Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-s2hrs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T05:34:18.619Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 1 - Climate Distress among Young People

An Overview

from Part I - Conceptual Foundations of Climate Distress in Young People

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 June 2024

Elizabeth Haase
Affiliation:
University of Nevada, Reno
Kelsey Hudson
Affiliation:
Climate Psychology Alliance North America

Summary

It is well established that climate change poses significant threats to human health and well-being. Young people, who face a future burdened by climate change, will be among those most affected. It is understandable that increasing awareness of these threats brings increasing distress. Many young people will experience anxiety and other distressing emotions in relation to climate change, but some may experience a level of anxiety that threatens their mental health. In order to provide help needed by young people who are affected, there is a need to further understand the nature, predictors and consequences of climate distress. This chapter thus provides an overview of what we know and what we need to know about young people who experience climate-related distress.

Type
Chapter
Information
Climate Change and Youth Mental Health
Multidisciplinary Perspectives
, pp. 3 - 20
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2024

Introduction

Awareness of climate change is growing. Along with the increasingly definitive and detailed visible evidence that the climate is changing, there is increased recognition of negative impacts on human well-being. The sixth assessment report from the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC) described the threats, from impacts on physical health through exposure to disasters, heat, poor air quality, and disease vectors; to impacts on food systems and the economy; and to negative effects on social inequity and individual mental health (IPCC, Reference Pörtner, Roberts, Tignor, Poloczanska, Mintenbeck, Alegría, Craig, Langsdorf, Löschke, Möller, Okem and Rama2022).

This increased awareness brings increasing worry. A 2022 international survey, for example, found that more than nine out of ten respondents in seven countries (among 192 countries and territories surveyed) were worried about climate change. Majorities in most countries expected climate change to harm future generations and to threaten their own country or territory in the next twenty years, although a smaller number expected to be personally harmed (Leiserowitz et al., Reference Leiserowitz, Carman, Buttermore, Neyens, Rosenthal, Marlon, Schneider and Mulcahy2022). In a 2021 survey of 10,000 young people around the world (Hickman et al., Reference Hickman, Marks, Pihkala, Clayton, Lewandowski, Mayall and van Susteren2021), more than half said that they were “extremely” or “very” worried about climate change, and only 5 percent said they were not worried.

In the face of these challenges, worry and anxiety in response to climate change are not surprising. Worry and anxiety can be adaptive when people are faced with a threat. Where worry is the usual reaction to negative news, anxiety is a future-oriented emotional response that tends to focus attention on the need to respond to a problem. In this way it can be associated with appropriate preparations for the imminent threat, such as behavioral modifications that will decrease the threat and/or increase the ability to cope with it. However, extreme levels of anxiety can become overwhelming, associated with the maintenance of unresolved worry and continued focus on a problem. When anxiety is difficult to control or manage, causes difficulty functioning in important domains (e.g., work, family, life), and is accompanied by physical symptoms and severe distress (Barlow, Reference Barlow2000), it could be considered a disorder.

Climate change is beyond the ability of any individual to fully resolve. Indeed, as a “wicked” problem (i.e., an issue that is complex, challenging, or impossible to solve), it is unlikely to be fully resolved in any manner. Instead, it describes an evolving state of affairs whose full impacts cannot be clearly predicted but which is likely to reduce quality of life for almost everyone. Thus, it is reasonable that thinking about climate change would evoke some degree of anxiety. One way for people to respond is through emotion-focused coping, which involves managing their negative emotions, for example by avoiding the issue or by cognitively reframing the issue as not important. This is a maladaptive response as it fails to address the problem, which becomes even larger as we ignore it. But facing the issue may also be problematic if the negative emotions become overwhelming, leaving the anxious individual paralyzed with fear (Lazarus & Folkman, Reference Lazarus and Folkman1984).

As children, adolescents, and young adults face the reality of an altered climate that threatens human well-being, many will experience anxiety and other distressing emotions. This is not a pathological response or de facto evidence of mental illness, but for some the anxiety will reach levels that impair mental health. In order to help those who experience these powerful impacts, it is important to understand the nature, predictors, and consequences of climate change-related distress and anxiety.

Describing Climate Anxiety

In recent years, media and anecdotal reports about “climate anxiety,” “climate change anxiety,” or “eco-anxiety” have grown substantially. In order to know what climate anxiety is and whether climate anxiety itself is growing, however, there needs to be a consistent definition and a reliable way of assessing it. One key question concerns its intensity and the extent to which it is “practical” or “paralyzing.” Many surveys have simply asked about climate worry or concern, which does not necessarily imply a reaction that is intense enough to motivate behavior or to threaten mental health. Based on her experiences talking with children, often in therapeutic settings, Hickman (Reference Hickman2020) distinguished multiple levels of eco-anxiety. The “significant” and “severe” levels were associated with emotional distress, with impacts on cognition and behavior. Similarly, in discussing climate anxiety, Pihkala (Reference Pihkala2022) distinguished between “practical” or “constructive” anxiety, and “paralyzing” anxiety, to acknowledge the different behavioral responses, or lack of response, that may accompany individuals’ experiences of anxiety.

A systematic approach to assessing the potential clinical implications was developed by Clayton and Karazsia (Reference Clayton and Karazsia2020), who focused on the question of whether climate change anxiety (CCA) might indicate the kind of functional impairment that would make it clinically relevant. Their paper presented a Climate Change Anxiety Scale (CCAS) that was based on established measures of rumination and functional impairment. In two studies based on US samples, the CCAS showed a reliable structure with two components: cognitive/emotional impairment, associated with difficulty concentrating or regulating emotions; and functional impairment, associated with difficulty working, socializing, or having fun. Notably, both components were significantly correlated with having experience of climate change (e.g., experienced a natural weather disaster) and with a short measure of depression and anxiety. Levels of climate anxiety were generally low, with about 3–10 percent of respondents saying they “often or almost always” experienced these symptoms, and as many as 20–25 percent saying they “sometimes” experienced them (Clayton & Karazsia, Reference Clayton and Karazsia2020). A later US study (Schwartz et al., Reference Schwartz, Benoit, Clayton, Parnes, Swenson and Lowe2022), using separate measures of depression – the Patient Health Questionnaire-8 (PHQ-8) – and anxiety – the General Anxiety Disorder-7 (GAD-7), found that CCAS scores were significantly correlated with both variables. There was no correlation with age in this study, but the range was limited because the sample consisted mostly of undergraduate students.

In the past few years, multiple studies have used the CCAS in other countries. Wullenkord et al. (Reference Wullenkord, Toger, Hamann, Loy and Reese2021) found that the CCAS was reliable in Germany, though they did not replicate the factor structure of the original study. Levels of climate anxiety were low and did not differ according to age, education, or income, though women reported higher levels of climate anxiety. CCAS scores were positively correlated with anxiety and depression, support for pro-environmental policies, and pro-environmental behavior intentions. In a Polish study (Larionow et al., Reference Larionow, Sołtys, Izdebski, Mudło-Głagolska, Golonka, Demski and Rosińska2022), women, again, showed higher levels of climate anxiety; age and education were both slightly negatively correlated with CCAS. Subscale scores were related to depression but not anxiety using the PHQ-4. A similar factor structure was reported in Belgium (Mouguiama-Daouda et al., Reference Mouguiama-Daouda, Blanchard, Coussement and Heeren2022). In a large sample from multiple French-speaking countries, almost 12 percent of the sample reported experiencing anxiety frequently (Heeren et al., Reference Heeren, Mouguiama-Daouda and Contreras2022). Women and younger adults showed higher climate anxiety, but there were no differences associated with education. In a sample of adolescents from the Philippines (Simon et al., Reference Simon, Pakingan and Aruta2022), higher scores on the CCAS again related positively to both experience of climate change and behavioral engagement. Although the samples cannot be directly compared, it is worth noting that scores were higher in the Philippines than in the US studies. In another study from the Philippines, CCAS was negatively correlated with mental health, and positively correlated with psychological distress (Reyes et al., Reference Reyes, Carmen, Luminarias, Mangulabnan and Ogunbode2021). In samples from India, China, and Japan, as well as the United States, Tam et al. (Reference Tam, Chan and Clayton2023) found that a two-factor structure was reliable across countries, but demographic predictors varied. In sum, these studies suggest the CCAS is a reliable instrument that can discriminate climate anxiety from clinical anxiety and depression across a variety of populations, although further work should explore differences in levels and discrepancies in the factor structures.

Climate change anxiety by no means represents the only emotional response to climate change, or even the only negative emotion. Several researchers have emphasized grief or depression as a response to climate change (Cunsolo & Ellis, Reference Cunsolo and Ellis2018; Ojala et al., Reference Ojala, Cunsolo, Ogunbode and Middleton2021); this grief or sadness is based on the perceived loss of something valued, such as a healthy environment or specific natural places, and is thus likely to be stronger among people who place a higher value on the natural environment and/or have greater experience with the loss or change of places that hold importance to them (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Leviston, Hurlstone, Lawrence and Walker2018). Many people also experience emotions with a moral judgment component: personal guilt or shame; anger at those who are contributing the most to climate change; or a feeling of betrayal because those who are supposed to be responsible for assuring people’s well-being (e.g., parents, governments) are failing to act. Based on a review of the literature, Pihkala (Reference Pihkala2022) describes a taxonomy of climate emotions, with the most commonly discussed including anxiety, grief, guilt or shame, hope, and anger or frustration. The term “climate distress” may be used to recognize this range of emotions that one may feel in relation to climate change.

Emotional responses are complex, and most people may feel more than one of these emotional responses to climate change, either sequentially or simultaneously. Indeed, Galway and Beery (Reference Galway and Beery2022) found that hope was positively correlated with most of the negative emotions (e.g., worry, fear, sadness, anxiety). One implication is that allowing people the opportunity to describe and discuss their own negative emotions will not prohibit their ability to also feel positive emotions. Several studies have suggested the importance of acknowledging the negative emotional response to help people cope with those reactions (Doherty et al., Reference Doherty, Lykins, Piotrowski, Rogers, Sebree, White and Asmundson2022; Ojala, Reference Ojala2015). For young people, it may therefore be important for educational contexts, in which information about climate change is presented, to also allow discussion and validation of emotional responses (Baker et al., Reference Baker, Clayton and Bragg2021; Ojala, Reference Ojala2015).

In studying and describing these emotions, we are concerned about their implications for well-being and for behavior. Is climate anxiety a threat to mental health? Alternatively, if climate anxiety motivates sustainable behavior, could it actually increase well-being? Research shows that emotional responses to climate change, and negative emotions in particular, are important predictors of behavior (Brosch, Reference Brosch2021). However, negative emotions about climate change are also related to insomnia and poorer (self-reported) mental health (Ogunbode et al., Reference Ogunbode, Pallesen, Böhm, Doran, Bhullar, Aquino and Lomas2021; Schwartz et al., Reference Schwartz, Benoit, Clayton, Parnes, Swenson and Lowe2022).

There is no threshold score on the CCAS above which people are considered to have problematic “climate anxiety,” and it is important to acknowledge that climate-related distress is not a diagnosable disorder according to diagnostic classification systems. The significance of a particular emotional response depends on the individual’s social context, both because the acceptability of expressing specific emotions will vary across cultures, and because the social support received will help to determine the impact of feeling a specific emotional reaction. Iniguez-Gallardo et al. (Reference Iniguez-Gallardo, Lenti Boero and Tzanopoulos2021) remind us that the emotional response to climate change will be affected by cultural factors. In their Ecuadorian sample, powerlessness was a stronger emotional theme than it has been found to be in US samples, presumably because Ecuador has less impact on global policies and global carbon emissions. Powerlessness may also be a particularly significant emotion for some younger people, since most of them are not yet in positions of financial, social, or political influence.

The media frequently comment on declining mental health among young people. Yet media outlets also often describe young people as representing hope for the future because of their willingness to engage in climate change activism. It is important to attend to the levels and correlates of climate anxiety and distress among youth, both for their own health and for them to have, in turn, the resilience to attend to the health of the planet.

Climate Distress in Young People

As climate change disrupts the environment, children are being forced to grow up in an increasingly dangerous world.

(UNICEF 2021)

Young people face a future burdened by climate change, and the negative implications are increasingly coming into focus (Sanson et al., Reference Sanson, Van Hoorn and Burke2019). Physically, young people are more susceptible to health risks related to air pollution, heat, infectious diseases, as well as water and food shortages (Akachi et al., Reference Akachi, Goodman and Parker2009; Helldén et al., Reference Helldén, Andersson, Nilsson, Ebi, Friberg and Alfvén2021). As extreme weather-related disasters increase in severity and frequency, young people are most at risk of injury and death (Bartlett, Reference Bartlett2008). Climate change may even pose risks during prenatal development; exposure to increased humidity and natural disasters in utero are associated with preterm birth, prenatal complications, and adverse long-term mental health outcomes (Davenport et al., Reference Davenport, Grace, Funk and Shukla2017; Rothschild & Haase, Reference Rothschild and Haase2022; Subramaniam, Reference Subramaniam2007). As climate change evolves, young people may progressively experience climate-related stressors that can have a profound impact on mental health (e.g., disrupted education, forced displacement, trauma).

Young people are vulnerable not just because of physiological differences but because of their social position in society: less independence means they can experience more indirect and vicarious impacts, for example based on the ways in which parents, caregivers, or teachers are affected by climate change. Their stage of psychological development is also relevant as they may experience a strong emotional reaction to perceived betrayal or neglect of responsibility by those in charge, including governmental authorities, or to the intergenerational inequities that climate change presents (Thiery et al., Reference Thiery, Lange, Rogelj, Schleussner, Gudmundsson, Seneviratne and Wada2021). For some young people, climate change may disrupt important connections to culture and place.

Youth is a critical life phase spanning from late childhood through to young adulthood, which is typically understood as between the ages of 12 and 25 years. The United Nations defines “youth” as the “transition from the dependence of childhood to adulthood’s independence.” This phase is marked by rapid changes and maturation across social, emotional, psychological, and physical development; it is a time of growing independence, identity, and responsibility. These transitions can be difficult for young people and the presence of adverse events can greatly impact a young person’s long-term well-being (Hardgrove et al., Reference Hardgrove, Pells, Boyden and Dornan2014). For current and future younger generations, this period in life is when most may come to learn about climate change and realize the gravity of the situation. When we use terms such as “young people” we are referring to studies that investigate people between the ages of 12 and 25 years. However, even children as young as 8 years old are reporting worries about climate change, with some describing associated bad dreams or impacts on sleeping and eating habits (Atherton, Reference Atherton2020).

I guess for me it [climate change] became real during the Black Summer bushfires. It was like an apocalypse. I remember seeing all these pictures where the sky was red – the air was thick and smoky. There were all these posts on my feed about how many animals were lost in the fires and how much vegetation was being destroyed. I remember my friends saying, “this is climate change”. Sometimes it feels like we’re trying to beat something that feels impossible. It’s suffocating.

(20-year-old young person, anonymous personal communication, 2022)

Prevalence

There is a plethora of ways in which young people encounter climate change. Some may be spatially and temporally exposed to climate-induced stressors such as extreme weather disasters, erosion, drought, and extreme heat; others may learn of climate change through the media, at school, and through their parents or friends. Irrespective of how a young person is connected to climate change, evidence suggests that a high proportion of young people are concerned about the climate crisis. In their global study, Hickman and colleagues (2021) found that the majority of respondents in each and every country studied reported being moderately to extremely worried.

Research has generally shown that young people report greater concern about and belief in climate change, compared to adults (Clayton & Karazsia, Reference Clayton and Karazsia2020; Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Palm and Feng2019). Young people are also more likely to report worry, anger, and guilt, as well as less hope about climate change, than older generations (Swim et al., Reference Swim, Aviste, Lengieza and Fasano2022). This appears to be especially prominent in countries from the Global South, where concern, belief, and willingness to act are generally found to be greater than in countries such as Australia, the United States, and the United Kingdom (Hickman et al., Reference Hickman, Marks, Pihkala, Clayton, Lewandowski, Mayall and van Susteren2021; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Gjersoe, O’Neill and Barnett2020).

What Does Climate Distress Look Like in Children and Youth?

As with adults, climate distress in young people comprises emotional, cognitive, and behavioral elements. Emotions towards climate change can range in salience, severity, and valence (i.e., pleasant to unpleasant). Young people may respond to or manage these emotions through active (behavior) or intrapersonal (cognitive) means. Some young people may think and act in positive, constructive, and helpful ways; others may be unable to act or think in helpful ways or may use maladaptive coping mechanisms. How these aspects of climate distress manifest will likely differ according to a young person’s internal (e.g., one’s sense of connection to the environment) and external (e.g., experience of climate change) circumstances (Crandon et al., Reference Crandon, Scott, Charlson and Thomas2022), as well as personal resilience and psychological style. While experiences of climate distress are unique for each young person, it is useful to consider patterns or themes in the range of emotions, cognitions, and behaviors of young people.

Emotions

I feel angry that we have to deal with this. I also feel a lot of fear – I don’t know how much time we have left to do something and that really scares me. Sometimes I feel hypocritical too, because I haven’t always thrown the right thing in the right bin, but I’m also really worried.

(13-year-old young person, anonymous, personal communication, 2022)

Climate distress may involve an array of complex, distressing, and unpleasant emotional experiences, including fear, anger, shame, guilt, and helplessness. In the global survey conducted by Hickman and colleagues (2021), young people reported many negative emotions, but rarely indifference (29 percent). Perhaps the aspect most investigated in young people is the experience of anxiety and concern about climate change, often described with the term climate anxiety. As described above, anxiety can serve an adaptive biological and evolutionary function, prompting one to prepare for anticipated threats, or it can be paralyzing. For young people, a future laden with the uncertainty and threat of climate change is likely to evoke anxiety and fear.

Studies have shown that young people also express confusion, betrayal, and a sense of abandonment in response to adult inaction. Young people, who are most likely to be affected by climate change, generally have less agency and power to create meaningful change compared to adults. For some young people, as they develop increasing independence, choice and responsibility, this may highlight inaction by their peers, family, or community members. Perceiving older generations as failing to protect younger generations or to prepare them for impending harm may evoke generational anger and resentment (Hickman et al., Reference Hickman, Marks, Pihkala, Clayton, Lewandowski, Mayall and van Susteren2021). This distress may be furthered when young people feel silenced, which may be the case when young people are excluded from decision making (e.g., voting) or are criticized by others for demanding action (Crandon et al., Reference Crandon, Scott, Charlson and Thomas2022). For example, some young people may feel dismissed when characterized by government officials or the media as experiencing “needless anxiety” (Crowe, Reference Crowe2019).

Climate change, and the way one responds to it, can also evoke pleasant emotions such as hope, optimism, connection to others, motivation, a “warm glow” (a feeling one gets when acting in an environmentally friendly way), and “soliphilia” (a feeling of protectiveness, love, and responsibility towards the planet) (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Zaval and Markowitz2021). For young people, these feelings may be evoked when involved in environmental projects (e.g., at home, at school, or in the community), when connecting with their peers on climate change issues (e.g., school projects or climate action marches), or when spending time in nature. To date, there is little evidence on the extent to which young people experience pleasant climate emotions, although some young people have reported feeling optimistic and hopeful (Hickman et al., Reference Hickman, Marks, Pihkala, Clayton, Lewandowski, Mayall and van Susteren2021; Ojala, Reference Ojala2012). Emotions of a pleasant nature may be more likely to motivate people to engage in pro-environmental behavior. Young people who are more hopeful about climate change, for example, may be more likely to report a willingness or greater engagement with pro-environmental action (Ojala, Reference Ojala2011; Stevenson & Peterson, Reference Stevenson and Peterson2016). Another study found that eliciting anticipated pride (e.g., imagining the feelings of pride that may follow engaging in pro-environmental behavior) may lead to higher motivation to act compared to guilt (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Zaval, Weber and Markowitz2017).

Cognitions

Every time I see something about climate change, I just think we’re too late to fix it. I think about the ice caps melting. I think that it’s gonna keep getting hotter. I think resources will keep getting smaller and everyone will be fighting for one thing. Things will become more in-demand – things that we need on a daily basis.

I think about having kids and I’m worried about their future, what it’s gonna be like for them.

(21-year-old young person, Aquino, J., personal communication, 2022)

Evidence suggests that young people may have both positive (e.g., trusting scientists to come up with solutions) and negative (e.g., perceiving humanity as doomed) thoughts about climate change. Whether a young person’s thoughts are positive or negative is likely to be driven by their understanding of the situation. Hickman and colleagues (2021), for example, showed that young people reported having more negative thoughts if they also reported feeling betrayed by their governmental response to climate change.

Young people’s thoughts can also take the form of cognitive strategies to cope with or influence their feelings about climate change. For example, meaning-based coping involves positively reappraising a threat or finding benefits within adverse events, such as about how others are actively working together to address climate change. Meaning-focused coping has been linked to positive affect (e.g., life satisfaction, optimism) and less negative affect in young people (Ojala, Reference Ojala2012). Other young people may use problem-solving coping, which involves identifying and planning to implement actionable solutions. Both meaning-focused coping and problem-solving coping have been associated with greater environmental engagement in young people (Ojala, Reference Ojala2012).

Another cognitive strategy identified in youth is rumination, a passive and repetitive negative focus on one’s distress, its causes, and consequences. For example, an individual may have repetitive negative thoughts about their own emotional distress, or how their behavior might contribute to climate change (Clayton & Karazsia, Reference Clayton and Karazsia2020; Hogg et al., Reference Hogg, Stanley, O’Brien, Wilson and Watsford2021). It has been suggested that rumination may be used when attempting to counterintuitively control or avoid the threat (Ojala, Reference Ojala2012; Stroebe et al., Reference Stroebe, Boelen, van den Hout, Stroebe, Salemink and van den Bout2007).

Positive or negative thoughts and cognitive strategies do not inherently indicate how adaptive a young person’s response to climate change may be. Rather, it could be argued that a young person’s adaptation is better reflected by whether their emotions or thoughts lead to meaningful behavioral coping.

Behaviors

In every choice I make, I see the reaction. Energy consumption, food consumption, transport emissions, waste. I see how I contribute to a growing climate crisis. Though, in these everyday actions I also see how awareness has changed my behaviour. Reducing my plastic waste, composting scraps, eating less meat, catching public transport.

I think for previous generations the threat of “climate change” was easy to ignore, something that’s “too far off”. For me, it is tangible and too real to ignore.

(24-year-old young person, Lindsay, A., personal communication, 2022)

Climate distress may lead to changes in behavior, which may be considered helpful or unhelpful for the individual as well as for climate mitigation efforts. Studies have shown that young people who are distressed, anxious, or concerned about climate change may be more engaged or willing to engage in pro-environmental behavior. One longitudinal study also found that sustained climate worry over time was linked to greater engagement with news and politics (Sciberras & Fernando, Reference Sciberras and Fernando2021). Yet climate change can also lead to functional impairment in young people. More specifically, climate distress in youth has been associated with difficulties engaging in school or work, as well as spending time with family and friends (Clayton & Karazsia, Reference Clayton and Karazsia2020; Hickman et al., Reference Hickman, Marks, Pihkala, Clayton, Lewandowski, Mayall and van Susteren2021). Climate distress can also impact the life choices and long-term behaviors of young people; some young people have reported hesitancy to have children or to travel due to their feelings about climate change. A survey of Americans aged 13–29 in 2021 found that they expected climate change to affect decisions about where to live, what to buy, what to eat, and whether to have children, and only 15 percent said it would not influence their future decisions (Isaacs-Thomas, Reference Isaacs-Thomas2021).

Moving from Climate Distress to Posttraumatic Growth and Resilience

Young people have the capacity to be leaders in sustainability, influence policy decision-making, communicate about climate change to the public, as well as direct and contribute to efforts that mitigate climate change (Haynes & Tanner, Reference Haynes and Tanner2015). However, there is a risk of assigning young people an unfair proportion of responsibility for addressing the crisis. There is the potential for young people to become overwhelmed by continuous climate-related triggers over time (e.g., weather disasters, conflict between nations, governmental inaction) (Crandon et al., Reference Crandon, Scott, Charlson and Thomas2022). Given the ongoing risks that climate change poses for young people, there is an urgent need to support youth to cope with climate change in ways that are protective of their mental health. One way to do this may be in strengthening individual and community psychological resilience.

Resilience has been described as “bouncing back” from adversity; that is, one’s capacity to return to psychological equilibrium and functioning following stressful events. Much of the research regarding resilience and climate-related stressors focuses on natural disasters. Following disasters, young people may be more likely than adults to experience high levels of mental health problems such as stress, anxiety, and depression, and less likely to exhibit resilience, when compared to adults (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Bagrodia, Pfeffer, Meli and Bonanno2020). Although many young people who experience post-disaster distress recover or develop resilience in the year following the disaster (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Bagrodia, Pfeffer, Meli and Bonanno2020), it is possible that climate distress could become chronic as young people face gradual and long-term climate-related impacts (e.g., changing environments and biodiversity loss over time), as well as stressors that will become more frequent and intense as climate change evolves. Trajectories of resilience versus psychopathology following climate adversities will also be influenced by contextual and systemic factors. For example, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms in children and young people following bushfire/wildfire disasters may be greater when young people perceive their own life is under threat, or when they are experiencing ongoing loss and disruption (McDermott et al., Reference McDermott, Lee, Judd and Gibbon2005; Yelland et al., Reference Yelland, Robinson, Lock, La Greca, Kokegei, Ridgway and Lai2010). As another example, young people living in an underresourced or historically disinvested community may also experience different and more intense psychological consequences.

One of the ways in which climate anxiety and distress manifest among young people may be through their decisions about the future. Attempts to promote resilience among youth may need to focus on providing new skills for making decisions under conditions of increasing uncertainty.

Limited empirical evidence explores the effectiveness of interventions that aim to strengthen resilience in children and young people experiencing climate distress. Despite this, there is an established evidence base for programs and interventions that aim to foster potential drivers of resilience, such as the ability to manage emotional distress through meditation, mindfulness, and other techniques, increased engagement in climate advocacy and mitigation, as well as building feelings of hope, agency, and pro-environmental behavior in young people (Haynes & Tanner, Reference Haynes and Tanner2015).

Importantly, resilience focuses on recovery and maintaining coping long term. This focus may neglect the critical need to challenge and change social-ecological, economic, and political systems to adequately address climate change (Adams et al., Reference Adams, Blackburn and Mantovani2021; Bahadur & Tanner, Reference Bahadur and Tanner2014). Indeed, some research has shown that psychological resilience to extreme weather events may be associated with a diminished motivation for climate change mitigation (Ogunbode et al., Reference Ogunbode, Böhm, Capstick, Demski, Spence and Tausch2019). Several authors have thus suggested a focus on transformational resilience (Doppelt, Reference Doppelt2016). This strategy is based on the concept of posttraumatic growth, in which difficult events provide an opportunity for change and realignment of priorities towards greater social connection and spiritual growth. Within the posttraumatic growth model, climate stressors can be catalysts to improve outcomes for the environment, the climate, and human and social well-being (Doppelt, Reference Doppelt2016). For young people, this would mean holding uncertainty and despair, whilst also looking to reimagining and building a different future.

A comprehensive view of resilience should acknowledge the interdependent links between human and ecosystem health, so that a shortsighted attempt to emphasize individual mental health does not take priority over working toward a mutually sustainable and healthy environment (Bingley et al., Reference Bingley, Tran, Boyd, Gibson, Kalokerinos, Koval and Greenaway2022) and instead is based on the recognition that a healthy environment is fundamental to mental health. The goal should not be to eliminate negative emotions in response to climate change but to encourage a healthy understanding of and reaction to those emotions.

Future Directions

Research provides increasing evidence that young people are distressed about climate change; however, there remain wide and far-reaching gaps that research, community-based interventions and public health policy must promptly address to support the coping and mental health of different groups of young people (as discussed by Wortzel in Chapter 9). The various threats surrounding diverse groups of young people (e.g., from different geographical locations, cultures, socioeconomic backgrounds) lead to different threats to mental health as well. The subsequent chapters of this book will use a range of perspectives to bring these complex causal connections into deeper focus.

Vulnerability to climate change, including its emotional impacts, is not evenly distributed (Dooley et al., Reference Dooley, Sheats, Hamilton, Chapman and Karlin2021). There are inequities in exposure to the effects of climate change, in the degree of impact, and in the sources of resilience that are available. Poorer people and countries, for example, are often situated in areas that are more strongly affected by climate change. Within these areas, young people often have fewer resources to protect themselves from those impacts and to recover from them (Hanna & Oliva, Reference Hanna and Oliva2016; Sanson et al., Reference Sanson, Van Hoorn and Burke2019). Young people living in rural areas may also be more strongly affected by climatic changes that threaten their community’s predominant source of food, water, and employment. Indigenous peoples may find that their entire culture and traditional practices continue to be threatened in a way that is less true for other individuals. Indeed, the spiritual and cultural significance of the land for some Indigenous young people is such that its decay may have a profound impact on their psychological health and identity.

Beyond the research community, it is especially important for the adults surrounding young people (e.g., families, educators, and mental health professionals) and particularly those in positions of power (e.g., those in government, policy, the fossil fuel industry) to consider the impacts of climate change on young people and the distress it may evoke. In recognizing and responding to these impacts, it is critical to work alongside and uplift young people as active agents of change and key stakeholders in planetary health, their own physical and mental well-being, as well as the future.

References

Adams, H., Blackburn, S., & Mantovani, N. (2021). Psychological resilience for climate change transformation: Relational, differentiated and situated perspectives. Current Opinion in Environmental Sustainability, 50, 303309. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cosust.2021.06.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Akachi, Y., Goodman, D., & Parker, D. (2009). Global climate change and child health: A review of pathways, impacts and measures to improve the evidence base. Innocenti Discussion Papers. https://doi.org/10.18356/32199510-enCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atherton, R. (2020). Climate anxiety: Survey for BBC Newsround shows children losing sleep over climate change and the environment. www.bbc.co.uk/newsround/51451737Google Scholar
Bahadur, A., & Tanner, T. (2014). Transformational resilience thinking: Putting people, power and politics at the heart of urban climate resilience. Environment and Urbanization, 26(1), 200214. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956247814522154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, C., Clayton, S., & Bragg, E. (2021). Educating for resilience: Parent and teacher perceptions of children’s emotional needs in response to climate change. Environmental Education Research, 27(5), 687705. https://doi.org/10.1080/13504622.2020.1828288CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barlow, D. H. (2000). Unraveling the mysteries of anxiety and its disorders from the perspective of emotion theory. American Psychologist, 55(11), 12471263. https://doi.org/10.1037//0003-066x.55.11.1247CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartlett, S. (2008). The implications of climate change for children in lower-income countries. Children, Youth and Environments, 18(1), 7198. www.jstor.org/stable/10.7721/chilyoutenvi.18.1.0071CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bingley, W. J., Tran, A., Boyd, C. P., Gibson, K., Kalokerinos, E. K., Koval, P., … Greenaway, K. H. (2022). A multiple needs framework for climate change anxiety interventions. American Psychologist, 77(7), 812. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brosch, T. (2021). Affect and emotions as drivers of climate change perception and action: A review. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 42, 1521. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.02.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, S., Bagrodia, R., Pfeffer, C. C., Meli, L., & Bonanno, G. A. (2020). Anxiety and resilience in the face of natural disasters associated with climate change: A review and methodological critique. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 76, 102297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102297CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clayton, S., & Karazsia, B. T. (2020). Development and validation of a measure of climate change anxiety. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 69, 101434. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101434CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crandon, T. J., Scott, J. G., Charlson, F. J., & Thomas, H. J. (2022). A social-ecological perspective of climate anxiety in children and adolescents. Nature Climate Change, 12, 123131. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41558-021-01251-yCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowe, D. (2019). Morrison warns against “needless anxiety” after Thunberg climate speech. The Sydney Morning Herald, September 25.Google Scholar
Cunsolo, A., & Ellis, N. R. (2018). Ecological grief as a mental health response to climate change-related loss. Nature Climate Change, 8(4), 275281. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41558-018-0092-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davenport, F., Grace, K., Funk, C., & Shukla, S. (2017). Child health outcomes in sub-Saharan Africa: A comparison of changes in climate and socio-economic factors. Global Environmental Change, 46, 7287. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2017.04.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doherty, T. J., Lykins, A. D., Piotrowski, N. A., Rogers, Z., SebreeJr, D. D., & White, K. E. (2022). 11.12 – Clinical psychology responses to the climate crisis. In Asmundson, G. J. G. (Ed.), Comprehensive clinical psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 11, pp. 167183). Elsevier. https://doi.org/doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-818697-8.00236-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dooley, L., Sheats, J., Hamilton, O., Chapman, D., & Karlin, B. (2021). Climate change and youth mental health: Psychological impacts, resilience resources, and future directions. See Change Institute.Google Scholar
Doppelt, B. (2016). Transformational resilience: How building human resilience to climate disruption can safeguard society and increase wellbeing. Routledge.Google Scholar
Galway, L. P., & Beery, T. (2022). Exploring climate emotions in Canada’s provincial north. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 920313. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.920313CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanna, R., & Oliva, P. (2016). Implications of climate change for children in developing countries. The Future of Children, 26(1), 115132. www.jstor.org/stable/43755233CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardgrove, A., Pells, K., Boyden, J., & Dornan, P. (2014). Youth vulnerabilities in life course transitions. UNDP (United Nations Development Programme).Google Scholar
Haynes, K., & Tanner, T. M. (2015). Empowering young people and strengthening resilience: Youth-centred participatory video as a tool for climate change adaptation and disaster risk reduction. Children’s Geographies, 13(3), 357371. https://doi.org/10.1080/14733285.2013.848599CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heeren, A., Mouguiama-Daouda, C., & Contreras, A. (2022). On climate anxiety and the threat it may pose to daily life functioning and adaptation: A study among European and African French-speaking participants. Climatic Change, 173(1), 15. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-022-03402-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helldén, D., Andersson, C., Nilsson, M., Ebi, K. L., Friberg, P., & Alfvén, T. (2021). Climate change and child health: A scoping review and an expanded conceptual framework. The Lancet Planetary Health, 5(3), e164–e175. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2542-5196(20)30274-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickman, C. (2020). We need to (find a way to) talk about … eco-anxiety. Journal of Social Work Practice, 34(4), 411424. https://doi.org/10.1080/02650533.2020.1844166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickman, C., Marks, E., Pihkala, P., Clayton, S., Lewandowski, R. E., Mayall, E. E., … van Susteren, L. (2021). Climate anxiety in children and young people and their beliefs about government responses to climate change: A global survey. The Lancet Planetary Health, 5(12), E863E873. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2542-5196(21)00278-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogg, T., Stanley, S., O’Brien, L., Wilson, M., & Watsford, C. (2021). The Hogg eco-anxiety scale: Development and validation of a multidimensional scale. Global Environmental Change, 71, 102391. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2021.102391CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iniguez-Gallardo, V., Lenti Boero, D., & Tzanopoulos, J. (2021). Climate change and emotions: Analysis of people’s emotional states in southern Ecuador [original research]. Frontiers in Psychology, 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.644240CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
IPCC (2022). Climate change 2022: Impacts, adaptation, and vulnerability. Contribution of Working Group II to the Sixth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change [Pörtner, H.-O., Roberts, D. C., Tignor, M., Poloczanska, E. S., Mintenbeck, K., Alegría, A., Craig, M., Langsdorf, S., Löschke, S., Möller, V., Okem, A., & Rama, B. (eds.)]. Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/9781009325844.Google Scholar
Isaacs-Thomas, I. (2021). How young people feel about climate change and their future. PBS News Hour, November 5. www.pbs.org/newshour/science/young-people-are-optimistic-that-theres-time-to-prevent-the-worst-effects-of-climate-changeGoogle Scholar
Larionow, P., Sołtys, M., Izdebski, P., Mudło-Głagolska, K., Golonka, J., Demski, M., & Rosińska, M. (2022). Climate change anxiety assessment: The psychometric properties of the Polish version of the Climate Anxiety Scale. Frontiers in Psychology, 13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.870392CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. Springer.Google Scholar
Lee, K., Gjersoe, N., O’Neill, S., & Barnett, J. (2020). Youth perceptions of climate change: A narrative synthesis. WIREs Climate Change, 11(3), e641. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcc.641CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leiserowitz, A., Carman, J., Buttermore, N., Neyens, L., Rosenthal, S., Marlon, J., Schneider, J., & Mulcahy, K. (2022). International public opinion on climate change, 2022. Yale Program on Climate Change Communication and Data for Good at Meta.Google Scholar
Lewis, G. B., Palm, R., & Feng, B. (2019). Cross-national variation in determinants of climate change concern. Environmental Politics, 28(5), 793821. https://doi.org/10.1080/09644016.2018.1512261CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDermott, B. M., Lee, E. M., Judd, M., & Gibbon, P. (2005). Posttraumatic stress disorder and general psychopathology in children and adolescents following a wildfire disaster. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry/La Revue canadienne de psychiatrie, 50(3), 137143. https://doi.org/10.1177/070674370505000302CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mouguiama-Daouda, C., Blanchard, M. A., Coussement, C., & Heeren, A. (2022). On the measurement of climate change anxiety: French validation of the Climate Anxiety Scale. Psychologica Belgica, 62(1), 123135. https://doi.org/10.5334/pb.1137CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ogunbode, C. A., Böhm, G., Capstick, S. B., Demski, C., Spence, A., & Tausch, N. (2019). The resilience paradox: Flooding experience, coping and climate change mitigation intentions. Climate Policy, 19(6), 703715. https://doi.org/10.1080/14693062.2018.1560242CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ogunbode, C. A., Pallesen, S., Böhm, G., Doran, R., Bhullar, N., Aquino, S., … Lomas, M. J. (2021). Negative emotions about climate change are related to insomnia symptoms and mental health: Cross-sectional evidence from 25 countries. Current Psychology, 42, 845854. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01385-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ojala, M. (2011). Hope and climate change: The importance of hope for environmental engagement among young people. Environmental Education Research, 18(5), 625642. https://doi.org/10.1080/13504622.2011.637157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ojala, M. (2012). Regulating worry, promoting hope: How do children, adolescents, and young adults cope with climate change? International Journal of Environmental and Science Education, 7(4), 537561. https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ997146.pdfGoogle Scholar
Ojala, M. (2015). Hope in the face of climate change: associations with environmental engagement and student perceptions of teachers’ emotion communication style and future orientation. Journal of Environmental Education, 46(3), 133148. https://doi.org/10.1080/00958964.2015.1021662CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ojala, M., Cunsolo, A., Ogunbode, C. A., & Middleton, J. (2021). Anxiety, worry, and grief in a time of environmental and climate crisis: A narrative review. Annual Review of Environment and Resources, 46(1), 3558. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-environ-012220-022716CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pihkala, P. (2022). Toward a taxonomy of climate emotions. Frontiers in Climate, 3. https://doi.org/10.3389/fclim.2021.738154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reyes, M. E. S., Carmen, B. P. B., Luminarias, M. E. P., Mangulabnan, S. A. N. B., & Ogunbode, C. A. (2021). An investigation into the relationship between climate change anxiety and mental health among Gen Z Filipinos. Current Psychology, 42, 74487456. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02099-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothschild, J., & Haase, E. (2022). The mental health of women and climate change: Direct neuropsychiatric impacts and associated psychological concerns. International Journal of Gynecology & Obstetrics, 160(2), 405413. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijgo.14479CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanson, A. V., Van Hoorn, J., & Burke, S. E. L. (2019). Responding to the impacts of the climate crisis on children and youth. Child Development Perspectives, 13(4), 201207. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, C. R., Zaval, L., & Markowitz, E. M. (2021). Positive emotions and climate change. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 42, 114120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.04.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, C. R., Zaval, L., Weber, E. U., & Markowitz, E. M. (2017). The influence of anticipated pride and guilt on pro-environmental decision making. PLoS One, 12(11), e0188781. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188781CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, S. E. O., Benoit, L., Clayton, S., Parnes, M. F., Swenson, L., & Lowe, S. R. (2022). Climate change anxiety and mental health: Environmental activism as buffer. Current Psychology, 42, 16708–16721. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-02735-6Google Scholar
Sciberras, E., & Fernando, J. W. (2021). Climate change-related worry among Australian adolescents: An eight-year longitudinal study. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 27(1), 2229. https://doi.org/10.1111/camh.12521CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, P. D., Pakingan, K. A., & Aruta, J. J. B. R. (2022). Measurement of climate change anxiety and its mediating effect between experience of climate change and mitigation actions of Filipino youth. Educational and Developmental Psychologist, 39(1), 1727. https://doi.org/10.1080/20590776.2022.2037390CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevenson, K., & Peterson, N. (2016). Motivating action through fostering climate change hope and concern and avoiding despair among adolescents. Sustainability, 8(1), 6. www.mdpi.com/2071-1050/8/1/6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stroebe, M., Boelen, P. A., van den Hout, M., Stroebe, W., Salemink, E., & van den Bout, J. (2007). Ruminative coping as avoidance. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 257(8), 462472. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-007-0746-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Subramaniam, V. (2007). Seasonal variation in the incidence of preeclampsia and eclampsia in tropical climatic conditions. BMC Women’s Health, 7(1), 18. https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6874-7-18CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swim, J. K., Aviste, R., Lengieza, M. L., & Fasano, C. J. (2022). OK Boomer: A decade of generational differences in feelings about climate change. Global Environmental Change, 73, 102479. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2022.102479CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tam, K.-P., Chan, H.-W., & Clayton, S. (2023). Climate change anxiety in China, India, Japan, and the United States. Journal of Environmental Psychology, in press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thiery, W., Lange, S., Rogelj, J., Schleussner, C.-F., Gudmundsson, L., Seneviratne, S. I., … Wada, Y. (2021). Intergenerational inequities in exposure to climate extremes. Science, 374(6564), 158160. https://doi.org/doi:10.1126/science.abi7339CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
UNICEF (2021, 19 August). The impacts of climate change put almost every child at risk. www.unicef.org/stories/impacts-climate-change-put-almost-every-child-riskGoogle Scholar
Wang, S., Leviston, Z., Hurlstone, M., Lawrence, C., & Walker, I. (2018). Emotions predict policy support: Why it matters how people feel about climate change. Global Environmental Change, 50, 2540. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2018.03.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wullenkord, M., Toger, J., Hamann, K. R., Loy, L., & Reese, G. (2021). Anxiety and climate change: A validation of the Climate Anxiety Scale in a German-speaking quota sample and an investigation of psychological correlates. Climatic Change, 168(3), 123. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/76ez2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yelland, C., Robinson, P., Lock, C., La Greca, A. M., Kokegei, B., Ridgway, V., & Lai, B. (2010). Bushfire impact on youth. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 23(2), 274277. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.20521CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×