Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T15:26:55.413Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

14 - Beyond Flights of Fancy?

The Relations between Children’s Imaginary Companions, Creativity, and Coping

from Part III - Modes of Enhancement

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 November 2021

Sandra W. Russ
Affiliation:
Case Western Reserve University, Ohio
Jessica D. Hoffmann
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Both past and current research indicates that the creation of an imaginary companion is a common, normative, and healthy form of elaborated role-play that emerges in early childhood. Imaginary companions are often the invisible friends that children create for themselves, or the special stuffed animals or dolls that children imbue with personalities. Children often describe and experience their imaginary companions in ways that parallel real friendships with peers. Their emotional investment in these invented characters raises important questions about the broader roles they serve in their lives. Current research in this area has focused on the developmental significance of imaginary companions, and the extent to which they might have a real and meaningful impact on children’s development. This chapter reviews the extant literature on imaginary companions, with a particular focus on the relations between children’s imaginary companions, creativity, and coping with adversity.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aguiar, N. R., Mottweiler, C. M., Taylor, M., & Fisher, P. A. (2017). The imaginary companions created by children who have lived in foster care. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 36(4), 340355. doi:10.1177/0276236617700590Google Scholar
Benson, R. M., & Pryor, D. B. (1973). When friends fall out: Developmental interference with the function of some imaginary companions. Journal of American Psychoanalytic Association, 21(3), 457473. doi:10.1177/000306517302100301CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonne, O., Canetti, L., Bachar, E., De-Nour, A. K., & Shalev, A. (1999). Childhood imaginary companionship and mental health in adolescents. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 29(4), 277286. doi:10.1023/A:1021345015520Google Scholar
Busse, T. V., Blum, P., & Gutride, M. (1972). Testing conditions and the measurement of creative abilities in lower-class preschool children. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 7(3), 287298. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr0703_2Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., Tahiroglu, D., & Taylor, M. (2008). Links between dissociation and role play in a nonclinical sample of preschool children. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 9(2), 149171. doi:10.1080/15299730802045799Google Scholar
Christiano, B. A., & Russ, S. W. (1996). Play as a predictor of coping and distress in children during an invasive dental procedure. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 25(2), 130138. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp2502_1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, S. D. (1998). Childhood imagination in the face of chronic illness. In de Rivera, J & Sarbin, T. (Eds.), Believed-in imaginings: The narrative construction of reality (pp. 87100). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/10303-005Google Scholar
Coetzee, H., & Shute, R. (2003). “I run faster than him because I have faster shoes”: Perceptions of competence and gender role stereotyping in children’s imaginary friends. Child Study Journal, 33(4), 257272.Google Scholar
Colton, S., & Wiggins, G. A. (2012, August). Computational creativity: The final frontier?. Ecai, 12, 2126. doi:10.3233/978-1-61499-098-7-21Google Scholar
Davies, D., Jindal-Snape, D., Collier, C., Digby, R., Hay, P., & Howe, A. (2013). Creative learning environments in education – A systematic literature review. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 8, 8091. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2012.07.004Google Scholar
Freud, A. (1968). Indications and contraindications for child analysis. The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 23, 3746. doi:10.1177/000306517302100308CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gleason, T. R. (2004). Imaginary companions and peer acceptance. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28(3), 204209. doi:10.1080/01650250344000415Google Scholar
Gleason, T. R. (2002). Social provisions of real and imaginary relationships in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 38(6), 979992. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.979Google Scholar
Gleason, T. R. (2013). Imaginary relationships. In Taylor, M. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of the development of imagination. New York: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195395761.013.0017Google Scholar
Gleason, T. R. (2017). The psychological significance of play with imaginary companions in early childhood. Learning & Behavior, 45(4), 432440. doi:10.3758/s13420–017-0284-zGoogle Scholar
Gleason, T. R., & Hoffman, L. M. (2006). Concepts of real and imaginary friendships in early childhood. Social Development, 15(1), 128144. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2006.00333.xGoogle Scholar
Gleason, T. R., & Kalpidou, M. (2014). Imaginary companions and young children’s coping and competence. Social Development, 23(4), 820839. doi:10.1111/sode.12078Google Scholar
Gleason, T. R., Sebanc, A. M., & Hartup, W. W. (2000). Imaginary companions of preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 36(4), 419428. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.36.4.419Google Scholar
Gough, H. G. (1979). A creative personality scale for the Adjective Check List. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(8), 13981405. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.37.8.1398Google Scholar
Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Gupta, A., & Desai, N. G. (2006). Pathological fantasy friend phenomenon. International Journal of Psychiatry in Clinical Practice, 10(2), 149–15. doi:10.1080/13651500600578961Google Scholar
Harter, S., & Chao, C. (1992). The role of competence in children’s creation of imaginary friends. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38, 350363.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. V. (2004). A friend living inside me: The forms and functions of imaginary companions. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 24 (2), 151189. doi:10.2190/4M9J-76M2–4Q4Q-8KYTGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E. V. (2013). The relationship between pretend play and creativity. In Taylor, M. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of the development of imagination (pp. 403416). New York: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195395761.013.0026Google Scholar
Hoffner, C. (2008). Parasocial and online social relationships. In Calvert, S. L. & Wilson, B. J. (Eds.), The handbook of children, media, and development (pp. 309333). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. (D. Horton & R. R. Wohl, 1956). doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195395761.013.0026CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E., Rogers, P., & Rogers, C. (2010). The personality correlates of adults who had imaginary companions in childhood. Psychological Reports, 107(1), 163172. doi:10.2466/02.04.10.PR0.107.4.163-172Google Scholar
Kushnir, J., & Sadeh, A. (2012). Assessment of brief interventions for nighttime fears in preschool children. European Journal of Pediatrics, 171(1), 6775. doi:10.1007/s00431–011-1488-4Google Scholar
Lillard, A. S., & Kavanaugh, R. D. (2014). The contribution of symbolic skills to the development of an explicit theory of mind. Child Development, 85(4), 15351551. doi:10.1111/cdev.12227CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lillard, A. S., Lerner, M. D., Hopkins, E. J., Dore, R. A., Smith, E. D., & Palmquist, C. M. (2013). The impact of pretend play on children’s development: A review of the evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 139(1), 134. doi:10.1037/a0029321CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovinger, S. L. (1983). Multiple personality: A theoretical view. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research & Practice, 20(4), 425434. doi:10.1037/h0088503Google Scholar
Lynn, S. J., Rhue, J. W., & Green, J. P. (1988). Multiple personality and fantasy proneness: Is there an association or dissociation?, British Journal of Experimental & Clinical Hypnosis, 5(3), 138142.Google Scholar
Majors, K. (2013). Children’s perceptions of their imaginary companions and the purposes they serve: An exploratory study in the United Kingdom. Childhood, 20(4), 550565. doi:10.1177/0907568213476899Google Scholar
Majors, K., & Baines, E. (2017). Children’s play with their imaginary companions: Parent experiences and perceptions of the characteristics of the imaginary companions and purposes served. Educational and Child Psychology, 34(3), 3756.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manosevitz, M., Fling, S., & Prentice, N. M. (1977). Imaginary companions in young children: Relationships with intelligence, creativity and waiting ability. Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 18(1), 7378. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1977Google Scholar
Manosevitz, M., Prentice, N. M., & Wilson, F. (1973). Individual and family correlates of imaginary companions in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 8(1), 7279. doi:10.1037/h0033834Google Scholar
McLewin, L. A., & Muller, R. T. (2006). Childhood trauma, imaginary companions, and the development of pathological dissociation. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 11, 531545. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2006.02.001Google Scholar
Moriguchi, Y., & Todo, N. (2019). Prevalence of imaginary companions in Japanese children. International Journal of Psychology, 54(2), 269276. doi:10.1002/ijop.12446CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mottweiler, C. M. (2017). The development of creativity [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. University of Oregon.Google Scholar
Mottweiler, C. M., & Taylor, M. (2014). Elaborated role play and creativity in preschool age children. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8(3), 277. doi:10.1037/a0036083Google Scholar
Murphy, L. B. (1962). The widening world of childhood: Paths toward mastery. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Newson, J., & Newson, E. (1968). Four years old in an urban community. London: George Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1993). Friendship and friendship quality in middle childhood: Links with peer group acceptance and feelings of loneliness and social dissatisfaction. Developmental Psychology, 29(4), 611. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.29.4.611Google Scholar
Partington, J. T., & Grant, C. (1984). Imaginary playmates and other useful fantasies. In Smith, P. K. (Ed.), Play in animals and humans (pp. 217240). New York: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Pearson, D., Rouse, H., Doswell, S., Ainsworth, C., Dawson, O., Simms, K., … Faulconbridge, J. (2001). Prevalence of imaginary companions in normal child population. Child: Care, Health and Development, 27, 1222. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2214.2001.00167.xGoogle Scholar
Peterson, G., & Putnam, F. W. (1994). Preliminary results of the field trial of proposed criteria for dissociative disorder of childhood. Dissociation: Progress in the Dissociative Disorders, 7(4), 212220.Google Scholar
Putnam, F. W. (1997). Dissociation in children and adolescents: A developmental perspective. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Sadeh, A., Hen-Gal, S., & Tikotzky, L. (2008). Young children’s reactions to war related stress: A survey and assessment of an innovative intervention. Pediatrics, 121, 4653. doi:10.1542/peds.2007-1348Google Scholar
Sanders, B. (1992). The imaginary companion experience in multiple personality disorder. Dissociation: Progress in the Dissociative Disorders, 5(3), 159162.Google Scholar
Schaefer, C. E. (1969). Imaginary companions and creative adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 1, 747749. doi:10.1037/h0028270Google Scholar
Seiffge-Krenke, I. (1997). Imaginary companions in adolescence: Sign of a deficient or positive development?. Journal of Adolescence, 20, 137154. doi:10.1006/jado.1996.0072Google Scholar
Silberg, J. L. (1998). Dissociative symptomatology in children and adolescents as displayed on psychological testing. Journal of Personality Assessment, 71, 421439. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa7103_10Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J., Winterstein, B. P., Willse, J. T., Barona, C. M., Cram, J. T., Hess, K. I., … Richard, C. A. (2008). Assessing creativity with divergent thinking tasks: Exploring the reliability and validity of new subjective scoring methods. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2(2), 6885. doi:10.1037/1931-3896.2.2.68Google Scholar
Singer, D. G. (1993). Playing for their lives: Helping troubled children through play therapy. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Singer, J. L., & Singer, D. G. (1981). Television, imagination, and aggression: A study of preschoolers. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Singer, J. L., & Singer, D. G. (1992). The house of make-believe. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Smogorzewska, J. (2012). Storyline and associations pyramid as methods of creativity enhancement: Comparison of effectiveness in 5-year-old children. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 7(1), 2837. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2011.12.003Google Scholar
Soberman, L. H. (1995). Psychometric validation of a teacher screening instrument. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences, 56(2–A), 514.Google Scholar
Svendsen, M. (1934). Children’s imaginary companions. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 2, 985999.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tahiroglu, D., Mannering, A. M., & Taylor, M. (2011). Visual and auditory imagery associated with children’s imaginary companions. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 31(1), 99112. doi:10.2190/IC.31.1-2.iGoogle Scholar
Taylor, M. (1999). Imaginary companions and the children who create them. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, M., & Aguiar, N. R. (2013). How real is the imaginary? Children’s capacity to gain comfort from imaginary relationships. In Gelman, S. A & Banaji, M. R (Eds.), Navigating the social world: The early years. New York: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199890712.003.0021Google Scholar
Taylor, M., & Carlson, S. M. (1997). The relation between individual differences in fantasy and theory of mind. Child Development, 68(3), 436455. doi:10.2307/1131670Google Scholar
Taylor, M., Carlson, S. M., Maring, B. L., Gerow, L., & Charley, C. M. (2004). The characteristics and correlates of fantasy in school-age children: Imaginary companions, impersonation, and social understanding. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 11731187. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1173Google Scholar
Taylor, M., Carlson, S. M., & Shawber, A. B. (2007). Autonomy and control in children’s interactions with imaginary companions. In Roth, I. (Ed.), Imaginative minds. Oxford: British Academy and Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, M., Cartwright, B. S., & Carlson, S. M. (1993). A developmental investigation of children’s imaginary companions. Developmental Psychology, 29(2), 276. doi:10.1037/0012–1649.29.2.276Google Scholar
Taylor, M., Hulette, A. C., & Dishion, T. J. (2010). Longitudinal outcomes of young high-risk adolescents with imaginary companions. Developmental Psychology, 46(6), 1632. doi:10.1037/a0019815Google Scholar
Taylor, M., & Mannering, A. M. (2006). Of Hobbes and Harvey: The imaginary companions created by children and adults. In Göncü, A. & Gaskins, S. (Eds.), The Jean Piaget symposium series. Play and development: Evolutionary, sociocultural, and functional perspectives (pp. 227245). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Taylor, M., & Mottweiler, C. M. (2008). Imaginary companions: Pretending they are real but knowing they are not. American Journal of Play, 1(1), 4754.Google Scholar
Taylor, M., Sachet, A. B., Mannering, A. M., & Maring, B. L. (2013). The assessment of elaborated role-play in young children: Invisible friends, personified objects and pretend identities. Social Development, 22, 7593. doi:10.1111/sode.12011Google Scholar
Taylor, M., Shawber, A. B., & Mannering, A. M. (2009). Children’s imaginary companions: What is it like to have an invisible friend?. In Markman, K. D., Klein, W. M. P., & Suhr, J. A. (Eds.), Handbook of imagination and mental simulation. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Tellegen, A. (1982). Brief manual for the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire [Unpublished manuscript]. University of Minnesota, Minneapolis.Google Scholar
Trionfi, G., & Reese, E. (2009). A good story: Children with imaginary companions create richer narratives. Child Development, 80(4), 13011313. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01333.xGoogle Scholar
Trujillo, K., Lewis, D. O., Yeager, C. A., & Gidlow, B. (1996). Imaginary companions of school boys and boys with dissociative identity disorder/multiple personality disorder: A normal to pathological continuum. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 5, 375391.Google Scholar
Udwin, O. (1983). Imaginative play training as an intervention method with institutionalized children. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 53, 3239. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8279.1983.tb02533.xGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1967). Play and its role in the mental development of the child. Soviet Psychology, 5(3), 618.Google Scholar
Zeng, L., Proctor, R. W., & Salvendy, G. (2011). Can traditional divergent thinking tests be trusted in measuring and predicting real-world creativity?. Creativity Research Journal, 23(1), 2437. doi:10.1080/10400419.2011.545713Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×