Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T04:23:57.207Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

20 - Symptom Perception

from Theme 4: - Responding to Symptoms

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2019

Carrie D. Llewellyn
Affiliation:
University of Sussex
Susan Ayers
Affiliation:
City, University of London
Chris McManus
Affiliation:
University College London
Stanton Newman
Affiliation:
City, University of London
Keith J. Petrie
Affiliation:
University of Auckland
Tracey A. Revenson
Affiliation:
City University of New York
John Weinman
Affiliation:
King's College London
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Affleck, G., Tennen, H., Urrows, S. & Higgins, P. (1992). Neuroticism and the pain– mood relation in rheumatoid arthritis: insights from a prospective daily study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 119126.Google Scholar
Bogaerts, K., Wan, L., Van Diest, I., et al. (2012). Peak–end memory bias in laboratory-induced dyspnea: a comparison of patients with medically unexplained symptoms and healthy controls. Psychosomatic Medicine 74, 974981.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Constantinou, E., Bogaerts, K., Van Diest, I. & Van den Bergh, O. (2013). Inducing symptoms in high symptom reporters via emotional pictures: the interactive effects of valence and arousal. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 74, 191196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, P. T. Jr & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Somatic complaints in males as a function of age and neuroticism: a longitudinal analysis. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 3(3), 245257.Google Scholar
Cox, D. J., Clarke, W. L., Gonder-Frederick, L., et al. (1985). Accuracy of perceiving blood glucose in IDDM. Diabetes Care, 8, 529538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crichton, F., Dodd, G., Schmid, G., et al. (2014). The power of positive and negative expectations to influence reported symptoms and mood during exposure to wind farm sound. Health Psychology, 33, 15881592.Google Scholar
Faasse, K. & Petrie, K. J. (2013). The nocebo effect: patient expectations and medication side effects. Postgraduate Medical Journal, 89, 540546.Google Scholar
Faasse, K. & Petrie, K. J. (2016). From me to you: the effect of social modelling on treatment outcomes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25, 438443.Google Scholar
Faasse, K., Grey, A., Horne, R. & Petrie, K. J. (2015). High perceived sensitivity to medicines is associated with higher medical care utilisation, increased symptom reporting and greater information‐seeking about medication. Pharmacoepidemiology and Drug Safety, 24(6), 592599. DOI: 10.1002/pds.3751CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fahrenberg, J., Franck, M., Baas, U. & Jost, E. (1995). Awareness of blood pressure: introception or contextual judgement?. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 39, 1118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, P. J., Cohen, S., Doyle, W. J., Skoner, D. P. & Gwaltney, J. M. Jr (1999). The impact of personality on the reporting of unfounded symptoms and illness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(2), 370378.Google Scholar
Fibrinolytic Therapy Trialists’ (FTT) Collaborative Group. (1994). Indications for fibrinolytic therapy in suspected acute myocardial infarction: collaborative overview of early mortality and major morbidity results from all randomized trials of more than 1000 patients. Lancet, 343, 311322.Google Scholar
Horne, R., Faasse, K., Cooper, V., et al. (2012). The perceived sensitivity to medicines (PSM) scale: an evaluation of validity and reliability. British Journal of Health Psychology, 18, 1830.Google Scholar
Jewett, D. L., Fein, G. & Greenberg, M. H. (1990). A double-blind study of symptom provocation to determine food sensitivity. New England Journal of Medicine, 323(7), 429433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D., Krueger, D. A., Schkade, D. A., Schwarz, N. & Stone, A. A. (2004). A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: the day reconstruction method. Science, 306, 17761780Google Scholar
Kendrick, A. H., Higgs, C. M., Whitfield, M. J. & Laszlo, G. (1993). Accuracy of perception of severity of asthma: patients treated in general practice. BMJ, 307, 422424.Google Scholar
Meechan, G., Collins, J. & Petrie, K. (2002). Delay in seeking medical care for self-detected breast symptoms in New Zealand women. The New Zealand Medical Journal, 115(1166), U257.Google Scholar
Meyer, D., Leventhal, H., & Gutmann, M. (1985). Common-sense models of illness: the example of hypertension. Health Psychology, 4, 115135.Google Scholar
Myers, M. G., Cairns, J. A. & Singer, J. (1987). The consent form as a possible source of side effects. Clinical Pharmacology Therapy, 42, 250253.Google Scholar
Page, L. A., Petrie, K. & Wessely, S. C. (2006). Psychosocial responses to environmental incidents: a review and a proposed typology. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 60, 413422.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W. (1980). Perceptual and environmental determinants of coughing. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 1, 8391.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W. (1981). Stimulus characteristics influencing estimation of heart rate. Psychophysiology, 18, 540548.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W. (1994). Psychological bases of symptom reporting: perceptual and emotional aspects of chemical sensitivity. Toxicology and Industrial Health, 10, 497511.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W. (2000). Psychological factors influencing the reporting of physical symptoms. In Stone, A. A., Turkkan, J. S., Bachrach, C. A., et al. (eds) The Science of Self-Report: Implications for Research and Practice (pp. 299315). London: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W. and Epstein, D. (1983). Implicit psychophysiology: effects of common beliefs and idiosyncratic physiological responses symptom reporting. Journal of Personality, 51, 468496.Google Scholar
Perry, K., Petrie, K. J., Ellis, C. J., Horne, R. & Moss-Morris, R. (2001). Symptom expectations and delay in acute myocardial infarction patients. Heart, 86, 9193.Google Scholar
Petrie, K. & Weinman, J. A. (2003). More focus needed on symptom appraisal. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 54, 401403.Google Scholar
Petrie, K. & Weinman, J. (2012). Patients’ perceptions of their illness: the dynamo of volition in health care. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 6065.Google Scholar
Petrie, K., Moss-Morris, R., Grey, C. & Shaw, M. (2004). The relationship of negative affect and perceived sensitivity to symptom reporting following vaccination. British Journal of Health Psychology, 9, 101111.Google Scholar
Petrie, K. J., Faasse, K., Crichton, F. & Grey, A. (2014). How common are symptoms? Evidence from a New Zealand national telephone survey. BMJ Open, 4(6).Google Scholar
Richards, M. A., Westcombe, A. M., Love, S. B., Littlejohns, P. & Ramirez, A. J. (1999). Influence of delay on survival in patients with breast cancer: a systematic review. Lancet, 353, 11191126.Google Scholar
Rief, W. & Broadbent, E. (2007). Explaining medically unexplained symptoms: models and mechanisms. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 821841.Google Scholar
Rief, W., Nanke, A., Emmerich, J., Bender, A. & Zech, T. (2004). Causal illness attributions in somatoform disorders associations with comorbidity and illness behavior. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 57, 367371.Google Scholar
Salovey, P. & Birnbaum, D. (1989). Influence of mood on health-relevant cognitions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 539551.Google Scholar
Severeijns, R., Vlaeyen, J. W. S. & van den Hout, M. A. (2004). Pain catastrophizing is associated with health indices in musculoskeletal pain: a cross-sectional study in the Dutch community. Health Psychology, 23, 4957.Google Scholar
Tan, K., Petrie, K. J., Faasse, K., Bolland, M. J. & Grey, A. (2014). Unhelpful information about adverse drug reactions. BMJ, 349, g5019.Google Scholar
Van den Bergh, O. & Walentynowicz, M. (2016). Accuracy and bias in retrospective symptom reporting. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 29, 302308.Google Scholar
Van den Bergh, O., Winters, W., Devriese, S., et al. (2004). Accuracy of respiratory symptom perception in persons with high and low negative affectivity. Psychology & Health, 19, 213222.Google Scholar
Verbrugge, L. M. (1985). Triggers of symptoms and health care. Social Science & Medicine, 20, 855876.Google Scholar
Walentynowicz, M., Bogaerts, K., Van Diest, I., Raes, F. & Van den Bergh, O. (2015). Was it so bad? The role of retrospective memory in symptom reporting. Health Psychology, 34, 11661174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D. & Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Health complaints, stress, and distress: exploring the central role of negative affectivity. Psychological Review, 96, 234254.Google Scholar
Zijlema, W. L., Stolk, R. P., Löwe, B., et al. (2013). How to assess common somatic symptoms in large-scale studies: a systematic review of questionnaires. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 74, 459468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×