Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T16:43:13.418Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Personality and Emotion

from Part I - Foundational Issues: History and Approaches to Personality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 September 2020

Philip J. Corr
Affiliation:
City, University London
Gerald Matthews
Affiliation:
University of Central Florida
Get access

Summary

Since its beginnings as a subdiscipline of psychology (e.g., Allport, 1937; Shand, 1914), personality psychologists have pursued two different, though related goals (see e.g., Cervone, 2005; Mischel & Shoda, 1998). The first goal is to construct a general theory of the person, understood as the integrated whole of the several subsystems of the mind. The second goal is to describe and explain the important psychological differences between individuals, that is, those relatively stable psychological attributes of individuals that allow us to uniquely characterize them and to distinguish them from each other. Most psychologists would agree that the emotion system is a central subsystem of personality, and that interindividual differences traceable to this system are important for describing individuals. However, if one accepts this, then it follows immediately that, to attain its goals, personality psychology must consider the emotions. In accordance with this conclusion, (1) most classical personality theorists proposed an affective (or affective–motivational) system as a core system of the mind (see, e.g., Shand, 1914; Murray, 1938), and emotions also play a prominent role in recent theories of personality (e.g., Mischel & Shoda, 1995). Furthermore, (2) most taxonomic models of stable and general (transsituational) psychological dispositions (usually called personality traits) include a subset of dispositions that refer directly or indirectly to emotions (see the second part of this chapter). Nonetheless, the in-depth investigation of emotions from a personality perspective has only begun comparatively recently, in the wake of an upsurge of interest in emotions that arose in the 1980s and continues to this day. Since that time, the previously largely separate fields of personality psychology and emotion psychology – the latter being the subdiscipline of psychology that studies emotions – are becoming increasingly integrated, to the benefit of both fields.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Aquino, K., Douglas, S., & Martinko, M. J. (2004). Overt anger in response to victimization: Attributional style and organizational norms as moderators. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 9, 152164.Google Scholar
Arnold, M. B. (1960). Emotion and personality (Vols. 1, 2). New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Aunger, R., & Curtis, V. (2013). The anatomy of motivation: An evolutionary-ecological approach. Biological Theory, 14, 4963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Averill, J. R. (1982). Anger and aggression: An essay on emotion. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
Barford, K. A., & Smilie, L. (2016). Mixed emotions, Big Five, openness, extraversion, neuroticism. Personality and Individual Differences, 102, 118122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bar-On, R. (1997). Emotional Quotient Inventory: Technical manual. Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
Barr, L. K., Kahn, J. H., & Schneider, W. J. (2008). Individual differences in emotion expression: Hierarchical structure and relations with psychological distress. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 27, 10451077.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., DeWall, C. N., & Zhang, L. (2007). How emotion shapes behavior: Feedback, anticipation, and reflection, rather than direct causation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 167203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumert, A., & Schmitt, M. (2016). Justice sensitivity. In Sabbagh, C. & Schmitt, M. (Eds.), Handbook of social justice theory and research (pp. 161180). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Bentham, J. (1789/1970). An introduction to the principles of morals and legislation. London: Athlone Press.Google Scholar
Bleidorn, W., Kandler, C., & Caspi, A. (2014). The behavioral genetics of personality development in adulthood: Classic, contemporary, and future trends. European Journal of Personality, 28, 244255.Google Scholar
Bouchard, T. J. Jr. (2004). Genetic influence on human psychological traits. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 148151.Google Scholar
Brinker, J. K., & Dozois, D. J. A. (2008). Ruminative thought styles and depressed mood. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65, 119.Google Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Grässmann, R. (1998). Personal goals and emotional well-being: The moderating role of motive dispositions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 494508.Google Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Maier, G. W. (1999). The pursuit of personal goals: A motivational approach to well-being and life adjustment. In Brandtstädter, J. & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Action and self-development: Theory and research through the life span (pp. 169196). New York: Sage.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2009). How can evolutionary psychology successfully explain personality and individual Differences? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 359366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, D. (1964). Repression-sensitization as a dimension of personality. In Maher, B. A. (Ed.), Progress in experimental personality research (Vol. 1, pp. 169220). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Feinstein, J. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 197253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carver, C. S. (2004). Negative affects deriving from the behavioral approach system. Emotion, 4, 322.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S. (2006). Approach, avoidance, and the self-regulation of affect and action. Motivation and Emotion, 30, 105110.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S., & Connor-Smith, J. (2010). Personality and coping. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 679704.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. G. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 267283.Google Scholar
Cervone, D. (2005). Personality architecture: Within-person structures and processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 423452.Google Scholar
Cox, W. M., & Klinger, E. (2004). Handbook of motivational counseling. Chichester, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error. New York: Avon.Google Scholar
Davis, M. H. (1983). The effects of dispositional empathy on emotional reactions and helping: A multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality, 51, 167184.Google Scholar
Day, A. L., Therrien, D. L., & Carroll, S. A. (2005). Predicting psychological health: Assessing the incremental validity of emotional intelligence beyond personality, type A behaviour, and daily hassles. European Journal of Personality, 19, 519536.Google Scholar
Deffenbacher, J. L., Oetting, E. R., Thwaites, G. A., Lynch, R. S., Baker, D. A., Stark, R. S., Thacker, S., & Eiswerth-Cox, L. (1996). State-trait anger theory and the utility of the trait anger scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 43, 131148.Google Scholar
Deffenbacher, J. L., Stephens, A. N., & Sullman, M. J. M. (2016). Driving anger as a psychological construct: Twenty years of research using the Driving Anger Scale. Transportation Research, Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 42, 236247.Google Scholar
Del Barrio, V., Aluja, A., & García, L. F. (2004). Relationship between empathy and the Big Five of personality traits in a sample of Spanish adolescents. Social Behavior and Personality, 32, 677682.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2005). Sources of Openness/Intellect: Cognitive and neuropsychological correlates of the fifth factor of personality. Journal of Personality, 73, 825858.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets and domains: 10 Aspects of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 880896.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diener, E., Smith, H., & Fujita, F. (1995). The personality structure of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 130141.Google Scholar
Durán, J. I., Reisenzein, R., & Fernández–Dols, J.-M. (2017). Coherence between emotions and facial expressions: A research synthesis. In Russell, J. A. & Fernández–Dols, J.-M. (Eds.), The science of facial expression (pp. 107129). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., & Eisenkraft, N. (2010). The relationship between displaying and perceiving nonverbal cues of affect: A meta-analysis to solve an old mystery. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 301318.Google Scholar
Ellsworth, P. C., & Scherer, K. R. (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R. & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of Affective Sciences (pp. 572595). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Emmons, R. A. (1986). Personal strivings: An approach to personality and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 10581068.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emmons, R. A. (1996). Striving and feeling: Personal goals and subjective well-being. In Gollwitzer, P. M. & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 313337). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Endler, N. S., Speer, R. L., Johnson, J. M., & Flett, G. L. (2001). General self-efficacy and control in relation to anxiety and cognitive performance. Current Psychology: Developmental, Learning, Personality, Social, 20, 3652.Google Scholar
Fletcher, G. J. O. (1993). The scientific credibility of commonsense psychology. In Craik, K. H., Hogan, R. & Wolde, R. N. (Eds.), Fifty years of personality psychology (pp. 251268). New York: Springer Science and Media.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. J., Hollenstein, T., & Mackey, A. M. (2010). The effect of suppressing and not accepting emotions on depressive symptoms: Is suppression different for men and women? Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 582586.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge,UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1994). Emotions are functional, most of the time. In Ekman, P. & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of emotion (pp. 112136). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1994). Three fundamental emotion systems. In Ekman, P. & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of emotion (pp. 243248). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Greifeneder, R., Bless, H., & Pham, M. T. (2011). When do people rely on affective and cognitive feelings in judgment? A review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15, 107141.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271299.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26, 126.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (1997). Revealing feelings: Facets of emotional expressivity in self-reports, peer ratings, and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 435448.Google Scholar
Güsewell, A., & Ruch, W. (2012). Are only emotional strengths emotional? Character strengths and disposition to positive emotions. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 4, 218239.Google Scholar
Hastings, M. E., Tangney, J. P., & Stuewig, J. (2008). Psychopathy and identification of facial expressions of emotion. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 14741483.Google Scholar
Hildebrandt, A., Olderbak, S., & Wilhelm, O. (2015). Facial emotion expression, individual differences. In Wright, J. D. (Ed.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (Vol. 8, 2nd ed.) (pp. 667675). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, E. T., Bond, R. N., Klein, R., & Strauman, T. (1986). Self-discrepancies and emotional vulnerability: How magnitude, accessibility, and type of discrepancy influence affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 515.Google Scholar
Hudlicka, E. (2011). Guidelines for developing computational models of emotions. International Journal of Synthetic Emotions, 2, 2679.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E., Libero, D. Z., Putnam, P., & Haynes, O. M. (1993). Stability of emotional experiences and their relations to traits of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 847860.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jensen-Campbell, L. A., Knack, J. M., Waldrip, A. M., & Campbell, S. D. (2007). Do Big Five personality traits associated with self-control influence the regulation of anger and aggression? Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 403424.Google Scholar
John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Individual differences in emotion regulation. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 351372). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In Pervin, L. A. & John, O. P. (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 102138). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Junge, M., & Reisenzein, R. (2013). Indirect scaling methods for testing quantitative emotion theories. Cognition and Emotion, 27, 12471275.Google Scholar
Kaiser, C. R., Major, B., & McCoy, S. K. (2004). Expectations about the future and the emotional consequences of perceiving prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 173184.Google Scholar
Krohne, H. W. (2003). Individual differences in emotional reactions and coping. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R. & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of affective science (pp. 698725). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kubiak, T., Wiedig-Allison, M., Zgoriecki, S., & Weber, H. (2011). Habitual goals and strategies in anger regulation: The anger-related reactions and goals inventory (ARGI). Journal of Individual Differences, 32, 113.Google Scholar
Kuppens, P. (2005). Interpersonal determinants of trait anger: Low agreeableness, perceived low social esteem, and the amplifying role of the importance attached to social relationships. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuppens, P. (2008). Individual differences in the relationship between pleasure and arousal. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 10531059.Google Scholar
Kuppens, P., & Tong, E. M. W. (2010). An appraisal account of individual differences in emotional experience. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 11381150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuppens, P., Van Mechelen, I., Smits, D. J. M., De Boeck, P., & Ceulemans, J. L. (2009). Individual differences in patterns of appraisal and anger experience. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 689713.Google Scholar
Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 132140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S., Averill, J. R., & Opton, E. M. Jr. (1970). Toward a cognitive theory of emotion. In Arnold, M. B. (Ed.), Feelings and emotions (pp. 207232). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Leki, E. F., & Wilkowski, B. M. (2017). Trait anger, neuroticism, and the hostile reaction to provocation: Examining the hierarchical organization of affective traits in context. Motivation and Emotion, 41, 713729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lerner, J. S., & Tiedens, L. Z. (2006). Portrait of the angry decision maker: How appraisal tendencies shape anger’s influence on cognition. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 115137.Google Scholar
Linden, W., Hogan, B. E., Rutledge, T., Chawla, A., Lenz, J. W., & Leung, D. (2003). There is more to anger coping than “in” or “out.” Emotion, 3, 1229.Google Scholar
Lotz, C., & Sparfeldt, J. R. (2017). Does test anxiety increase as the exam draws near? Students’ state test anxiety recorded over the course of one semester. Personality and Individual Differences, 104, 397400.Google Scholar
Lucas, R. E., & Fujita, F. (2000). Factors influencing the relation between extraversion and pleasant affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 10391056.Google Scholar
Luszczynska, A., Gutiérrez-Doña, B., & Schwarzer, R. (2005). General self-efficacy in various domains of human functioning: Evidence from five countries. International Journal of Psychology, 40, 8089.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyubomirsky, S., & Tkach, C. (2004). The consequences of dysphoric rumination. In Papageorgiou, C. & Wells, A. (Eds.), Depressive rumination: Nature, theory and treatment (pp. 2142). Chichester, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar
Marsella, S., Gratch, J., & Petta, P. (2010). Computational models of emotion. In Scherer, K., Bänziger, T. & Roesch, E. (Eds.), Blueprint for affective computing (pp. 2146). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mauss, I. B., & Robinson, M. D. (2009). Measures of emotion: A review. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 209237.Google Scholar
Mayer, J. D., Roberts, R. D., & Barsade, S. G. (2008). Human abilities: Emotional intelligence. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 507536.Google Scholar
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2004). Emotional intelligence: Theory, findings, and implications. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 197215.Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R. (2007). Aesthetic chills as a universal marker of openness to experience. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. Jr. (1999). A five-factor theory of personality. In Pervin, L. A. & John, O. P. (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 139153). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
McDougall, W. (1908/1960). An introduction to social psychology. London, UK: Methuen.Google Scholar
Meinong, A. (1894). Psychologisch-ethische Untersuchungen zur Werttheorie [Psychological-ethical investigations in value theory]. Graz, Austria: Leuschner & Lubensky.Google Scholar
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. Psychological Review, 102, 246268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1998). Reconciling processing dynamics and personality dispositions. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 229258.Google Scholar
Mitchell, A. E., Dickens, G. L., & Picchioni, M. M. (2014). Facial emotion processing in borderline personality disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology Review, 24, 166184.Google Scholar
Mitchell, S. (1995). Function, fitness and disposition. Biology and Philosophy, 10, 3954.Google Scholar
Mohiyeddini, C., & Schmitt, M. J. (1997). Sensitivity to befallen injustice and reactions to unfair treatment in a laboratory situation. Social Justice Research, 10, 333353.Google Scholar
Moors, A., Ellsworth, P. C., Scherer, K. R., & Frijda, N. H. (2013). Appraisal theories of emotion: State of the art and future development. Emotion Review, 5, 119124.Google Scholar
Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Newell, A. (1973). You can’t play 20 questions with nature and win. In Chase, W. G. (Ed.), Visual information processing (pp. 283308). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569582.Google Scholar
Olatunji, B. O., Naragon-Gainey, K., & Wolitzky-Taylor, K. B. (2013). Specificity of rumination in anxiety and depression: A multimodal meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 20, 225257.Google Scholar
Olderbak, S. G., Mokros, A., Nitschke, J., Habermeyer, E., & Wilhelm, O. (2018). Psychopathic men: Deficits in general mental ability, not emotion perception. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127, 294304.Google Scholar
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Parrott, W. G. (1993). Beyond hedonism: Motives for inhibiting good moods and for maintaining bad moods. In Wegner, D. M. & Pennebaker, J. W. (Eds.), Handbook of mental control (pp. 278305). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Pekrun, R. (1988). Emotion, motivation und persönlichkeit [Emotion, motivation and personality]. München: Psychologie Verlags Union.Google Scholar
Peña-Sarrionandia, A., Mikolajczak, A., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Integrating emotion regulation and emotional intelligence traditions: A meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, Educational Psychology, 6, 160.Google Scholar
Penke, L., Denissen, J. J. A., & Miller, G. F. (2007). The evolutionary genetics of personality. European Journal of Personality, 21, 549587.Google Scholar
Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (Eds.) (2004). Character strengths and virtues: A handbook and classification. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pfeiler, T. M., Weber, H., & Kubiak, T. (2018). Experiencing anger in a social interaction: The role of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 132, 4551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pretsch, J., Ehrhardt, N., Engl, L., Risch, B., Roth, J., Schumacher, S., & Schmitt, M. (2016). Injustice in school and students’ emotions, well-being, and behavior: A longitudinal study. Social Justice Research, 29, 119138.Google Scholar
Price, C. (2006). Affect without object: Moods and objectless emotions. European Journal of Analytic Philosophy, 2, 4968.Google Scholar
Pytlik Zillig, L. M., Hemenover, S. H., & Dienstbier, R. A. (2002). What do we assess when we assess a “Big Five” trait? A content analysis of the affective, behavioral, and cognitive (ABC) processes represented in Big Five personality inventories. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 847858.Google Scholar
Rafaeli, E., Rogers, G. M., & Revelle, W. (2007). Affective synchrony: Individual differences in mixed emotions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 915932.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R. (1996). Emotional action generation. In Battmann, W. & Dutke, S. (Eds.), Processes of the molar regulation of behavior (pp. 151165). Lengerich, Germany: Pabst Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R. (2001). Appraisal processes conceptualized from a schema-theoretic perspective: Contributions to a process analysis of emotions. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A. & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 187201). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R. (2007). What is a definition of emotion? And are emotions mental-behavioral processes? Social Science Information, 46, 424428.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R. (2009). Emotions as metarepresentational states of mind: Naturalizing the belief-desire theory of emotion. Cognitive Systems Research, 10, 620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reisenzein, R. (2012). What is an emotion in the belief-desire theory of emotion? In Paglieri, F., Tummolini, L., Falcone, R. & Miceli, M. (Eds.), The goals of cognition: Essays in honor of Cristiano Castelfranchi (pp. 181211). London, UK: College Publications.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R. (2015). On the universality of the attribution-affect model of helping. International Journal of Psychology, 50, 308311.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R. (2017). Cognitive theory of emotion. In Zeigler-Hill, V. & Shackleford, T. K. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences (pp. 111). New York: Springer International Publishing.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R., & Horstmann, G. (2018). Emotion. In Kiesel, A. & Spada, H. (Eds.), Lehrbuch Allgemeine Psychologie (4. Auflage, pp. 423492). Bern, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R., Hudlicka, E., Dastani, M., Gratch, J., Hindriks, K., Lorini, E., & Meyer, J.-J. C. (2013). Computational modeling of emotion: Toward improving the inter-and intradisciplinary exchange. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, 4, 246266.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R., Junge, M., Studtmann, M., & Huber, O. (2014). Observational approaches to the measurement of emotions. In Pekrun, R. & Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. (Eds.), International handbook of emotions in education (pp. 580606). Philadelphia, PA: Taylor & Francis/Routledge.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R., & Schimmack, U. (1999). Similarity judgments and covariations of affects: Findings and implications for affect structure research. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 539555.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R., & Siemer, M. (2013). Stimmungen aus der Sicht der kognitiv-motivationalen Theorie der Emotion [Moods from the perspective of the cognitive-motivational theory of emotion]. In Reents, F. & Meyer-Sickendiek, B. (Eds.), Stimmung und Methode [Mood and method] (pp. 95117). Tübingen, Germany: Mohr Siebeck.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R., Meyer, W.-U., & Niepel, M. (2012). Surprise. In Ramachandran, V. S. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human behavior (2nd ed., pp. 564570). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R., & Spielhofer, C. (1994). Subjectively salient dimensions of emotional appraisal. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reiss, S. (2000). Who am I? The 16 basic desires that motivate our actions and define our personality. New York: Tarcher Putnam.Google Scholar
Revelle, W., & Wilt, J. (2016). The data box and within subject analyses: A comment on Nesselroade and Molenaa. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 51, 419423.Google Scholar
Riggio, H. R., & Riggio, R. (2002). Emotional expressiveness, extraversion, and neuroticism: A meta-analysis. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 26, 195218.Google Scholar
Roseman, I. J. (1979, September). Cognitive aspects of emotions and emotional behavior. Paper presented at the 87th Annual Convention of the APA, New York City.Google Scholar
Roseman, I. J., Antoniou, A. A., & Jose, P. E. (1996). Appraisal determinants of emotions: Constructing a more accurate and comprehensive theory. Cognition and Emotion, 10, 241277.Google Scholar
Roseman, I. R., & Smith, C. A. (2001). Appraisal theory: Overview, assumptions, varieties, controversies. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A. & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 319). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruch, W. (1997). State and trait cheerfulness and the induction of exhilaration: A FACS study. European Psychologist, 2, 328341.Google Scholar
Rudolph, U., Roesch, S., Greitemeyer, T., & Weiner, B. (2004). A meta-analytic review of help giving and aggression from an attributional perspective: Contributions to a general theory of motivation. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 815848.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145172.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. M. (1999). On the bipolarity of positive and negative affect. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 330.Google Scholar
Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 9, 185211.Google Scholar
Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (2001). Optimism, pessimism, and psychological well-being. In Chang, E. C. (Ed.), Optimism and pessimism: Implications for theory, research, and practice (pp. 189216). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (2007). Componential emotion theory can inform models of emotional competence. In Matthews, G., Zeidner, M. & Roberts, R. (Eds.), Science of emotional intelligence: Knowns and unknowns (pp. 101126). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (1984). On the nature and function of emotion: A component process approach. In Scherer, K. R. & Ekman, P. (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 293317). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (2001). Appraisal considered as a process of multilevel sequential checking. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A. & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 92120). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Schimmack, U. (1997). Affect intensity: Separating intensity and frequency in repeatedly measured affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 13131329.Google Scholar
Schimmack, U. (2001). Pleasure, displeasure, and mixed feelings: Are semantic opposites mutually exclusive? Cognition and Emotion, 15, 8197.Google Scholar
Schimmack, U. (2003). Affect measurement in experience sampling research. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4, 79106.Google Scholar
Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2002). Cultural influences on the relation between pleasant emotions and unpleasant emotions: Asian dialectic philosophies or individualism-collectivism? Cognition and Emotion, 16, 705719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmitt, M. (1996). Individual differences in sensitivity to befallen injustice. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 320.Google Scholar
Schmukle, S. C., Egloff, B., & Burns, L. R. (2002). The relationship between positive and negative affect in the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 463475.Google Scholar
Schutte, N., Malouff, J., Thorsteinsson, E., Bhullar, N., & Rooke, S. (2007). A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between emotional intelligence and health. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 921933.Google Scholar
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2007). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In Kruglanski, A. W. & Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (2nd ed., pp. 385407). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1995). Generalized Self-Efficacy scale. In Weinman, J., Wright, S. & Johnston, M. (Eds.), Measures in health psychology: A user’s portfolio. Causal and control beliefs (pp. 3537). Windsor, UK: NFER-NELSON.Google Scholar
Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1999). Skalen zur Erfassung von Lehrer- und Schülermerkmalen [Scales for the assessment of teacher and student characteristics]. Berlin, Germany: Freie Universität Berlin.Google Scholar
Shand, A. F. (1914). The foundations of character. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Sheldon, K. M., Elliot, A. J., Kim, Y., & Kasser, T. (2001). What is satisfying about satisfying events? Testing 10 candidate psychological needs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 325339.Google Scholar
Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., & John, O. P. (2006). Positive emotion dispositions differentially associated with Big Five personality and attachment style. Journal of Positive Psychology, 1, 6171.Google Scholar
Siemer, M. (2009). Mood experience: Implications of a dispositional theory of moods. Emotion Review, 1, 256263.Google Scholar
Siemer, M. (2001). Mood-specific effects on appraisal and emotion judgments. Cognition & Emotion, 15, 453485.Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1967). Motivational and emotional controls of cognition. Psychological Review, 74, 2939.Google Scholar
Slovic, P., Peters, E., Finucane, M. L., & MacGregor, D. G. (2005). Affect, risk, and decision making. Health Psychology, 24, 3540.Google Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Kirby, L. D. (2001). Toward delivering on the promise of appraisal theory. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A. & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 121138). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, T. W. (1992). Hostility and health: Status of a psychosomatic hypothesis. Health Psychology, 11, 139150.Google Scholar
Smits, D. J. M., De Boeck, P., Kuppens, P., & van Mechelen, I. (2002). The structure of negative emotion scales: Generalization over contexts and comprehensiveness. European Journal of Personality, 16, 127141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solberg Nes, L., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2006). Dispositional optimism and coping: A meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 235251.Google Scholar
Spielberger, C. D. (1999). Manual for the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory-2. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Stark, R., Walter, B., Schienle, A., & Vaitl, D. (2005). Psychophysiological correlates of disgust and disgust sensitivity. Journal of Psychophysiology, 19, 5060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stouten, J., De Cremer, D., & van Dijk, E. (2006). Violating equality in social dilemmas: Emotional and retributive reactions as a function of trust, attribution, and honesty. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 894906.Google Scholar
Symister, P., & Friend, R. (2003). The influence of social support and problematic support on optimism and depression in chronic illness: A prospective study evaluating self-esteem as a mediator. Health Psychology, 4, 249288.Google Scholar
Tamir, M., Chiu, C.-Y., & Gross, J. E. (2007). Business or pleasure? Utilitarian versus hedonic considerations in emotion regulation. Emotion, 7, 546554.Google Scholar
Tellegen, A. (1985). Structures of mood and personality and their relevance to assessing anxiety, with an emphasis on self-report. In Tuma, A. H. & Maser, J. D. (Eds.), Anxiety and the anxiety disorders (pp. 681706). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Terracciano, A., McCrae, R. R., Hagemann, D., & Costa, P. T. Jr. (2003). Individual difference variables, affective differentiation, and the structures of affect. Journal of Personality, 71, 669703.Google Scholar
Thomas, N., Baumert, A., & Schmitt, M. (2013). Moderators of the link between the perception of injustice and emotional reactions. In Mohiyeddini, C. (Ed.), Emotional relationships: Types, challenges and physical/mental health impacts (pp. 2338). New York: Nova Publishers.Google Scholar
Thomsen, D. K. (2006). The association between rumination and negative affect: A review. Cognition and Emotion, 20, 12161235.Google Scholar
Tignor, S. M., & Colvin, C. R. (2017). The interpersonal adaptiveness of dispositional guilt and shame: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Personality, 85, 341363.Google Scholar
Tong, E. M. W. (2010). Personality influences in appraisal-emotion relationships: The role of neuroticism. Journal of Personality, 78, 398417.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990). On the universality of human nature and the uniqueness of the individual: The role of genetics and adaptation. Journal of Personality, 58, 1767.Google Scholar
Vanderbeeken, R., & Weber, E. (2002). Dispositional explanations of behavior. Behavior and Philosophy, 30, 43–59.Google Scholar
Van Rooy, D. L., & Viswesvaran, C. (2004). Emotional intelligence: A meta-analytic investigation of predictive validity and nomological net. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 65, 7195.Google Scholar
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1992). On traits and temperament: General and specific factors of emotional experience and their relation to the Five-Factor Model. Journal of Personality, 60, 441476.Google Scholar
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1997). Measurement and mismeasurement of mood: Recurrent and emergent issues. Journal of Personality Assessment, 68, 267296.Google Scholar
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 10631070.Google Scholar
Weber, H., & Wiedig-Allison, M. (2007). Sex differences in anger-related behaviour: Comparing expectancies to actual behaviour. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 16691698.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1995). Judgments of responsibility: A foundation for a theory of social conduct. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Wilkowski, B. M., & Robinson, M. D. (2008). The cognitive basis of trait anger and reactive aggression: An integrative analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12, 321.Google Scholar
Wilt, J., & Revelle, W. (2008). Extraversion. In Leary, M. & Hoyle, R. H. (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in social behavior (pp. 2745). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Wolpe, J., & Lang, P. J. (1964). A fear survey schedule for use in behavior therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 2, 2730.Google Scholar
Wrzus, C., & Roberts, B. W. (2017). Processes of personality development in adulthood: The TESSERA Framework. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 21, 253277.Google Scholar
Zeidner, M. (2014). Anxiety in education. In Pekrun, R. & Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. (Eds.), International handbook of emotions in education (pp. 265288). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×