version 16.4, www.neurobs.com). The task consisted of four conditions: a congruent go condition (62.5%), an incongruent go condition (12.5%), a no-go condition (12.5%), and a stop condition (12.5%). The total duration of the HRI task took between 8 and 9 min to complete.

Interference inhibition is defined as the ability to suppress interfering response tendencies.Reference Sebastian, Baldermann and Feige24 Interference inhibition was measured by the Simon interference effect in the HRI task. The Simon interference effect was calculated by subtracting the mean reaction time (RT) of congruent trials from the mean RT of incongruent trials.

Action withholding is defined as the ability to withhold a motor response.Reference Sebastian, Baldermann and Feige24 Action withholding was determined by the percentage of no-go commission errors. Commission errors were measured as the percentage of errors made where the participant did not inhibit their response on the no-go and stop trials.

Action cancellation is defined as the ability to inhibit an already ongoing motor response.Reference Sebastian, Baldermann and Feige24 Action cancellation was measured with the stop signal RT (SSRT) and percentage of stop commission errors. The SSRT was estimated using the integration method by varying the stop-signal delay (SSD) and computing the SSRT (ms) scores by subtracting the mean SSD from the mean percent RT, replacing go-omission errors with the maximum RT to compensate for lacking responses, producing the most reliable and least biased nonparametric estimates.Reference Verbruggen, Aron and Band25 For further information on the HRI task, please see the paper by Sebastian et al.Reference Sebastian, Pohl and Kloppel11

Statistical analysis

The program G*Power version 3.1 was used to estimate the statistical power of the study, assuming a medium effect size of 0.25, a sample size of 24 participants, and a significance level of 0.05.Reference Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner and Lang26 The power analysis indicated a power of 0.65. The analysis included covariates determined through an independent-samples t-test to assess education differences between groups and a Fisher–Freeman–Halton test to explore the relationship between education level and group. This was to account for the significant correlation between education and scores on behavioral measures of response inhibition found in other literature.Reference Votruba and Langenecker27

A two-way mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) with a repeated-measures within– between interaction was used to conduct the analysis. The presence of outliers was assessed with visual inspection of boxplots. Standardized residuals were also calculated to assess the presence of residual outliers outside the range of ±3. The analysis was performed for the following variables: (i) pre-/post-iTBS mean RT of go trials; (ii) pre-/post-cTBS mean RT of go trials; (iii) pre-/post-iTBS Simon interference effect; (iv) pre-/post-cTBS Simon interference effect; (v) pre-/post-iTBS SSRT; and (vi) pre-/post-cTBS SSRT. Assumptions of normality, homogeneity of variance, and homogeneity of covariance were checked. The dependent variables that did not follow a normal distribution underwent transformations until the normality assumption was met. An interaction effect was first investigated between time (pre/post) and group (BD/control) on the outcome variables. Next, the main effects of time and group were assessed. The analyses were further explored by examining the pairwise comparisons of group and time based on estimated marginal means with the Bonferroni correction (to account for multiple comparisons) for all possible pairs of categories. The upper bound and lower bound 95% confidence intervals (95% CIs) were also evaluated. For a statistically significant difference, p ≤ 0.05.

The non-normal distribution of commission error percentages, attributed to a high concentration of zeroes (a floor effect where participants made no errors, reflecting ceiling-level performance), violated the ANOVA normality assumption. Thus, an ordinal logistic regression analysis was conducted for the following variables: (i) pre-/post-iTBS no-go commission errors; (ii) pre-/post-cTBS no-go commission errors; (iii) pre-/post-iTBS stop commission errors; and (iv) pre-/post-cTBS stop commission errors. To assess any baseline differences between the BD and control groups in pre-TBS commission errors while controlling for covariates, an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was conducted. The assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variances were checked. For the ordinal logistic regression analysis, the proportional odds assumption was checked. The model was assessed for goodness of fit, and a likelihood ratio (omnibus) test was conducted. The difference between the pre- and post-TBS commission errors was computed, and results were placed into three categories: decreased (improved) = 2, no change (stable) = 1, and increased (worsened) = 0. The odds ratio indicated the magnitude and direction of the effect. The “no change” category captures all participants whose error count did not change—whether they remained at zero (ceiling of optimal performance) or at any other level—thereby treating preserved performance as a non-worsened outcome. Thus, a value >1 indicated that BD is associated with a higher odd of having a stable or better outcome (ie, “no change” or “improved”), and a value >0 and <1 indicated that BD is associated with a higher odd of a worsened outcome. Finally, cross-tabulations were conducted to view the frequency and percentages of BD participants and controls in each category of the commission errors to visualize any differences between groups. All analyses were conducted in IBM Statistical Package (SPSS 28, IBM Corporation, Chicago, IL).

Results

The demographics of the sample are shown in Table 1. Education level differed between the BD and control groups (p = 0.03) and was therefore included as a covariate.

Table 1. Demographics of the Participants (N = 24)

Note. State anxiety scores were derived from the STAI. Higher scores indicate greater anxiety.

a N = 9 due to missing or declined to respond data.

b N = 11 due to missing or declined to respond data.

When controlled for education level, the group showed a significant effect (p = 0.01) on the pre-/post-iTBS no-go commission errors, a measure of action withholding. We found the BD group to have a strong effect on the no-go commission errors (Exp(B) = 31.92; 95% CI = 2.26, 450.52), indicating participants in the BD group made fewer no-go commission errors following iTBS. The odds ratio showed that the BD group had 32 times higher odds of having a stable or better outcome (“no change” or “improved”) following iTBS. Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 2. This was further investigated in the cross-tabulation (Table 3), where the BD group had 50% of their participants with improved no-go scores post iTMS compared to controls who had 16.7% of their participants with improved scores.

Table 2. Descriptive Statistics of iTBS No-Go Commission Errors by Group and Time Point

Table 3. Cross-Tabulation of iTBS No-Go Commission Errors

The ANCOVA revealed no significant differences between groups in pre-TBS no-go commission errors (F(1, 22) = 1.968, p = 0.175), indicating that the observed effects following iTBS are not attributable to preexisting disparities in no-go errors between the groups.

There was no significant effect of the group on the pre-/post-cTBS no-go commission errors. There were also no other significant interactions or main effects for the following outcome variables pre/post iTBS or cTBS, such as mean RT, Simon interference effect, SSRT, and stop commission errors. Therefore, the results showed the BD group, but not the control group, had significantly higher odds of improving on measures of action withholding following iTBS, but not after cTBS.

Participants’ comfort ratings immediately following each TBS session are summarized in Table 4. In terms of next-day adverse effects, of the 12 BD participants, 4 reported adverse effects after iTBS (1 mild headache, 2 mild–moderate fatigue, 1 mild light-headedness, 1 moderate agitation, 1 moderate neck pain, and 1 sleep disturbance). After cTBS, 6 BD participants reported adverse effects (3 mild–moderate headaches, 3 moderate fatigues, 1 moderate neck pain, and 1 sleep disturbance). Of the 9 controls with complete data (3 missing due to decline to respond or missing responses), none reported adverse effects the morning after iTBS, while 3 reported adverse effects after cTBS (1 mild headache, 1 sleep disturbance, and 1 mild discomfort).

Table 4. Adverse Effects Reported Immediately after TBS (Ratings: 1 = Not at all; 10 = Extremely)

a N = 9 due to missing or declined to respond data.

Discussion

The present study investigated the effects of increasing cortical excitability (iTBS) and decreasing cortical excitability (cTBS) of the rIFG on the performance scores of a novel computerized task measuring three subcomponents of response inhibition in BD and controls. The response inhibition subcomponent “action withholding”—measured by no-go commission errors—was significantly improved in the HRI task following iTBS for the BD group. There were no other significant effects observed.

A possible explanation for why increasing the cortical excitability of the rIFG only had a significant effect on action withholding and not interference inhibition or action cancellation is that the three subcomponents are required at different time points in the programming and generation of a response.Reference Sebastian, Pohl and Kloppel11 In the cognitive process of response inhibition, action withholding is positioned in between interference inhibition and action cancellation. Interference inhibition is an early subcomponent because it requires the inhibition of response tendencies that are involuntarily activated by incongruent stimuli, which are thought to arise before response initiation.Reference Jacobson, Javitt and Lavidor9 In contrast, action cancellation is considered a late subcomponent because it assesses the inhibition of an ongoing response.Reference Sebastian, Pohl and Kloppel11 Action withholding is an intermediate subcomponent of response inhibition because it comprises aspects of both action selection and inhibitory action.Reference Sebastian, Pohl and Kloppel11 Therefore, the rIFG may be more involved in the “intermediate stage” of the response inhibition process in individuals with BD.

Furthermore, interference inhibition and action cancellation both involve a spatial response selection in addition to a movement initiation, while action withholding only requires participants to decide between responding and withholding a response. Interference inhibition and action cancellation also have increased pre-supplemental motor area (pre SMA) activation relative to action withholding in the HRI task.Reference Sebastian, Pohl and Kloppel11 Therefore, the pre-SMA region might be more involved in downstream movement executions (eg, initiating and retracting the movement in action cancellation and performing a button press in interference inhibition).Reference Sebastian, Pohl and Kloppel11 In contrast, stimulation of the rIFG in BD might be more involved in withholding a response, and thus only improves the action withholding subcomponent. Future studies should consider investigating the differences between stimulating the rIFG and the pre-SMA, their effects on the three response inhibition subcomponents, and how each of these subcomponents may relate to behavioral and/or treatment outcomes.

In addition, recent theories of TMS effects suggest that short-term cortical reorganization may occur, triggering within-network compensation and potentially canceling out any TMS effects over the rIFG.Reference Friehs, Siodmiak and Donzallaz28 This could explain why no changes were observed following cTBS; decreasing cortical excitability may prompt another brain area to upregulate, compensating for the disruption caused by cTBS.Reference Friehs, Siodmiak and Donzallaz28 Future research is needed to further explore this hypothesis.

Limitations

There were some limitations in this study. First, the power of the study was determined to be 0.65, which is lower than the typical desired power of ≥0.80.Reference Hintze29 This reduced the chances of detecting a true effect and, therefore, the model was more likely to produce false negative results (Type II errors). Future investigations of iTBS in BD with a larger sample size would increase the power of the study and may therefore be able to detect more significant differences in the analyses.

Recruitment challenges also resulted in a predominantly female sample of BD participants (83.33% females and 16.67% males), with sex-matched controls to eliminate group differences. Globally, the male-to-female prevalence ratio for BD is 0.8.Reference Ferrari, Stockings and Khoo30 The gender imbalance in our sample does not represent the proportion of males and females with BD in the general population and may therefore influence the study’s external validity and restrict the extent to which the conclusions can be applied to the wider population.

The final limitation to our study was the lack of double-blinding of the type of TBS administered. iTBS was delivered during Session 2, and cTBS was delivered during Session 3. While the participants were unaware of the specific treatment order, the researchers were aware of the assigned protocols. Nonetheless, efforts were made to ensure Sessions 2 and 3 were conducted uniformly, differing only in the type of TBS administered. Furthermore, there was sufficient time between sessions, ranging from 24 h to 9 d apart, together with the brief aftereffects of a single rTMS session (<70 min).Reference Thut and Pascual-Leone31 This ensured the effect from the first session did not affect the second session. A placebo condition was not included to blind participants in the study, as the primary goal was to compare HRI performance scores pre and post each TBS session.

Conclusion

Our findings indicated that iTBS of the rIFG affected a specific subcomponent of response inhibition, ‘action withholding’, in individuals with BD that was not seen in controls. Overall, the exploratory analyses offered an initial evaluation of the effects of neuromodulation on response inhibition in BD, demonstrating the potential efficacy of this novel intervention. However, larger studies investigating rTMS of the rIFG are needed to draw definitive conclusions from our findings. In summary, this proof-of-concept study represented an important early step toward developing a novel therapeutic approach tailored to address the difficulties in impulse control often associated with BD to ultimately improve treatment outcomes.

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the participants for completing the study and the research assistants who helped with data collection.

Author contribution

Methodology: A.S.; Investigation: B.C.; Conceptualization: C.S., F.V., K.L.; Formal analysis: L.S.

Financial support

This work was supported by the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation (formerly NARSAD) (Grant ID: 25951; F18-02165).

Disclosures

Fidel Vila-Rodriguez receives research support from CIHR, Brain Canada, Michael Smith Foundation for Health Research, Vancouver Coastal Health Research Institute, and Weston Brain Institute for investigator-initiated research. He also receives philanthropic support from Seedlings Foundation and in-kind equipment support for this investigator-initiated trial from MagVenture. He received honoraria from Allergan for participation in an advisory board and serves as a volunteer director on the board of directors of the British Columbia Schizophrenia Society. Christian Schutz is on the Scientific Advisory Board of Clearmind Medicine and has a partner academic appointment with UBC, supported by funding from the Provincial Health Services Authority (PHSA), working as a psychiatrist and clinical scientist at PHSA. All other co-authors have nothing to disclose.

References

Smith, DJ, Whitham, EA, Ghaemi, SN. Bipolar disorder. Handb Clin Neurol. 2012;106:251–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-52002-9.00015-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moreira, ALR, Van Meter, A, Genzlinger, J, Youngstrom, EA. Review and meta-analysis of epidemiologic studies of adult bipolar disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2017;78(9):e1259–69. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.16r11165CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, JN, Black, DW. Bipolar disorder and suicide: a review. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2020;22(2):6. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-020-1130-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldessarini, RJ, Tondo, L, Vazquez, GH. Pharmacological treatment of adult bipolar disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2019;24(2):198217. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-018-0044-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dean, OM, Gliddon, E, Van Rheenen, TE, et al. An update on adjunctive treatment options for bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2018;20(2):8796. https://doi.org/10.1111/bdi.12601CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, SL, Carver, CS, Tharp, JA. Suicidality in bipolar disorder: the role of emotion-triggered impulsivity. Suicide Life Threat Behav. 2017;47(2):177–92. https://doi.org/10.1111/sltb.12274CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ajmi, S, Najjar, S, Masmoudi, J. Bipolar disorder’s treatment and impulsivity. Eur Psychiatry. 2021;64(S1):S200. https://doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2021.532CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, EK, Cohen, JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:167202. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobson, L, Javitt, DC, Lavidor, M. Activation of inhibition: diminishing impulsive behavior by direct current stimulation over the inferior frontal gyrus. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011;23(11):3380–7. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chan, CC, Alter, S, Hazlett, EA, et al. Neural correlates of impulsivity in bipolar disorder: a systematic review and clinical implications. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2023;147:105109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sebastian, A, Pohl, MF, Kloppel, S, et al. Disentangling common and specific neural subprocesses of response inhibition. Neuroimage. 2013;64:601–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garavan, H, Ross, TJ, Stein, EA. Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: an event-related functional MRI study. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1999;96(14):8301–6. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.96.14.8301CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hajek, T, Alda, M, Hajek, E, Ivanoff, J. Functional neuroanatomy of response inhibition in bipolar disorders: combined voxel based and cognitive performance meta-analysis. J Psychiatr Res. 2013;47(12):1955–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.08.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klomjai, W, Katz, R, Lackmy-Vallee, A. Basic principles of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and repetitive TMS (rTMS). Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2015;58(4):208–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rehab.2015.05.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, YZ, Edwards, MJ, Rounis, E, Bhatia, KP, Rothwell, JC. Theta burst stimulation of the human motor cortex. Neuron. 2005;45(2):201–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.033CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baeken, C, Brem, AK, Arns, M, et al. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation treatment for depressive disorders: current knowledge and future directions. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2019;32(5):409–15. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000533CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kozel, FA. Clinical repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for posttraumatic stress disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, and bipolar disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2018;41(3):433–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psc.2018.04.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman-Norlund, RD, Gibson, M, McConnell, PA, Froeliger, B. Dissociable effects of theta-burst repeated transcranial magnetic stimulation to the inferior frontal gyrus on inhibitory control in nicotine addiction. Front Psychiatry. 2020;11:260. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00260CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Konstantinou, G, Hui, J, Ortiz, A, et al. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) in bipolar disorder: a systematic review. Bipolar Disord. 2022;24(1):1026. https://doi.org/10.1111/bdi.13099CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheehan, DV, Lecrubier, Y, Sheehan, KH, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59(Suppl 20):2233.Google ScholarPubMed
McClintock, SM, Reti, IM, Carpenter, LL, et al. Consensus recommendations for the clinical application of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) in the treatment of depression. J Clin Psychiatry. 2018;79(1). https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.16cs10905CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gough, PM, Nobre, AC, Devlin, JT. Dissociating linguistic processes in the left inferior frontal cortex with transcranial magnetic stimulation. J Neurosci. 2005;25(35):8010–6. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2307-05.2005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papademetris, X, Jackowski, MP, Rajeevan, N, et al. BioImage Suite: an integrated medical image analysis suite: an update. Insight J. 2006;2006:209.Google ScholarPubMed
Sebastian, A, Baldermann, C, Feige, B, et al. Differential effects of age on subcomponents of response inhibition. Neurobiol Aging. 2013;34(9):2183–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.03.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verbruggen, F, Aron, AR, Band, GP, et al. A consensus guide to capturing the ability to inhibit actions and impulsive behaviors in the stop-signal task. Elife. 2019;8. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.46323Google ScholarPubMed
Faul, F, Erdfelder, E, Buchner, A, Lang, AG. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. 2009;41(4):1149–60. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Votruba, KL, Langenecker, SA. Factor structure, construct validity, and age- and education-based normative data for the parametric go/no-go test. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2013;35(2):132–46. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2012.758239CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friehs, MA, Siodmiak, J, Donzallaz, MC, et al. No effects of 1 Hz offline TMS on performance in the stop-signal game. Sci Rep. 2023;13(1):11565. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-38841-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hintze, JL. Power analysis and sample size system (PASS) for windows User’s Guide I. Kaysville, UT: NCSS; 2008. https://www.ncss.com/wp-content/uploads/2012/09/PASS11UG1.pdf. Accessed June 15, 2024.Google Scholar
Ferrari, AJ, Stockings, E, Khoo, JP, et al. The prevalence and burden of bipolar disorder: findings from the global burden of disease study 2013. Bipolar Disord. 2016;18(5):440–50. https://doi.org/10.1111/bdi.12423CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thut, G, Pascual-Leone, A. A review of combined TMS-EEG studies to characterize lasting effects of repetitive TMS and assess their usefulness in cognitive and clinical neuroscience. Brain Topogr. 2010;22(4):219–32. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10548-009-0115-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographics of the Participants (N = 24)

Figure 1

Table 2. Descriptive Statistics of iTBS No-Go Commission Errors by Group and Time Point

Figure 2

Table 3. Cross-Tabulation of iTBS No-Go Commission Errors

Figure 3

Table 4. Adverse Effects Reported Immediately after TBS (Ratings: 1 = Not at all; 10 = Extremely)