Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T01:32:15.443Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - How Human Hunters Hunt

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 August 2022

Julia E. Fa
Affiliation:
Manchester Metropolitan University and Center for International Forestry (CIFOR), Indonesia
Stephan M. Funk
Affiliation:
Nature Heritage
Robert Nasi
Affiliation:
Centre for International Forestry Research (CIFOR), Indonesia

Summary

We start the chapter by describing the different modes of hunting and clarify that in this book we primarily focus on subsistence hunting and commercial hunting; the latter provides wild meat for local rural and urban markets. The main objective of subsistence hunting is to provide food for the hunters and their families. This activity plays a vital role in the sustenance and even survival of many peoples in the tropics and subtropical regions of the world. We start the chapter by describing the hunting technology used by humans throughout time and how this has impacted human evolution. We then concentrate on describing modern hunting techniques used from an extensive analysis of published studies of hunter-gatherers and rural communities in the tropics and subtropics. Hunters are described, including hunting by children and the cultural aspects involved in hunting. We end the chapter by entering into the debate of whether rural or Indigenous Peoples manage their resources sustainably as a prelude to the next chapter.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This content is Open Access and distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 https://creativecommons.org/cclicenses/

3.1 Introduction

Hunting of wild animals, for meat and other body parts (e.g., pelts, horns, antlers) has been part of the human story for millennia. In this book, we focus only on the hunting of wild animals by humans for food. In this chapter we describe how humans hunt with a focus on technology and on the cultural and anthropological aspects of hunting; the topic of optimal hunting is addressed in Chapter 4. In broad terms, hunting can be subsistence, commercial, or recreational (Fig. 3.1, Ojasti Reference Ojasti1996). We do not address recreational hunting, which refers to activities in which the main objective is the personal enjoyment of the hunter, rather than food or profit (e.g. trophy lion hunting, Whitman et al. Reference Whitman, Starfield, Quadling and Packer2004). Recreational hunting may have roots in traditional subsistence hunting or commercial hunting activities (McCorquodale Reference McCorquodale1997). We also do not address the removal of predators that can be dangerous to people or domestic animals, or the removal of pests that destroy crops or kill livestock, except when these animals are being used as wild meat.

Figure 3.1 Comparison of main attributes of subsistence, commercial and recreational hunting.

(modified from Ojasti Reference Ojasti1996)

Only in subsistence hunting is the sole purpose to provide food for the hunters and their families and hence it plays a vital role in their sustenance and even survival (Peres Reference Peres2000). By contrast, commercial hunting takes place when natural products are exploited to be sold for profit. The consequences of unsustainable commercial harvesting of marine and terrestrial wild animals are now clearly felt throughout the globe (Di Minin et al. Reference Di Minin, Brooks and Toivonen2019). The impacts of uncontrolled commercial hunting on wildlife in the tropics and subtropics are also significant; these topics are discussed more in depth in Chapters 2 and 6 of this book.

In some countries, subsistence hunting is defined by law as just hunting for personal consumption, and it is often considered illegal to sell any surplus. Conversely, the sale of some of the animals hunted but not eaten by the hunters or their families, sometimes the most valuable species, can be an important source of income (Alexander et al. Reference Alexander, McNamara, Rowcliffe, Oppong and Milner-Gulland2015; Coad et al. Reference Coad, Abernethy, Balmford, Manica, Airey and Milner-Gulland2010; Schulte-Herbrüggen et al. Reference Abernethy, Coad, Taylor, Lee and Maisels2013). However, Van Vliet et al. (Reference Van Vliet2019) argue strongly that formal regulations in many tropical countries are ill adapted to the reality in which rural and Indigenous Peoples live and that reforms which clarify the rights to sell surplus of meat and align land tenure rights with wildlife use rights are imminently needed. Many rural families sell surplus wild meat (see e.g. El Bizri et al. Reference El Bizri, Morcatty and Ferreira2020b), and this can provide a very important source of income; if the right regulations are in place, surplus sale of wild meat should be permitted in rural settings (see also Chapter 8). Frequency of wild meat consumption and sale of wild meat are positively associated with proximity to markets, especially in urban centres (e.g. Sierra et al. Reference Sierra, Rodriguez and Losos1999).

3.2 Hunting Technology

Since at least the past 200,000 years, when modern humans evolved, subsistence hunting for protein acquisition has dominated (Stanford & Bunn Reference Hawkes, Stanford and Bunn2001). In fact, humans have spent more time as hunter-gatherers than as agriculturalists, industrialists or post-industrialists. This not only emphasizes the importance of hunting animals for our survival over many millennia but also highlights its role in the emergence and evolution of individual and social behaviour in our species. Because human beings are ill-adapted predators, lacking fangs, claws or high speed, hunting technology has filled this gap. Tools for hunting allow humans to expand the range of prey captured, reduce pursuit times and extend diet breadth by the use of methods for killing at a distance (spears, nets etc.), passive forms of animal capture (traps) and the use of methods to lure prey (decoys). In particular, projectile weaponry (such as the bow and arrow, and spearthrower and dart) has been a key strategic innovation that has aided ecological niche broadening and has allowed the dispersal of humans throughout the world (Shea Reference Shea2006; Shea & Sisk Reference Shea and Sisk2010).

Numerous depictions of animals in prehistoric cave art clearly manifest the importance of hunting for human beings. Prehistoric cave art provides the most direct insight that we have into the earliest storytelling (Mithen Reference Mithen1989). One of the most primitive is the image portraying several figures that appear to represent therianthropes (human beings who metamorphose into other animals by means of shapeshifting) hunting wild pigs and dwarf bovids, the latter probably anoa, in the Leang Bulu’ Sipong 4 cave in the limestone karsts of Maros-Pangkep, South Sulawesi, Indonesia (Fig. 3.2). The animals are being hunted by figures with animal characteristics who carry long thin objects that the authors interpret as spears and/or ropes. The interpretation of the scene is that it is a communal hunt, likely a game drive where animals of any species are driven from cover and directed towards waiting hunters. This image, created at least 43,900 years ago and described in 2019 (Aubert et al. Reference Aubert, Lebe and Oktaviana2019), has already been replaced as the oldest-known painting by the discovery of a figurative painting of a Sulawesi warty pig, dated to be 45,500 years old (Brumm et al. Reference Brumm, Oktaviana and Burhan2021).

Figure 3.2 Drawings found in Leang Bulu’Sipong 4 cave, south of Sulawesi, showing a buffalo being hunted by part-human, part-animal creatures holding spears and possibly ropes.

(from Aubert et al. Reference Aubert, Lebe and Oktaviana2019; reprinted with permission from Nature Springer and M. Aubert)

In the following sections we describe the available evidence for the different forms of weapons and techniques used in hunting animals in the past and highlight both the importance hunting has had in the sustenance of humans over millennia but also how hunting technology itself has impacted their physical, social and cognitive evolution.

3.2.1 Projectile Hunting: Changes for the Better

Like most aspects of early biological and cultural evolution (e.g. Groucutt et al. Reference Groucutt, Petraglia and Bailey2015), the origin and development of projectile technology remain poorly understood. The earliest evidence for launched weapons used in both hunting and warfare for any hominid comes from several wooden spears found in Schöningen, Germany. These spears, dated ~400,000 BP, that is before the emergence of modern humans (Thieme Reference Thieme1997), were either hand-held, short-ranged thrusting weapons (Shea Reference Shea2006; Thieme Reference Thieme1997) or throwing weapons for distances up to 20 metres (Milks et al. Reference Milks, Parker and Pope2019). Relatively large, heavy spears sometimes tipped with stone armatures were typical of Neanderthal hunting, where Neanderthal upper limb and upper body morphology has been suggested to be an adaptation to the energetic and mechanical requirements of using thrusting spears (Churchill Reference Churchill and Churchill2014). Thrusting and short-distance throwing spears preconditioned hunting strategies, and hunting with spears has often been equated with large hunter group size (Wadley Reference Wadley2010). The subsequent appearance of distance weapons is a critical development in human hunting technology. As a result, the arsenal of spears used by AMH was considerably larger, compared to Neanderthals who only used thrusting spears (Churchill Reference Churchill and Churchill2014) or short-distance throwing lances (Milks et al. Reference Milks, Parker and Pope2019). What were previously thought to be the oldest-known spears, those found in Clacton-on-Sea, UK (Oakley et al. Reference Oakley, Andrews, Keeley and Clark1977), and Lehringen, Germany (Movius Reference Movius1950), dated to the Middle Pleistocene, have been debated because associated faunal remains do not necessarily demonstrate hunting (Klein Reference Klein, Nitecki and Nitecki1987). For example, the Lehringen spear was found between the ribs of an elephant skeleton, but some authors have suggested that these sites are spring-, stream- or lakeside localities where it is difficult to separate bones that may represent natural deaths from human kills (Klein Reference Klein, Nitecki and Nitecki1987). By contrast, the association of the Schöningen spears with stone tools and butchered remains of more than ten horses confirms them as hunting weapons, suggesting that systematic hunting that employed projectile technology was already present in pre-modern hominids.

The oldest backed stone blades have been found at Twin Rivers, Zambia, in deposits of approximately 300,000 BP (Barham Reference Barham2002). Backed stone blades have one thick, blunt side suitable for hafting, with the attachment of the blades to wooden spears or arrows (Fig. 3.3). The design and preparation of backed tools, the preparation of hafts and the final hafting require problem-solving and planning that is usually associated with modern humans (Ambrose Reference Ambrose2001). Indeed, new fossil finds, identified as Homo sapiens, from Jebel Irhoud, Morocco, place backed tool preparation at the same time as the emergence of AMH in Africa. These fossils, dated 315,000 ± 34,000 BP, show a mosaic of key modern human morphological features of early or recent AMH and more primitive cranial morphology (Hublin et al. Reference Hublin, Ben-Ncer and Bailey2017). The new technology together with new behaviour, including syntactic language, possibly allowed modern humans to expand their range into previously unoccupied Congo Basin tropical forests (Barham Reference Barham, Barham and Robson-Brown2001). Backed stone blades have been confirmed in East Africa at ~130,000 BP (Mehlman Reference Mehlman1990) and in southern Africa between ~70,000 and ~60,000 BP (Wadley & Mohapi Reference Wadley and Mohapi2008). The latter paper describes Howiesons Poort Industry tools from Sibudu in South Africa, which are particularly interesting as they comprise diverse stone projectiles linked to different types of hunting tools. Tools made from dolerite were relatively large, too large for arrow heads, but suitable for darts and spearheads. Quartz tools were of a size of arrowheads, whilst hornfels tools varied in size and appear to have been reused during their lifetime for different purposes by reshaping. Trace analyses suggest that most tools were parts of hunting weapons, in particular bows and arrows (Lombard & Phillipson Reference Lombard and Phillipson2010; Wadley & Mohapi Reference Wadley and Mohapi2008).

Figure 3.3 Backed tools from Twin Rivers.

(from Barham Reference Barham2002 reprinted with permission from Elsevier)

Although lithic projectile weaponry has been confirmed across Africa before ~50,000 BP, it appears not to have been widespread or in regular use in this continent, in Europe or the Levant (Shea Reference Shea2006). However, by 45,000–40,000 BP, lithic projectiles were in use in most parts of the world occupied by humans (Knecht Reference Cattelain and Knecht1997), after dispersing out of Africa and arriving at the periphery of the Old World, Australia, Siberia and northwest Europe by 50,000–40,000 BP (Groucutt et al. Reference Groucutt, Petraglia and Bailey2015). This widespread use of distance weapons for large animal hunting and warfare is regarded as an epiphenomenon of the ‘Upper Palaeolithic’ behavioural revolution as it involves planning, social organization and the application of suitable technology. Such use of projectile weapons allowed the more accurate killing of large, potentially dangerous prey at a safe distance compared to hand-delivered thrusting methods. The contention is that these technological changes diminished the need for muscular strength, thus affording the more gracile anatomically modern humans a competitive advantage over Neanderthals (Churchill & Rhodes Reference Churchill and Rhodes2006). Moreover, the development of mechanically delivered projectile weapons, spearthrower-darts or bows and arrows, further improved hunting efficiency in modern humans. The earliest evidence of mechanically delivered projectile weapons was found at Grotta del Cavallo in southern Italy, discovered alongside signs of hunting of young horses (Sano et al. Reference Sano, Arrighi and Stani2019). These projectiles, dated to between 40,000 and 45,000 BP, correspond to the early migration period of modern humans into Eurasia (Groucutt et al. Reference Groucutt, Petraglia and Bailey2015; O’Connell et al. Reference O’Connell, Allen and Williams2018). The Grotta del Cavallo projectiles pre-date the previously known spearthrowers and arrows from France and Germany by more than 20,000 years (Cattelain Reference Cattelain and Knecht1997; Sano et al. Reference Sano, Arrighi and Stani2019).

3.2.2 Nets, Traps, Snares and Other Methods

In contrast to lithic artefacts and, in some cases, larger wooden fragments, materials such as fibres do not preserve well in the archaeological record. Evidence for hunting with nets, traps and snares is therefore rare; such information is, however, of importance for our understanding of human evolution. The operation of these techniques constitutes a significant milestone in the development of memory and cognition in planning across space and time (Wynn & Coolidge Reference Wynn and Coolidge2003). Nonetheless, indirect evidence from the Sibudu site in South Africa points to the use of snares during the Howiesons Poort Industry ~65,000–60,000 BP, possibly even ~70,000 BP (Wadley Reference Wadley2010). The high frequency and the relatively high taxonomic diversity of comparatively small mammals found at the site are consistent with modern snare hunting (Chapter 6) and not with hunting with projectile weaponry (bows and arrows or spears). This follows from our understanding of optimal foraging strategies (Chapter 4), which indicates that hunters prefer larger prey, especially when faunal assemblages are not depleted by overhunting, since during the Pleistocene human population density was likely to have been low. In Sibudu, the high frequency of small carnivores (including felids, viverrids, mongooses, mustelids and canids) does not suggest net hunting as these species are difficult to capture with this method but are easily caught with snares or traps. Trapping of carnivores often requires bait, often living. Using one prey animal to catch another presupposes a certain commitment to catch the carnivore. Generally, catching carnivores is more about protection against physical harm by such animals than for food since their meat is rarely preferred (see Section 3.4). The high number of carnivore remains in Sibudu suggest that they may have been caught for symbolic, medicinal or magic reasons, e.g. for their fur, teeth and claws. The high frequency of bushpig remains in Sibudu also suggests snare hunting because the species is nocturnal and dangerous to hunt. Although net hunting for bushpigs could have occurred, as demonstrated by Pygmy net hunters (Dounias Reference Dounias2016), these animals are strong enough to tear nets or break through the line of beaters rather than be cornered (Skinner & Chimimba Reference Skinner and Chimimba2005). The abundant remains of blue duiker, a common forest antelope, may point also to snaring since this relatively small animal is widely caught nowadays with snares throughout West and Central Africa (Fa et al. Reference Fa, Ryan and Bell2005) but is more difficult to hunt with bow and arrow or spears. However, duikers can be attracted in to the open by hunters imitating the call of a distressed animal (Brosset Reference Brosset1966; van Vliet et al. Reference Van Vliet, Kaniowska, Bourgarel, Fargeot and Nasi2009). Finally, there is evidence that faunal assemblages in Sibudu became less diverse during the more recent Pleistocene, with fewer small mammals, bushpig or blue duiker remains being found. Wadley (Reference Wadley2010) explains this change to be the result of larger hunter group sizes appearing as a result of population growth, thus allowing the hunting of large prey with spears. It is possible that, as observed in modern hunter-gatherers, as human group sizes increased large animals may have been primarily targeted by men, with the added benefit for the more skilled hunters to attain social prestige and kudos (see Section 3.4). Although in some contexts women are known to participate in active hunts with men (e.g. Aka Pygmy net hunting, Noss & Hewlett Reference Noss and Hewlett2001), until recently when snaring has become commonplace due to the use of more affordable and accessible cable (Noss Reference Noss1998b), trapping animals may have been largely pursued by women. This division of labour between the sexes has been postulated in Fa et al. (Reference Fa, Stewart, Lloveras and Vargas2013) to explain the difference in hunting techniques used by Neanderthals and AMH in Pleistocene Iberia. Snare hunting therefore introduces an alternative to large prey hunting which increases food security when men are unable to bring prey home. For the Hadza in East Africa, the chances of obtaining large prey was only 3% or 45 days of failure between successes, whereas snaring was successful with only one to three days of failure in-between and an overall success rate 40 times higher than that for large prey (Hawkes et al. Reference Hawkes, O‘Connell and Blurton Jones1991, Reference Alvard and Kuznar2001).

It is possible that pit trap hunting may have occurred as early as 40,000 BP. In a study in the Japanese Archipelago as many as 376 traps were excavated from 51 Pleistocene sites (Sato Reference Sato2012). These were pits of about 1–2 metres in diameter, which may have been used for medium-sized prey such as wild boar and deer. Similarly, pit traps of over 20 metres in diameter have been discovered in Mexico containing a large assemblage of mammoth bones (Instituto Nacional de Antropología e Historia (Mexico) 2019). With an age of ~15,000 years they stem from the period of the peopling of the Americas and attest the versatility of humans hunting animals for food.

The oldest known confirmed evidence of the use of poison for hunting purposes dates to about 24,000 BP from the Border Cave in South Africa (d’Errico et al. Reference d’Errico, Backwell and Villa2012a). Although the identification and interpretation of archaeological micro-residuals of arrow poison is notoriously difficult (Bradfield et al. Reference Bradfield, Wadley and Lombard2015), biochemical traces have been identified as arrow poison (d’Errico et al. Reference d’Errico, Backwell and Villa2012b). The technical and symbolic items also found at the site suggest that the Border Cave inhabitants used the same material culture 44,000 BP as today’s San people, predating by 24,000 years the current consensus for the emergence of San hunter-gatherer cultural adaptations. The findings include bone points that are identical to San poisoned arrow points. Arrow points dated 37,000–35,000 BP, which could have been used with poison, have also been found at the White Paintings Rock Shelter in Botswana (Robbins et al. Reference Robbins, Campbell, Brook, Murphy and Hitchcock2012). Today, San use beetle larvae and plant extracts to prepare poison for their arrow heads (Lee et al. Reference Lee, Lee and DeVore1976).

Another technological revolution appeared with the use of nets for hunting small and medium-sized prey. The first evidence for net hunting comes from the Pavlov and Dolní Věstonice sites in the Czech Republic (Pringle Reference Pringle1997; Soffer Reference Soffer, Roebroeks, Mussi and Svoboda2000, Reference Alvard, Gillespie and Alvard2004) where clay fragments have been found bearing impressions of nets woven from wild plants and dated ~27,000 to ~25,00 BP. Evidence of the use of these nets is the large number of hare, fox and other small mammal bones found in the same site. The suggestion is that net-hunting is likely to have played an important role for food acquisition, introducing communal hunting practices that allowed children and women to participate (Soffer Reference Soffer, Roebroeks, Mussi and Svoboda2000). In the case of Aka Pygmy forest foragers of the Central African Republic women net-hunt more frequently than men (Noss & Hewlett Reference Noss and Hewlett2001). In this particular context, women participated in net hunts when game was relatively abundant, they received relatively high caloric returns from hunting, they had access to the means/technology just as men, and importantly, Aka men did not prohibit them from participating. Thus, systems that are flexible between the sexes and ages and that adapt to prey abundance are likely to have been advantageous. When large prey was less abundant, the possibility of obtaining large numbers of small and medium-sized prey might have contributed to the development of larger, more settled populations as indicated from numerous archaeological finds for the Gravettian hunter-gatherers from Spain to southern Russia (Soffer Reference Soffer, Roebroeks, Mussi and Svoboda2000). This new technology may have also contributed to the diminishing selection pressure for muscularity, vital for large mammal hunting without mechanically delivered projectiles (see Soffer, cited in Pringle Reference Pringle1997).

3.3 Modern Hunting Techniques

Table 3.1 gives an overview of hunting methods from a total of 125 study populations where adequate information has been published. Available data reflects a strong bias towards Africa (n = 90) compared to South/Central America (n = 21) and Asia (n = 13). In Africa, much research interest has been directed towards hunter-gatherer societies, in particular the different Pygmy communities (n = 33). Information on the proportions of different hunting methods encountered is often not directly comparable with each other because they refer to different baseline units such as hunters (e.g. 10% of hunters used guns only) or the number of animals killed with each method. Moreover, some studies distinguish between technology used to kill animals, such as firearms and traps, from techniques such as hunting with dogs and horses, whilst some studies combine both, especially hunting with dogs (dogs may be used to chase and corner animals but also to kill their prey). For example, Friant et al. (Reference Friant, Paige and Goldberg2015) report that techniques included traps (75%), guns (71%), machetes (71%) and dogs (18%). Elsewhere, they refer to ‘hunting with a gun and dog’, raising the question of how the prey was killed. Since hunters with dogs typically use other hunting technologies at the same time (Koster Reference Koster2009) and the use of ammunition is expensive, the question is how to interpret the data listed in publications. In the case of Friant et al.’s study (Reference Friant, Paige and Goldberg2015), did dogs kill 18% of prey and guns kill 71% or were guns even more important by killing 89% of prey? Hunting with nets is a similar problem because it remains often unclear how the netted animals were killed. Dounias (Reference Dounias2016) points out that spears are mainly used to deliver the coup de grace to animals cornered by dogs or nets and are rarely used as the sole hunting technique, but studies are often not clear whether spears are supporting net hunting or constitute separate strategies. Most studies imply but do not clearly indicate whether snares refer to modern snares that use wire cable made of metal or nylon. Snares made with plant fibres were recorded alongside snares made using metal wire in the Republic of Congo (Kitanishi Reference Kitanishi1995). Marrocoli et al. (Reference Marrocoli, Nielsen, Morgan, van Loon, Kulik and Kühl2019) refer to snare hunting throughout their document without specifying whether modern or traditional snares are used, but their reference to the Congolese law forbidding hunting with metal snares implies that the snare hunting in question refers to modern ones. Only three studies explicitly referred to traditional snares or snares made from vegetable fibres. Use of snares is generally illegal. Although all studies were careful to work on the basis of anonymity, the response especially regarding snares might be biased and may underestimate the general use of trapping with this method. Casual and unreliable reporting is a general problem with questionnaires, especially when illegal methods are involved. For example, in the state of Arunachal Pradesh in India, hunters reported 11 mammalian species hunted during formal interviews but another 22 species were observed during casual visits, festivals and informal discussions (Aiyadurai et al. Reference Aiyadurai, Singh and Milner-Gulland2010). Notwithstanding these shortcomings, several key features are discernible from Table 3.1, as examined below.

Table 3.1 Overview of hunting methods from a total of 125 study populations. When the mix of hunting methods differed or when different combinations of hunting methods were observed either over time or between study communities, studies are listed with separate entries for time or location

3.3.1 Hunters Use Many Different Technologies

Almost all studies list more than one technology used, their mix depending on the type of animals hunted and local traditions (Fig. 3.4). This variation is best illustrated by the broad spectrum of how bats are caught and killed. Bats are common wild meat items in Africa and Asia, but are rarely targeted in South and Central America with the exception of one Indigenous tribe, the Nambiquara of Western Brazil, who actively hunt three Phyllostomid bat species for food (Jenkins & Racey Reference Jenkins and Racey2009; Mickleburgh et al. Reference Mickleburgh, Waylen and Racey2009; Setz & Sazima Reference Setz and Sazima1987). Bats are usually hunted either at roosting sites or at feeding sites by a wide variety of methods – bait and hook, bamboo poles with thorns or fish hooks on the end, barbed burrs of some plant species, birdlime, bow and arrow, by hand, catapults and slingshots, fire, firearms including guns, rifles and airguns, funnel traps to catch bats emerging from caves, hand nets, kites with hooks, nylon lines and ropes with hooks, snares, specialized large nets or fishing nets at roost or feeding trees, sticks, stoning and thorny bushes (Mickleburgh et al. Reference Mickleburgh, Waylen and Racey2009).

Figure 3.4 (a) Asurini do Tocantins tribe member from the region between the Xingu and Tocantins rivers holding bow and arrow (WIN-Initiative/The Image Bank Unreleased/Getty Images); (b) Huaorani hunter from Ecuador using blowgun in forest (WIN-Initiative/The Image Bank Unreleased/Getty Images); (c) Bayaka or Aka tribesman holding spear in a forest in the Central African Republic (Timothy Allen/The Image Bank Unreleased/Getty Images); (d) Yangoru Boiken bat catcher retrieves a giant fruit bat from his net in the foothills of Mount Turu, East Sepik Province, Papua New Guinea.

(Timothy Allen/The Image Bank Unreleased/Getty Images)

3.3.2 Techniques Differ between Neighbouring Villages and Regions

Hunting techniques seem to be determined by a mix of tradition, prey species and their densities, but also by the motivations of the hunters themselves. Mbuti Pygmy hunters of the Ituri forest in the Northeastern Congo Basin can be divided according whether they preferentially use bows and arrows or net hunting (Carpaneto & Germi Reference Carpaneto and Germi1989, Reference Aunger1992; Ichikawa Reference Ichikawa1983; Terashima Reference Terashima1983). The use of bows and arrows is the original subsistence hunting strategy of all the Mbuti Pygmies and involves either solitary hunts in which the archer walks in the forest or lies in ambush, and communal beat-hunt hunting, the so-called mota. This Mbuti Efe subgroup in the northeastern parts of the Ituri forest is in social contact with local Sudanic Lese agriculturalists and has adopted iron arrowheads and spear blades from the Lese but never engages in net hunting. The Mbuti Swa use nets alongside bows and arrows. Living in the central, southern and western parts of the Ituri forest, they are in social contact with local Bantu Bira and Ndaka agriculturalists, from whom the Swa have adopted net technology when they invaded the forest about 1,000–3,000 BP.

The impact of the motivation to hunt with different methods is demonstrated by the four different ethnic groups living around the Banyang-Mbo Wildlife Sanctuary in southwestern Cameroon (Willcox & Nambu Reference Willcox and Nambu2007). The Mbo are almost exclusively hunters and gatherers while the Banyangi are hunters, gatherers and also cash-crop farmers. Both groups use shotguns and wire snares at almost the same rate (Banyangi 55% and 37%, respectively; Mbo 56% and 38%, respectively). The Bakossi and Basossi primarily farm cash crops (coffee and cocoa) and do not hunt, but their cash income allows them to buy domestic meat, wild meat and fish. In the oil palm plantation-dominated landscapes of Sumatra motivations and weapon choice are also aligned with the different ethnic groups (Luskin et al. Reference Luskin, Christina, Kelley and Potts2014). Subsistence hunting is predominant in Javanese immigrants and Malay smallholder farmers, who used snare hunting and air rifles to hunt macaques and wild boar for both pest control and consumption. Professional Chinese and Batak hunters specialized in herding wild boar into wire net traps for subsequent sale. Social and cultural reasons are also the motivations for weekly large social day hunts with dogs by ethnic Minangkabau. Finally, wealthy members of a sport hunting group used hunts with firearms from pick-up trucks at night for sport.

The choice of weapon can depend on which prey hunters pursue. For example, Rogan et al. (Reference Rogan, Lindsey and Tambling2017) demonstrates that prey size and weapon choice are significantly correlated with firearms preferred for large game. Optimal foraging theory (Chapter 4) predicts a broad prey profile when a less efficient hunting technology is used compared to a narrower profile for firearms. Indeed, Aché bow hunters’ prey profile is broad and includes less profitable species typically not targeted by shotgun hunters (Alvard, Reference Alvard1993; Hill & Hawkes Reference Hill, Hawkes, Hames and Vickers1983). Although firearm usage is not always correlated with the mean body mass of the target prey (Fa & García Yuste Reference Fa and García Yuste2001; Gonedelé Bi et al. Reference Benítez-López, Alkemade and Schipper2017), the general trend is the focus on large animals, one of the main threats against wildlife in Central Africa (Kümpel Reference Balée and Erickson2006). This is especially problematic for arboreal primates which are preferentially targeted with firearms (Bobo et al. Reference Bobo, Kamgaing, Kamdoum and Dzefack2015; Mittermeier Reference Mittermeier, Marsh and Mittermeier1987). Dounias (Reference Dounias2016) reports that Fang hunters shoot 82% of harvested primate biomass, although firearms contributed 37% of the hunted biomass overall. Traditional weapons may be superior for animals moving in large groups since silent weapons such as blowguns allow the pursuit of several group-members unlike rifles which will invariably disperse the targeted group after hearing the shot (Peres Reference Peres1990). However, shotguns can be used to kill several animals in large dense groups that remain in the same location, for example, the shooting of numerous individual mandrills who will remain on a refuge tree without dispersing, (see Fa and García Yuste (Reference Fa and García Yuste2001).

Prey density can also determine the hunting methods employed. In a study of hunting in two villages by Indigenous Central African foragers, Lupo and Schmitt (Reference Lupo and Schmitt2017) observed that although hunters used the same range of technology there were differences in the frequency of hunting strategies depending on prey density. Hunters from the village in whose area the mean encounter rates with blue duikers, a preferred prey species, was low, would use more individualized technologies such as hand capture, spears and snares. Village hunters from the area with overall high wildlife encounter rates would use nets more often, predominantly targeting blue duikers. In two adjacent Community Hunting Zones in Cameroon, the more densely populated and thus the more heavily hunted zone would be primarily hunted using firearms, providing 79% of the hunted prey, whereas the less densely populated zone was targeted by snares, providing 82% prey (Bobo et al. Reference Bobo, Kamgaing, Kamdoum and Dzefack2015).

3.3.3 Firearms Are the Dominant Hunting Technology Today

Firearms were used in three quarters of the study sites in Table 3.1. Wild animals are hunted with shotguns, rifles, airguns, modern automatic weapons such as the AK47 (Barnett Reference Barnett2000a) and ancient muzzle-loader guns (Barnett Reference Barnett2000a; Martin et al. Reference Arsuaga, Martinez and Arnold2013; Sirén & Wilkie Reference Sirén and Wilkie2016). There is limited information to determine when firearms started being used for hunting in the tropics. In Gabon, guns were traded in the late 1800s, but they only became common in the 1960s (Bernault, 1996, cited in Walters et al. Reference Walters, Schleicher, Hymas and Coad2015). In the Congo Basin, guns were imported from the 1840s onwards, but they did not become widespread until the twentieth century (Savorgnan de Brazza, 1888, cited in Dounias Reference Dounias2016). Technical advances in the mid 1800s, such as the invention of the breech-loading rifle, spread slowly because of the cost and restrictions imposed by the colonial masters. The price of gunpowder also remained prohibitive and delayed the advance of firearms for hunting until the 1930s (Dounias Reference Dounias2016). The subsequent change from black gun powder to the much more powerful smokeless powder improved hunting efficiency substantially as it dramatically increased shooting distance and, thus, outcompeted bows and arrows (Faure, 2002, cited in Dounias Reference Dounias2016). The final technical innovation making firearms highly detrimental for wildlife has been the introduction of flashlights (Dounias Reference Dounias2016), and more recently the even more efficient LED lights (Bowler et al. Reference Bowler, Beirne and Tobler2019). Lighting allows hunting at night, preferred by many hunters (e.g. Ahmadi et al. Reference Ahmadi, Maman and Zoumenou2018; Holmern et al. Reference Holmern, Mkama, Muya and Røskaft2006; Yasuoka et al. Reference Yasuoka, Hirai, Kamgaing, Dzefack, Kamdoum and Bobo2015) because many prey species are more easily detected due to eye reflection of light. Moreover, species such as duikers freeze when targeted by flashlights, making them especially easy prey for night hunters (Yasuoka et al. Reference Yasuoka, Hirai, Kamgaing, Dzefack, Kamdoum and Bobo2015).

Despite the cost of firearms having come down dramatically and the availability of relatively cheap, locally produced weapons, initial purchase and cost of ammunition remains prohibitive for many people in the tropics (Sirén & Wilkie Reference Sirén and Wilkie2016). Gun hunting is the prime activity of soldiers and of wealthier individuals, often from regional cities, such as in Madagascar (Golden Reference Golden2009) and Cameroon (Yasuoka Reference Yasuoka2006b). In many hunter-gatherer societies, gun hunting appears only secondary to snaring because of the higher cost and the greater skill involved (Kümpel Reference Balée and Erickson2006; Kümpel et al. Reference Albertí, Panea and Sañudo2008). This is borne out for most studies on Indigenous Peoples where there is quantitative data (Table 3.1). For example, Baka Pygmy hunters need to sell six duiker carcasses to buy a single rifle cartridge (Yasuoka Reference Yasuoka2006b). None of the Aka Pygmy hunters owned firearms, but most of their non-Pygmy neighbours owned shotguns and sometimes rifles (Kitanishi Reference Kitanishi1995). To overcome the problem of excessive costs of hunting with firearms, three strategies are often adopted. First, many hunters reload their cartridges with locally produced gunpowder (de Mattos Vieira et al. Reference De Mattos Vieira, von Muhlen and Shepard2015) or use muzzle-loaders (Sirén & Wilkie Reference Sirén and Wilkie2016). Second, Baka Pygmy and Aka Pygmy hunters provide services for their richer, non-Pygmy neighbours who provide them with firearms and ammunition in exchange for meat (Duda et al. Reference Duda2017; Kitanishi Reference Kitanishi1995; Yasuoka Reference Yasuoka2006b; Yasuoka et al. Reference Yasuoka, Hirai, Kamgaing, Dzefack, Kamdoum and Bobo2015). Here the main parts of a carcass are kept by the owner of the ammunition, thus making the social relationships between the parties involved also a strong social regulator (Hayashi Reference Hayashi2008). In the case of the Aka Pygmies, the hunter is given the head, neck and internal organs plus two cigarettes (Kitanishi Reference Kitanishi1995). Third, increased market participation can increase cash income and make firearms and ammunition more affordable (Levi et al. Reference Levi, Lu, Yu and Mangel2011a). For those who decide to follow this avenue, the extra cost forces hunters to maximize the amount of meat per shot (Jerozolimski & Peres Reference Jerozolimski and Peres2003), and to provide meat for sale rather than for their own consumption.

There are also social barriers against adopting hunting with firearms, especially amongst hunter-gatherers. Baka Pygmy hunters fear conflicts with the owners and lenders of guns, fear anti-poaching controls, fear the physical risks posed by old weapons and often do not have the required skills to use firearms (Duda Reference Duda2017; Duda et al. Reference Duda2017). On the other hand, firearm hunting bestows high social status. In Baka Pygmy society, shotgun hunters are often considered ‘the best hunters’ (Duda Reference Duda2017). When Baka hunters have access to guns, their success can exceed the success of non-Pygmy hunters dramatically (Fa et al. Reference Bartlett, Williams and Prescott2016). Where poaching carries the risk of detection, such as in protected areas, firearms are generally avoided as gunshots alert ranger patrols to the presence of the hunter (Gandiwa Reference Gandiwa2011). A new development in areas where hunting with firearms is illegal is the replacement of firearms by running prey down with motorbikes or horses and then killing the animals with axes, clubs, spears or machetes to avoid the sound of shots, for example, in Tanzania (Kiffner et al. Reference Kiffner, Kioko and Kissui2014) and Botswana (Rogan et al. Reference Rogan, Lindsey and Tambling2017).

Firearm use in the surveyed studies can vary from none to all hunters using firearms and none to all animals killed with firearms, indicating that a multitude of factors impact gun use. There is a general, but not uniform, trend of increasing gun usage over time. Gill et al. (Reference Gill, Fa, Rowcliffe and Kümpel2012) visited the same site during a period of rapid national economic growth in Equatorial Guinea and reported that hunters using guns increased from 2% in 2003 to 22% by 2010. Percentage of animals taken by shotgun rose over a 14-year period on Bioko Island in Equatorial Guinea (Cronin et al. Reference Cronin, Meñe and Butynski2010). In Cameroon, gun use increased, replacing snares, in both a 15-year study in the Lebialem Division, in the southwest of the country (Wright & Priston Reference Wright and Priston2010) and in three villages near the Dja Biosphere Reserve (southeast Cameroon) between 2003 and 2016 (Ávila et al. Reference Ávila, Tagg and Willie2019).

Gun use differs amongst ethnicities, reflecting changes in socio-economic systems and lifestyle. Shotgun use in Aka Pygmy hunters increased as a percentage of livelihood activities from 5–29% in 1992/3 to 18–56% in 2007/8. By contrast, their farmer-fisher neighbours kept shotgun use at a similar level; 7–29% in 1992/2 and 7–27% in 2007/8 (Riddell Reference Riddell2013). Yasuoka et al. (Reference Yasuoka, Hirai, Kamgaing, Dzefack, Kamdoum and Bobo2015) report on gun use of Konabembe in Cameroon in two adjacent areas and found that at one site 17% of hunters used guns (killing 16% of prey) whilst at a second site 58% of hunters used guns and killed 70% of prey. The two sites differed in their prey density, species composition, forest disturbance, hunting pressure and human density, but it remains unclear what caused the difference in gun use.

In Liberia, ancestral hunting methods have all disappeared and have been replaced by cable snares and shotguns since the civil conflict from 1989 to 2003 (Bene et al. Reference Alvard, Gillespie and Alvard2013). Similarly, civil conflicts which have ravaged the Congo Basin for decades have collapsed many economies and the easy access to firearms may have enabled even more wild meat hunting. In the Amazon Basin, Western contact has led to a rapid change from traditional methods to guns and dogs across all Indigenous societies (Jerozolimski & Peres Reference Jerozolimski and Peres2003). Amongst the Huaorani living in the Yasuní National Park, Ecuador, changes in hunting practices were reported in Papworth et al. (Reference Papworth, Milner-Gulland and Slocombe2013b). The only two South American studies that did not record the use of firearms were both from the 1980s and are for the Huaorani in Ecuador (Rival Reference Rival, Pescola and Palsson2003; Yost & Kelley Reference Yost, Kelley, Hames and Vickers1983). All studies from the 1990s and 2000s reported the use of firearms (Franzen Reference Franzen2006; Lu Reference Lu1999).

There is clear evidence that hunting with guns is much more efficient than hunting with bow and arrow or blowpipes in terms of the number of animals killed at least in the short term. Alvard and Kaplan (Reference Alvard, Kaplan and Stiner1991) compared two Indigenous communities in southeastern Peru, one of which hunted with guns and the other with traditional bow and arrow. Shotgun hunters had relatively short pursuits, averaged 1.3 shots per kill, killed prey often with a single shot, and had uniformly high return rates ranging between 1.2 and 1.5 kg per hour of hunting. In contrast, bow hunters had relatively long pursuits, averaged 30 shots per kill, often missed their target or wounded but did not kill the prey and had low return rates of 0.10 kg per hour of hunting. Wherever there is a mix of modern and traditional hunting technology used, guns attain higher prey kills in study sites in South America (Alvard & Kaplan Reference Alvard, Kaplan and Stiner1991), Africa (e.g. Baka Pygmy hunters, Duda et al. Reference Duda2017) and Asia (e.g. in Arunachal Pradesh, northeast India Aiyadurai et al. Reference Aiyadurai, Singh and Milner-Gulland2010).

The problem with hunting with guns lies in its long-term impact on prey populations if commercial hunting is the main aim. Guns allow the killing of many more animals in a shorter time than traditional hunting methods. When prey is hunted for hunters’ own consumption only, the increased efficiency afforded by guns reduces the required time for subsistence hunting and can allow hunters to reinvest spare time in other activities. When prey is being sold, however, the time saved can be invested in killing more animals. When animals and prey are not privately but communally owned, the Tragedy of the Commons dictates that as many animals as possible should be killed, leading to severe overhunting and unsustainability (Chapters 4 and 5).

3.3.4 Modern Snares Are the Second Most Utilized Hunting Technology Globally

Traditional snares are encountered rarely, with only five study sites reporting snares made of vines, mostly from the last century (Carpaneto & Germi Reference Carpaneto and Germi1989; Dounias Reference Dounias2016; Fa et al. Reference Bartlett, Williams and Prescott2016; Kitanishi Reference Kitanishi1995; Tessmann, Reference Tessmann1913a). Modern cable snares were used in 57% of study sites. Snare hunting is rare in the Neotropics, where only 2 out of 20 study sites listed them as a hunting technique, compared to 75% in Asia and 68% in Africa. Although snares appear to be a major cause of prey declines across tropical Asia, information on snare hunting is still sketchy (Gray et al. Reference Fiedel and Haynes2018; Harrison et al. Reference Harrison, Sreekar and Brodie2016). Their relative absence in the Neotropics might be related to the generally lower prey population densities of forest mammals, making snares comparatively unprofitable (Fa & Purvis Reference Fa and Purvis1997; Peres Reference Peres2000). However, although snares are illegal in most countries in Africa, they still account for most prey killed, as well as biomass, compared to firearms (Fa & Peres Reference Fa, Peres, Reynolds, Mace, Redford and Robinson2001). Their popularity is based on the fact that the material used, wire or nylon cord (Fig. 3.5), is easy to obtain, inexpensive, durable and strong enough for larger animals. Snares can be set under cover of darkness and their low detectability makes them a prime choice for poachers (Gandiwa Reference Gandiwa2011; Knapp Reference Knapp2012; Lindsey et al. Reference Lindsey, Romañach, Tambling, Chartier and Groom2011b). Snare hunting also requires relatively little skill compared to guns, but experienced hunters may be much more successful than inexperienced ones (Kümpel Reference Balée and Erickson2006). In some cases, wire can be stolen from wildlife and domestic fences, as in the Savé Valley Conservancy in Kenya, where rangers removed almost 85,000 snares, all of which were thought to have been made from the perimeter fence (Lindsey et al. Reference Lindsey, Balme and Becker2015). In Zimbabwe, stolen telephone copper cables and steel wire from a veterinary fence along a National Park have been used (Gandiwa Reference Gandiwa2011).

Figure 3.5 Blue duiker caught using nylon rope snares in Bioko Island, Equatorial Guinea.

(photo: M. Grande-Vega)

As for firearms, snares have become more popular over time. Wire cable was introduced in Cameroon after the Second World War and has almost totally replaced vegetal fibre, which rots with moisture, or elephant hair that is only suitable for small prey, although snares made of rattan are still used for small prey (Dounias Reference Dounias2016). As a result, the Fang in Cameroon have changed from more traditional methods to focus on snares, now accounting for 54% of the hunted prey biomass (Dounias Reference Dounias2016). Traditionally, Baka Pygmies used mainly spears for hunting (Bahuchet Reference Bahuchet1992), but wire snares were introduced in the 1960s after contact with neighbouring cultivators to replace traditional, plant-based snares (Hayashi Reference Hayashi2008; Yasuoka Reference Yasuoka2006b; Yasuoka et al. Reference Yasuoka, Hirai, Kamgaing, Dzefack, Kamdoum and Bobo2015). Nowadays, cable snares are the most common hunting technique amongst Pygmy hunters including Baka since this allows for a larger number of captures, therefore suitable for commercial wild meat production (Hayashi Reference Hayashi2008; Noss Reference Noss, Robinson and Bennett2000).

Snares are typically set, left unsupervised and then checked periodically (Fig. 3.5). The passive nature of snaring allows farmers to set and check snares in and at the margin of their fields. Their passive nature can result in indiscriminate capture of animals (Becker et al. Reference Becker, McRobb and Watson2013; MacMillan & Nguyen Reference MacMillan and Nguyen2014) although relatively targeted setting of snares is possible (Coad Reference Coad2007). The non-selectiveness is demonstrated by sex ratios of 1:1 in any captured species as predicted in a study in Equatorial Guinea (Fa & García Yuste Reference Fa and García Yuste2001). Although daily checks are desirable from an animal welfare point of view, longer periods appear to be the norm rather than the exception. For example, Baka hunters checked snares on average every three days (Yasuoka et al. Reference Yasuoka, Hirai, Kamgaing, Dzefack, Kamdoum and Bobo2015). The consequence of leaving snares unchecked or checking these sporadically can lead to significant animal welfare issues and loss of carcasses. But even when checked regularly, animals may suffer from injury and stress. Up to a quarter of total captures can be lost to scavengers and decomposition (Noss Reference Noss1998b), and around one third of animals can escape with injury (Lindsey et al. Reference Lindsey, Romañach, Tambling, Chartier and Groom2011b). In Zimbabwe, 59% of snared animals were found rotten or scavenged, 27% recovered by scouts and only 14% were extracted by poachers out of the 2,398 animals recorded as killed in snares from August 2005 to July 2009 (Lindsey et al. Reference Lindsey, Romañach, Tambling, Chartier and Groom2011b). In hunting zones around a study village in the Dja Biosphere Reserve in Cameroon, snare density was inversely related to distance from the village, but despite a 64% lower snare density in the farthest hunting zone compared to the closest zone, overall capture rate was four times higher and the number of rotting carcasses three times greater in the farthest zone (Muchaal & Ngandjui Reference Muchaal and Ngandjui1999). Dobson et al. (Reference Dobson, Milner-Gulland, Ingram and Keane2019) modelled the amount of usable harvest when 20 snares are checked daily versus only once over a 30-day snaring period to find a three-fold increase for the daily-check scenario.

Two main categories of snares exist – foot snares and neck snares, – whereby the latter constitute the vast majority (Kümpel et al. Reference Alves, Mendonça, Confessor, Vieira and Lopez2009; Noss Reference Noss, Robinson and Bennett2000; Yasuoka et al. Reference Yasuoka, Hirai, Kamgaing, Dzefack, Kamdoum and Bobo2015). When animals are entangled with their heads in neck snares, they are normally strangled to death. Larger animals can be caught by their feet in neck snares. After an animal steps into a typical foot snares, a bent rod springs loose, causing a wire fastened to the rod to wrap around the animal’s foot and lift it into the air (Yasuoka et al. Reference Yasuoka, Hirai, Kamgaing, Dzefack, Kamdoum and Bobo2015). Animals larger than those targeted by any type of snare can still be caught but can escape – often with injuries. The two methods are characterized by different prey profiles. For example, in a study in Equatorial Guinea carnivores were only captured in neck snares and rodents solely in foot snares, but a large proportion of duikers were caught in both types though some were also shot (Kümpel et al. Reference Alves, Mendonça, Confessor, Vieira and Lopez2009). Ungulates, rodents and carnivores are relatively more vulnerable to snares than to firearms (Fa & García Yuste Reference Fa and García Yuste2001). Medium-sized mammals are significantly more vulnerable to snaring (Duda et al. Reference Duda2017; Fa et al. Reference Fa, Ryan and Bell2005; Fa & García Yuste Reference Fa and García Yuste2001; Noss Reference Noss1998b). Rodents, for example, represented 92% of snared animals in southeastern Cameroon (Bobo et al. Reference Bobo, Kamgaing, Kamdoum and Dzefack2015). A review of 36 tropical moist forests sites in seven countries in West and Central Africa revealed higher harvest rates for snared than for shot species, significantly smaller body size of snared versus shot species, higher extraction rates for terrestrial versus arboreal species, higher extraction rates for fast and medium speed species versus slower ones and higher extraction rates for frugivore–herbivores versus other dietary categories (Fa et al. Reference Fa, Ryan and Bell2005).

Snares are typically set along trails and often in clusters. Densities can vary widely with values of up to 56/km2 as reported from Equatorial Guinea (Fa & García Yuste Reference Fa and García Yuste2001). In a private property near the Savé Valley Conservancy in Zimbabwe, 1.16 and 0.68 snares/km2 were removed in 1998 and 1999, respectively, but the number increased dramatically to around 90 snares/km2 per year from 2005 to 2009 after the settlement of adjacent land (Lindsey et al. Reference Lindsey, Romañach, Tambling, Chartier and Groom2011b). Whilst almost 85,000 snares were detected in a period of eight years in the Savé Valley Conservancy, 170,000 snares were removed from just two protected areas in Vietnam and Cambodia between 2011 and 2015 (Gray et al. Reference Fiedel and Haynes2018). The effects of snaring can be chilling. In Asia and in Africa, snaring is a major contributor to defaunation (Becker et al. Reference Becker, McRobb and Watson2013; Gray et al. Reference Fiedel and Haynes2018; Noss Reference Noss1998b). This not only affects target species but also by-catch. Becker et al. (Reference Becker, McRobb and Watson2013) analysed by-catch data from 1,038 antipoaching patrols in Zambia’s Luangwa valley focusing on savanna elephants and large carnivores, to show that additional mortalities of 32% for elephants, 20% for adult (>4 years) male lions and 14–50% of adult and yearling pack members of African wild dogs were inflicted.

3.3.5 Traditional Hunting Technology Generally Augments Modern Technology Today

In only 15% of sites in Table 3.1 neither firearms nor cable snares were used. This proportion is biased towards traditional hunting methods such as bow and arrow and nets because 11 of the 19 sites in the table stem from a single study on Mbuti Pygmies in the DRC that documented a different mix of hunting technologies at different sites (see Fa et al. Reference Bartlett, Williams and Prescott2016). Here, we introduce the five most important traditional techniques: spears, dogs, bow and arrow, nets and blowpipes.

3.3.5.1 Spears

Spears are the oldest hunting weapons discovered and predate the emergence of modern man. As outlined above (see Section 3.2.1) the 400,000 year old Schöningen spears were likely used for thrusting or hand-thrown at close quarters (Shea Reference Shea2006; Thieme Reference Thieme1997; Fig. 3.6). Virtually all historically known hunter-gatherers used spears but none employed spears alone without projectile weapons (Churchill Reference Churchill and Churchill2014). In an extensive survey of small-scale hunter-gatherer societies across the world, Churchill (Reference Churchill1993) observed that almost all had spears in their arsenal, but only half used them for terrestrial hunting. Other uses include hunting marine mammals, spearing fish, warfare and defence from predators. Most spears are thrusting spears and only some groups in Australia and Tasmania are known to use thin, light spears to hunt wallabies and kangaroos. Excluding these thin spears, the distance when thrown is 5.7 ± 0.9 m (Churchill Reference Churchill1993) or up to 20 m (Milks et al. Reference Milks, Parker and Pope2019), depending on the thrower’s skill. Typical use of thrusting spears is as a dispatching tool after prey has been driven by hunters, with or without accompanying dogs, into a disadvantaged position when using nets, snow drifts or geographical features such as valleys, rocks and swamps, or have been caught in traps. Spears were used as part of pitfall traps, but this technology has been replaced by snaring (Lewis & Phiri Reference Lewis and Phiri1998). The spearthrower, also called the atlatl, is a tool that uses leverage to throw a spear or dart at longer distances with a range of about 40 metres (Churchill Reference Churchill1993). The earliest known example is from the upper Palaeolithic in the Dordogne, France; it is made of reindeer antler and dated approx. 17,500 BP (Cattelain Reference Cattelain1989). As a hunting weapon, it is used for marine mammals and waterfowl in Oceania, the Arctic and part of the Americas, but terrestrial hunting is known only from Australia (Cattelain Reference Cattelain and Knecht1997; Churchill Reference Churchill1993).

Figure 3.6 Spear II. The 2.30 m long spear is shown to the left of an incomplete pelvis of a horse, and the base has been broken off. Inset shows a detail of the tip of spear II. Scale in cm.

(From Thieme Reference Thieme1997; reprinted with permission from Springer Nature.)

Spears have been reported for wild meat hunting in Africa, the Americas and Asia (Table 3.1). In the early twentieth century, spears were extensively used by the Fang during collective hunting expeditions (Tessmann Reference Tessmann1913a). Like weapons of war, spears were richly carved and ornamented, thus expressing a special cultural importance. Elevation into the mythical level was also observed elsewhere, such as the Huaorani in South America. With the advent of firearms, spears were slowly replaced and the Fang do not have spears in their arsenal any longer (Dounias Reference Dounias2016). Today, spears are a Pygmy specialty in the rainforest where non-Pygmy people do not use them, but are also still encountered in Madagascar, Eastern Africa savannas and amongst the San in the Kalahari desert (Dounias Reference Dounias2016; Fa et al. Reference Bartlett, Williams and Prescott2016; Golden Reference Golden2009; Holmern et al. Reference Holmern, Mkama, Muya and Røskaft2006; Ikeya Reference Ikeya1994). Pygmy groups use spears to kill larger and more aggressive mammals like elephants, African buffaloes, bushbucks, gorillas and wild pigs or as a dispatching tool, especially in net hunts and whenever any opportunity arises (Bahuchet Reference Bahuchet1992; Carpaneto & Germi Reference Carpaneto and Germi1989; Hayashi Reference Hayashi2008; Kitanishi Reference Kitanishi1995; Yasuoka Reference Yasuoka2006a).

3.3.5.2 Dogs

Wild and domestic animals have been widely used as ‘auxiliary animals’ to assist in foraging generally and hunting in particular (Dounias Reference Dounias, Stépanoff and Vigne2018). Several birds of prey, ungulates, elephantids and felids have been tamed as hunting auxiliaries. For example, horseback falconry is still actively performed in Central Asia today and can be traced back to the second millennium BCE (Soma Reference Soma2012). Indian hunters used tamed Indian antelope and Indian gazelle as decoys by sending them into a wild herd with nooses attached to their horns. Once an intruder was engaged by a wild animal its horns would easily become entangled with the tame animal’s noose and could then be easily caught (Menon Reference Menon2000). Similarly, tame female Asian elephants serve as decoys in Sri Lanka, India, Myanmar, Cambodia and Thailand, to attract wild individuals into places where they can more easily be trapped (Baker & Manwell Reference Baker and Manwell1983).

Domestic dogs are the undisputedly most important hunting auxiliary worldwide. Hunting has been discussed as one of the principle factors motivating the domestication of the dog from the wolf (Lupo Reference Lupo, Albarella and Trentacoste2011; Olsen Reference Olsen1985). Hunting dogs are used by numerous subsistence hunters in a broad range of locations and habitats across the world (Koster Reference Koster2009). An exception appears to be in the tropical New World, where many Amazonian societies apparently lacked dogs until the historical period, though dogs may have been brought to South America by Paleoindians (Fiedel Reference Fiedel2005). However, because bones do not preserve well in Neotropical settings it is difficult to make firm conclusions from the absence of archaeological evidence (Koster Reference Koster2009). Hunting with dogs is both a traditional and a modern technology. Parallel to the introduction of firearms many of the previously isolated Neotropical societies acquired dogs (Koster Reference Koster2009). Dogs are now common throughout Latin America, emphasizing their versatility and importance for hunting. Dogs were likely introduced to the Mbuti Pygmy hunters by agriculturalists (Carpaneto & Germi Reference Carpaneto and Germi1989) and have recently been used more frequently by numerous societies (Barnett Reference Barnett2000b; Franzen Reference Franzen2006; Ikeya Reference Ikeya1994). For example, hunting with dogs was once only of relatively low importance for the San in the Kalahari, but became more common as hunting with bow and arrows declined. This increase has been facilitated by a general increase in the dog population, by the ease of using these animals, by the convenience of having accompanying dogs when walking long distances and by the increase in market demand in a developing commercial economy (Ikeya Reference Ikeya1994). In Table 3.1, the percentage of hunts with dogs varied dramatically from no dogs to 86% by Mayangna and Miskito hunters in Nicaragua (Koster Reference Koster2008b) and 85% of hunts alongside hunters on horseback in the Brazilian Pantanal (Jean Desbiez et al. Reference Jean Desbiez, Keuroghlian, Piovezan and Bodmer2011). While Piro shotgun hunters brought dogs on only 3% of the hunts, the Machiguenga bow hunters living just 90 km away brought dogs on three quarters of their hunts (Alvard & Kaplan Reference Alvard, Kaplan and Stiner1991); this difference may have been caused by different prey densities (Koster Reference Koster2009). Among poachers, for example on farmland in KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa, some prefer using dogs over other methods such as hand-weapons, in this case ‘knobkerries’, traditional strong, short wooden clubs with a heavy rounded knob on one end used by Southern African tribes (Grey-Ross et al. Reference Grey-Ross, Downs and Kirkman2010). Hunting with dogs in some areas includes more women and juveniles than in other forms of hunting, which are primarily dominated by adult males (Koster Reference Koster2009).

Dogs are used to directly kill small prey but, more importantly and more frequently, are used to flush out or corner prey to be dispatched by the hunters or to drive prey into nets or traps. Sometimes, dogs are the only means to target species that are rare or are otherwise not susceptible to other hunting methods. For example, hunters in Lebialem Division, Cameroon, could only locate drills by hunting with dogs (Wright & Priston Reference Wright and Priston2010). Poachers in Zimbabwe primarily target hyrax (100%), warthog (93%), bushpig (85%) and baboons (79%) with dogs, whilst buffalo (100%), zebra (98%), wildebeest (97%), impala (97%) and eland (90%) are primarily killed with snares (Lindsey et al. Reference Lindsey, Romañach, Tambling, Chartier and Groom2011b). Advantages of dogs vary. An increased encounter rate with prey and, thus, a higher efficiency has been described in several studies (Alves et al. Reference Alves, Mendonça, Confessor, Vieira and Lopez2009; Koster Reference Koster2008b; Nobayashi Reference Nobayashi and Snyder2016). Among the San in the Kalahari, dogs are not reported to increase encounter rates, but they decrease handling time between detection and kill of prey such as gemsbok (Ikeya Reference Ikeya1994). In Nicaragua, the return rates of hunting tapirs, the largest prey species in the study, with dogs, with rifles, and with both guns and dogs were comparable (Koster Reference Koster2008b). On the other hand, hunters with dogs encounter more than eight times as many agoutis as hunters without dogs in the same study, indicating that hunting dogs can rival firearms in their overall effectiveness.

3.3.5.3 Bow and Arrow

Bows and arrows are used across the world, mainly in traditional hunter-gatherer societies (Table 3.1). The technique is very versatile, allowing ambush, pursuit and disadvantage hunting. Bow hunting allows the hunter to focus on the behavioural characteristics of a given prey species without the constraints of prey size and terrain features encountered when hunting with spears or atlatl (Churchill Reference Churchill1993). However, to kill the prey, the arrow must be precisely lodged between ribs or behind the shoulder blade, thus resulting in short effective distance of 25.8 m on average (Churchill Reference Churchill1993) and in low success rates (Liebenberg Reference Liebenberg2006). Iron-tipped arrows are deadlier than simple wooden arrows but are heavy and, thus, limited by short distances. Mbuti archers use iron-tipped arrows for terrestrial animals and light wooden arrows, often with poisoned tips, for arboreal prey, because they are light and can be used at larger distances than iron-tipped arrows, but they often require that wounded animals are tracked (Carpaneto & Germi Reference Carpaneto and Germi1989). Mbuti archers hunt either individually or in groups in forest environments (Carpaneto & Germi Reference Carpaneto and Germi1989), but Hadza, the hunter-gatherers of savannahs in Tanzania, usually hunt alone to be able to approach prey in open habitats close enough without being detected to be able to shoot (Marlowe Reference Marlowe2005). The prey portfolio of bow hunters differs from gun hunters. When Piro shotgun hunters used bows, they consistently pursued many species ignored when hunting with shotguns (Alvard Reference Alvard1993), which is consistent with foraging theory which predicts a broader prey selection when hunting with less efficient bow technology (Hill & Hawkes Reference Hill, Hawkes, Hames and Vickers1983). Although bows and arrows are still used by traditional hunters, they are largely been superseded by the more efficient firearms. The specialist knowledge and traditional skills required for bow and arrow hunting, such as how to prepare poison, has often vanished, for example in the Katu ethnic group in Vietnam (MacMillan & Nguyen Reference MacMillan and Nguyen2014).

3.3.5.4 Blowpipes

Blowpipes, also called blowguns, are long narrow tubes for shooting light darts, seeds or clay pellets primarily used for hunting wildlife. They have been used by many Indigenous Peoples in Eastern regions of North America (Riley Reference Riley1952), in tropical forests in Southeast Asia (Alvard Reference Alvard, Robinson and Bennett1999a; Bennett et al. Reference Alvard, Robinson and Bennett1999; Kuchikura Reference Kuchikura1988; Naito et al. Reference Naito, Abe, Okuda, Salleh, Masayoshi and Yintiso2005), South and Central America (Papworth et al., Reference Papworth, Milner-Gulland and Slocombe2013a, Reference Papworth, Milner-Gulland and Slocombeb; Riley Reference Riley1952; Rival Reference Rival, Pescola and Palsson2003; Yost & Kelley Reference Yost, Kelley, Hames and Vickers1983), and in Madagascar (Andrianaivoarivelo et al. Reference Andrianaivoarivelo, Andriafidison and Rahaingonirina2012; Jenkins & Racey Reference Jenkins and Racey2009). Blowpipes are exclusively employed by men who hunt typically alone or in small groups (Naito et al. Reference Naito, Abe, Okuda, Salleh, Masayoshi and Yintiso2005; Rival Reference Rival, Pescola and Palsson2003; Yost & Kelley Reference Yost, Kelley, Hames and Vickers1983). The Semaq Beri hunter-gatherers of Peninsular Malaysia sometimes hunt in pairs with one specialized blowpipe hunter accompanied by a ‘carrier’ who only gathers and carries the killed animals (Kuchikura Reference Kuchikura1988). Several types of blowpipes exist. In North and Central America, it is a simple tube; in the Guyana region of Northeastern South America, it is a tube fitted into a protective sheath; in the Amazon Basin, it is a composite of two longitudinal half-tubes, but there are also intermediate types (Riley Reference Riley1952). In Southeast Asia, double tubes are reported in which both the inner and outer tubes are composites of longitudinal half-tubes (Kuchikura Reference Kuchikura1988). Darts are either not poisoned, in North America, or poisoned with curare, a toxic extract from various plant species including the Curarea vine, in South and Central America. In Southeast Asia, poison from the sap of the ipoh tree is used (Kuchikura Reference Kuchikura1988). The poison is highly effective and relatively fast. In one reported case, it took the curare of 11 darts to bring a puma crashing down from a tree in just over half an hour (Yost & Kelley Reference Yost, Kelley, Hames and Vickers1983).

Blowpipes are typically used for overhead hunting of arboreal species, such as bats, birds, primates and squirrels, but terrestrial vertebrates, such as pigs and peccaries, can also be targeted (Piper & Rabett Reference Piper and Rabett2009; Rival Reference Rival, Pescola and Palsson2003). The technological differences between the blowpipes and darts and the effectiveness of the poison used impact prey choice of blowpipe hunters as demonstrated by the comparison of the different hunting strategies of the Huaorani of Ecuador (Yost & Kelley Reference Yost, Kelley, Hames and Vickers1983) and the Semaq Beri of Malaysia (Kuchikura Reference Kuchikura1988).The Semaq Beri focus on hunting dusky and banded leaf monkeys but occasionally also kill large-sized Malayan and cream-coloured giant squirrels. Middle-sized and small-sized squirrels and birds are, however, ignored. In contrast, the Huaorani’s prey included not only monkeys such as the Venezuelan red howler monkeys but also birds of all sizes (nearly 45% of the total catches) and ground-dwelling animals (15% of catches), whereby small-sized animals of less than 2 kg contribute nearly 60% of the total number of animals killed. The Huaorani’s blowpipes are longer and produce higher velocities and higher penetration for their darts than those of the Semaq Beri. Success rate for the Huaorani is approximately 2.4 times higher and return rate 4 times higher. Crucially, the preparation time for darts is half for Huaorani than for Semaq Beri allowing the Huaorani to carry and use more darts than the Semaq Beri (100–300 versus 10–50, respectively). Thus, the Huaorani do not conserve darts and shoot even at small birds of less than 50 g including hummingbirds whereas the Semaq Beri save darts until they encounter larger primates, their prey of choice.

Blowpipe hunter groups produce a diversity of blowpipe darts depending on the prey. Some dart types are easy to make in great quantities and can be used without moderation; some others should not be lost and are saved carefully for appropriate prey.

3.3.5.5 Nets

Hunting with nets mainly occurs in three main contexts. First, nets have been reported to be used to hunt bats and birds. For example, bat hunting is practiced in Benin, Cameroon, Madagascar and the Seychelles in Africa, and in India, Indonesia, Malaysia, Myanmar, Philippines, Thailand and Vietnam in Asia (Golden Reference Golden2009; Jenkins & Racey Reference Jenkins and Racey2009; Mickleburgh et al. Reference Mickleburgh, Waylen and Racey2009; Riley Reference Riley2002; Struebig et al. Reference Struebig, Harrison, Cheyne and Limin2007). Specialized mist nets are used in China, India, Indonesia and Laos (Mickleburgh et al. Reference Mickleburgh, Waylen and Racey2009). Second, nets can be components of traps, either underground for small mammals or as net traps to capture terrestrial mammals. Third, net hunting of terrestrial animals has been practiced by a variety of Indigenous Peoples in the Congo Basin (Dupré Reference Dupré1976) and is today a specialty of some Pygmy populations. Up to the 1950s, the Fang used nets, but abandoned them for modern hunting technology (Dounias Reference Dounias2016; Koch Reference Koch1968). According to Dounias (Reference Dounias2016), it is not surprising that ‘Pygmies have become the natural depositories of net hunting since this activity exacerbates egalitarian values like mutual aid, food sharing, social links between communities, spatial mobility and demographic fluidity.’ Mbuti Pygmies use large collective beat-hunts with women as beaters (Carpaneto & Germi Reference Carpaneto and Germi1989). The nets are between 1 m and 1.5 m high and between 30 m and 100 m long and belong to the man who wove it (Ichikawa Reference Ichikawa1983). Animals are driven into a circular array of individual nets where men hide near their own nets ready to kill the game running towards them. Those Mbuti who sell their prey animals to Muslim Bantu kill the animals by cutting their throat with a knife in order to adhere to Muslim requirements for slaughter (Carpaneto & Germi Reference Carpaneto and Germi1989). The gender role in the Aka is opposite to the Mbuti as the adult and adolescent men set the nets and beat the bush, whereas women capture the animals caught in the net, waiting around the nets; sometimes they lead net hunting and also beat the bush when no adult men participate in the hunt (Bailey & Aunger Reference Bailey and Aunger1989; Kitanishi Reference Kitanishi1995; Noss Reference Noss, Robinson and Bennett2000). In Gabon, game was driven into nets by fire, but this practice has been abandoned and fire is mainly being used to promote growth of grass to which grazing game is attracted (Walters et al. Reference Walters, Schleicher, Hymas and Coad2015). Whilst the Mbuti specialize in net hunting and the Aka use nets alongside cable snares, the sympatric Efe do not practice net hunting (Carpaneto & Germi Reference Carpaneto and Germi1989; Fa et al. Reference Bartlett, Williams and Prescott2016; Kitanishi Reference Kitanishi1995). Bailey and Aunger (Reference Bailey and Aunger1989) argue that the Efe never adopted net hunting because their neighbouring farmers do not use them whereas the Mbuti adopted net hunting from their neighbours. Baka pygmies might have used nets in the past, but do not carry out net hunting today (Hayashi Reference Hayashi2008; Yasuoka Reference Yasuoka2006b). In general, net hunting is declining and being replaced by snare hunting even amongst the Aka and Mbuti because of higher individual returns of snare hunting, greater involvement in formal employment and agriculture, and enforcement of regulations in protected areas (Noss Reference Noss1997). However, it has not been abandoned completely because of the growing demand for wild meat (Noss Reference Noss1997) and its suitability for illegal hunting, such as in communities adjacent to National Parks (Gandiwa Reference Gandiwa2011).

3.4 The Hunters

The typical profile of a hunter corresponds to any adult male with most hunters being around 25–50 years old (Bene et al. Reference Alvard, Gillespie and Alvard2013; Fimbel et al. Reference Fimbel, Curran, Usongo, Robinson and Bennet1999; Kamins et al., Reference Kamins, Restif and Ntiamoa-Baidu2011a; Martins & Shackleton Reference Martins and Shackleton2019; Pailler et al. Reference Pailler, Wagner, McPeak and Floyd2009; Pangau-Adam et al. Reference King, Adams, Carstens and Lefkowitz2012; Rogan et al. Reference Rogan, Miller, Lindsey and McNutt2018; Wright & Priston Reference Wright and Priston2010). Women hunters are generally very rare (Rogan et al. Reference Rogan, Miller, Lindsey and McNutt2018) but can sometimes contribute to traditional hunting by checking traps, contributing to dog and net hunting (Sections 3.1.2 and 3.2.5.5) and helping to make weapons (Duda et al. Reference Duda2017; Fimbel et al. Reference Fimbel, Curran, Usongo, Robinson and Bennet1999; Smith Reference Bliege Bird and Smith2005). Amongst Baka Pygmies, all shotgun hunters and most snare trappers are men but half of the traditional hunters can be women (Duda et al. Reference Duda2017). Except for the remaining hunter-gatherers, hunting wild meat is typically a secondary occupation alongside farming or employment and full-time hunters are rarely encountered, typically less than 10% (Ahmadi et al. Reference Ahmadi, Maman and Zoumenou2018; Bene et al. Reference Alvard, Gillespie and Alvard2013; Loibooki et al. Reference Loibooki, Hofer, Campbell and East2002; Pailler et al. Reference Pailler, Wagner, McPeak and Floyd2009; Pangau-Adam et al. Reference King, Adams, Carstens and Lefkowitz2012; Rao et al. Reference Rao, Myint, Zaw and Htun2005; Spira et al. Reference Antunes, Rebêlo and Pezzuti2019; Tumusiime et al. Reference Tumusiime, Eilu, Tweheyo and Babweteera2010). Socio-economic variables and a hunter’s personal profiles, such as skills, income security, number of dependants and physical ability, are typically correlated with hunting behaviour (Kümpel et al. Reference Alves, Mendonça, Confessor, Vieira and Lopez2009). For example, Rogan et al. (Reference Rogan, Miller, Lindsey and McNutt2018) report from South Africa that in contrast to other studies, households with some form of formal employment were 1.6 times more likely to hunt than unemployed households and seasonal employment had the strongest correlation with the likelihood of hunting. In Uganda, the probability for an individual to set snares in the forest decreased with rising levels of formal education and subsistence farmers were 10 times more likely to set snares as compared to those who had some other main occupation (Tumusiime et al. Reference Tumusiime, Eilu, Tweheyo and Babweteera2010).

Marlowe (Reference Marlowe2005) used the most extensive dataset of known Indigenous forager and hunter-gatherer societies worldwide contained in Binford (Reference Binford2001). The majority of these societies were located in sub-Saharan Africa, India and Southeast Asia, Australia, South America and North America. Marlowe observed that the percentage contribution of hunting to their diet was positively correlated with local group area and how often camps moved. Figure 3.7a displays the relationship between area size and percentage hunting, whereby area size is log-transformed, and percentage hunting was logit transformed (logit is the appropriate transformation for percentages). The correlation coefficient is r = 0.66 with n = 257, explaining 43% of the observed variance. Using the dataset, we also analysed the relationship between population density and hunting and found a strong negative correlation with r = -0.63 with n = 338, explaining 40% of the variance (Fig. 3.7). Local group area and population density are also negatively correlated with r = -0.90 with n = 258, explaining 81% of the variance. Fitting a linear model with both population density and local group area size as independent variables, reveals no significance for the former (p = 0.5) but significance for the latter (p < 0.0001). In other words, it is local group area size which best explains the percentage of hunting in the diet. Less land corresponds with higher input from agriculture and fishing.

Figure 3.7 (a) Local group area size versus percent hunting contribution to diet; (b) population density versus percent hunting contribution to diet.

(data from Binford Reference Binford2001)

In the wild meat literature, hunting by children is rarely mentioned (Bonwitt et al. Reference Bonwitt, Kandeh and Dawson2017). However, children learn hunting skills often from an early age and they also can actively engage in the hunting of wild meat. The vast cultural and environmental diversity of hunter gatherers results in a variety of learning and teaching subsistence skills during childhood. A meta-analysis of the anthropological literature has demonstrated several common trends across different societies (Lew-Levy et al. Reference Lew-Levy, Reckin, Lavi, Cristóbal-Azkarate and Ellis-Davies2017). Across these, learning foraging begins in infancy when children are taken to foraging expeditions and when they are given toy versions of the tools used in hunting, such as baskets, spears and bows. Children then transition through multi-age playgroups where social peer-to-peer learning is important, but they also continue to observe their parents’ subsistence activities and learn through participation. Small-animal hunting and trapping is primarily learned in playgroups. While children can become proficient at small-animal hunting relatively early in life, more complex hunting methods to take down larger animals are normally taught by adults, not necessarily the parents, from adolescence onwards. Learning to hunt big game may continue through life. There is a controversial debate whether teaching takes place in small-scale societies, including among foragers, but some research suggests that teaching rarely occurs (MacDonald Reference Albrechtsen, Macdonald, Johnson, Castelo and Fa2007). However, in many hunter-gatherer societies, hunting skills are generally taught through direct instructions, likely because of the complexity of hunting, demonstrating that teaching does indeed exist amongst these societies (Lew-Levy et al. Reference Lew-Levy, Reckin, Lavi, Cristóbal-Azkarate and Ellis-Davies2017). For example, San boys learn hunting skills through play, from five years old onwards, by imitating their playmates and not by overt instruction from adults (Imamura Reference Imamura, Terashima and Hewlett2016). While they learn the use and construction of tools through observational learning and trial and error, teaching is very rare and, if it happens, it is limited to older children assisting younger children in tool making. Similarly, Chabu children in Ethiopia start learning to spear hunt at young age, 6–7 years of age, through role-playing and collaborative learning with their peers; they also actively listen to stories and verbal instructions from their fathers and learn skills by active participation under guidance of adults, mostly uncles (Dira & Hewlett Reference Dira, Hewlett, Terashima and Hewlett2016).

Regarding active hunting, an example are older children and juveniles in the Serengeti National Park and adjoining protected areas, who hunt small animals irrespective whether they are boys or girls (Magige et al. Reference Magige, Holmern, Stokke, Mlingwa and Røskaft2009). In a survey of rural children in the Eastern Cape of South Africa, 62% of the children surveyed were supplementing their diets with wild foods by gathering and by hunting, predominantly by setting snares (McGarry & Shackleton Reference McGarry and Shackleton2009). In a study on Fang People in Cameroon, snare hunting in forest edges, agroforests, swiddens, fallows or home gardens near or within or near villages (so-called garden trapping) is an activity exclusively conducted by children (Dounias Reference Dounias2016). Children maintain the trapping and snaring knowledge, which is technically highly diversified, with little intervention from adults. Such hunted wild meat is generally eaten by the children as snack food and constitutes a valuable part of their diet. Also children of the Baka Pygmies in Cameroon participate in snare hunting not only for garden trapping but also together with adults during hunting expeditions in forest camps (Hayashi Reference Hayashi2008). Importantly, Baka children can conduct snare hunting by themselves whilst adult men devote days to spear-hunting expeditions (Yasuoka Reference Yasuoka2014). The non-overlap between children and adult hunting is also observed in village children in Sierra Leone, who regularly hunt from about 7 years of age, but focussing, in contrast to adults, on small animals using traps and nets (Bonwitt et al. Reference Bonwitt, Kandeh and Dawson2017). Boys were more likely to hunt than girls and they hunt alone or, especially for net hunting, in small groups.

3.5 Cultural Hunting Aspects

Hunting is shaped by prey type, density and distribution (i.e., the OFT, Chapter 4) and by cultural behaviours. Here we highlight the importance of food taboos and how costly signalling and taste of meat can determine which species are hunted and which are ignored. Taboos are considered here for they reveal ongoing changes affecting traditional hunting practices and the shift into more commercial hunting that not only deplete wildlife, but also deteriorate cultural systems that give a high value to animals. Another cultural factor is the taste of meat, which can bias prey selection contrary to the predictions of the OFT. Costly signalling in the context of hunting is hunting of prey that is dangerous or otherwise costly to hunt in terms of time, material investment, risk of failure and the opportunity to engage in more efficient foraging, but awards the specialized hunters an enhanced social status and reproductive advantage (Bahuchet Reference Bahuchet1990; Putnam Reference Putnam1948).

Food taboos and taboos against killing certain animal species were widespread amongst small-scale traditional societies worldwide. McDonald (Reference McDonald1977) reviewed food taboos amongst South American hunter-gatherers and found such taboos in all of the 11 societies under investigation. Food taboos are also reported from additional sites in South America (Jerozolimski & Peres Reference Jerozolimski and Peres2003; Redford & Robinson Reference Redford and Robinson1987; Rival Reference Rival1993), Africa (Hall et al. Reference Funtowicz, Ravetz and O’Connor1998; Inogwabini et al. Reference Hall, Saltonstall, Inogwabini and Omari2013; Jimoh et al. Reference Jimoh, Ikyaagba, Alarape, Obioha and Adeyemi2012; Wright & Priston Reference Wright and Priston2010), the Indian subcontinent (Aiyadurai et al. Reference Aiyadurai, Singh and Milner-Gulland2010; Velho & Laurance Reference Butler and Laurance2013), Inuit in Canada and Alaska (Burch Reference Burch, Harkin and Lewis2007), Southeast Asia (Tuck-Po Reference Akçakaya and Sjögren-Gulve2000) and virtually everywhere in the world. De-tabooing is widespread and reported in many studies, such as brocket deer in Central America (Hames & Vickers Reference Hames and Vickers1982), the tapir in South America (Rival Reference Rival1993), the bonobo in Africa (Inogwabini et al. Reference Hall, Saltonstall, Inogwabini and Omari2013) or the gaur, Himalayan serow and tiger on the Indian subcontinent (Velho & Laurance Reference Butler and Laurance2013). Taboos are often connected with religious belief (Meyer-Rochow Reference Meyer-Rochow2009). For example, Machiguenga hunters believe that certain monkeys (especially large adult males) and other prey species have vengeful spirits that can ‘take revenge’ on the hunter’s family (da Silva et al. Reference Da Silva, Shepard and Yu2005).This association between religious belief and taboos triggers changes and disappearance of taboos once people or societies convert. For example, a shift towards Christianity in Indigenous communities of Arunachal Pradesh probably led to an erosion of hunting taboos (Aiyadurai et al. Reference Aiyadurai, Singh and Milner-Gulland2010). When Christianity replaced the traditional system of beliefs about predator–prey relations in Inuit, the taboo vanished which previously significantly constrained the pursuit, processing and consumption of prey, vanished (Burch Reference Burch, Harkin and Lewis2007). In Africa, many who belong to a Western religion now see some of these taboos as satanic (Jimoh et al. Reference Jimoh, Ikyaagba, Alarape, Obioha and Adeyemi2012), demonstrating that the taboos have no inner connection with conservation. When taboos are based on religion, it does not guarantee protection. For example, in Cameroon the belief that people can transform into animals is widespread. Although 83% of those that believed in transformation did not shoot apes for fear of killing a person, there are others that had killed apes despite knowing about this traditional belief (Wright & Priston Reference Wright and Priston2010). Moreover, those that spoke of the belief were in the minority and the belief in animal totems has especially declined among the younger generation (Etiendem Reference Etiendem2008 cited in Wright & Priston Reference Wright and Priston2010). Erosion of taboos other than by religion is widespread. Jerozolimski and Peres (Reference Jerozolimski and Peres2003) have demonstrated that a shift from large to small-bodied prey species has taken place in the Neotropics and that taboos seem to play a minor role in determining the overall composition of target species. Increased consumption of meat previously avoided amongst the Yuqui community of Bolivia expresses how cultural attitudes change when preferred prey becomes scarce (Jerozolimski & Peres Reference Jerozolimski and Peres2003). Bonobos were not hunted historically in the DRC because of traditional taboos but the introduction of money, commodity hunting and commerce has caused bonobos to now be killed (Inogwabini et al. Reference Hall, Saltonstall, Inogwabini and Omari2013). This change might have been triggered by immigration of newly established ethnic groups that did not have such taboos. Finally, taboos might have spiritual or medicinal purposes, which have nothing to do with conservation. Alternatively, some taboos might have evolved as a means to prevent hunters from wasting time searching for game species that are difficult to find (Jerozolimski & Peres Reference Jerozolimski and Peres2003).

Taboos and the avoidance of some wild meat species have in common that the species are not hunted despite OFT’s prediction that some of these species should be exploited. For example, Machiguenga hunters in the Amazonian Peru prefer large primates while avoiding deer and ocelot (Shepard Reference Shepard, Fuentes and Wolfe2002); Piro in the Amazonian Peru never pursue otters, sloths, and pumas (Alvard Reference Alvard1993); and Kaxinawá and Katukina prefer peccaries while otters, kinkajous, rats, and marsupials are considered inedible (Kensinger Reference Kensinger1995a). Mayangna and Miskito hunters in Nicaragua generally focus on prey types that are in the optimal diet set and kill giant anteaters and northern tamanduas to protect their dogs, but they do not eat the meat despite both species being in the optimal diet set (Koster Reference Koster2008b, Reference Koster2008a). Mayangna and Miskito hunters also pursue the sympatric, similar sized, black-handed spider monkey and mantled howler monkey differentially despite both being in the optimal diet set (Koster et al. Reference Koster, Hodgen, Venegas and Copeland2010). Spider monkeys are predominantly frugivorous and howler monkeys are primarily folivorous, which may determine the different taste of their meat for humans (Shepard Reference Shepard, Fuentes and Wolfe2002). The taste of the meat of howler monkeys is unappealing to the hunters and the species is generally not pursued during hunts (Koster et al. Reference Koster, Hodgen, Venegas and Copeland2010). Moreover, several carnivore species including ocelots, pumas, jaguars and tayras are not pursued and eaten as they appear unpalatable to the hunters because these species eat raw meat which hunters associate with potentially harmful pathogens (Koster et al. Reference Koster, Hodgen, Venegas and Copeland2010). Similar to howler monkeys in Nicaragua, black colobus monkeys in Gabon are normally not pursued due to their meat’s bad taste despite their being easy to hunt because of their inactivity and large size (Brugière 1978). Nevertheless, they are heavily hunted if other, preferred species have become overhunted.

The pursuit of inefficient or expensive prey is a costly signalling strategy (Boone Reference Boone, Penn and Mysterud2017; Hawkes & Bliege Bird Reference Hawkes and Bliege Bird2002) that has been documented in many societies. Costly signalling theory has been applied to explain some seemingly maladaptive cultural practices, such as relatively inefficient or apparently suboptimal foraging behaviours, and generosity by the foragers’ motivation to broadcast honest information about their abilities (Hawkes & Bliege Bird Reference Hawkes and Bliege Bird2002). Spearfishing Meriam Islanders violate predictions of OFT when they bypass opportunities to harvest shellfish and focus on inefficient, thus costly, spearfishing instead (Bliege Bird et al. Reference Bliege Bird, Smith and Bird2001). These foraging decisions can be explained by the social status associated with being known as a successful spearfisher. The motivation underlying modern trophy hunting and big-game fishing, which are while rarely costly in terms of danger or difficulty but can be extraordinarily expensive, can be explained by costly signalling (Darimont et al. Reference Darimont, Codding and Hawkes2017). Turtle hunting by the Meriam Islanders (Bliege Bird & Smith Reference Bliege Bird and Smith2005) and torch fishing for dogtooth tuna in the Micronesian Ifaluk atoll are other outstanding examples (Sosis Reference Sosis2000). Differences between the sexes in the use of costly signalling have been documented. Bliege Bird and Bird (Reference Bliege Bird and Bird2008) showed that in Australian Martu hunters, women will optimize their hunting for consumption, while men will hunt to optimize their social and political status. Hunters involved in costly signalling can improve their reproductive success, enjoy larger networks of allies and/or trading partners, as well as increase their social and political standing (Bliege Bird & Smith Reference Bliege Bird and Smith2005; Smith Reference Smith2004; Sosis Reference Sosis2000). In prehistory there is some evidence that in some situations signallers (hunters) would have gained social and even reproductive benefits. This is the most plausible explanation for prehistoric dolphin hunting, as inferred from faunal collections from several archaeological sites in the California Channel Islands, and Baja California (Porcasi & Fujita Reference Porcasi and Fujita2000). The faunal remains reveal a distinctive maritime adaptation that is more heavily reliant on the riskier capture of pelagic dolphins than on near-shore pinnipeds, a phenomenon that cannot be explained by the OFT. Similarly, OFT does not explain the benefits accrued by big game hunters living in the whaling community of Lamalera, Indonesia (Alvard & Gillespie Reference Alvard, Gillespie and Alvard2004). Results indicate that big game hunting provides males a strong selective advantage. Harpooners, and, to a lesser degree, hunters in general reap substantial fitness benefits from their activities. Hunters, especially harpooners, have significantly more offspring than other men after controlling for age. Harpooners marry significantly earlier and start reproducing at an earlier age unlike other hunt group members or non-hunting participants – the technicians and the boat managers. These results are consistent with data from other hunting societies that show significant reproductive benefits for good hunters. Harpooners receive significantly more meat even after controlling for the effort they expend hunting, while at the same time suffer an increased risk of mortality. Some forms of collective hunting do not translate into larger quantities of meat for hunters than for the rest of the group, but this is compensated by obtaining other types of benefits such as, reputation and reproductive success. This public recognition may allow hunters to have more wives, who raise their children better (Blurton-Jones et al. Reference Blurton-Jones, Hawkes, O’Connell, Segal, Weisfeld and Weisfeld1997), as well as being considered a desirable neighbour and ally (Hawkes Reference Hawkes, Stanford and Bunn2001). Among cooperative Hadza hunters (Blurton-Jones et al. Reference Blurton-Jones, Hawkes, O’Connell, Segal, Weisfeld and Weisfeld1997), meat is shared almost evenly between all the households in camp. Men with a higher reputation have higher reproductive success. First, they tend to have wives who produce live children faster because these wives are more efficient foragers than other women. Second, older Hadza men with a high reputation have young wives, usually married after deserting older wife by whom they had fathered children, thus increasing reproductive success. Finally, the costly signalling theory might apply in situations where hunters elect to forfeit hunting profitable prey types if it is linked to social costs. Koster et al. (Reference Koster, Hodgen, Venegas and Copeland2010) argue it might be preferable to return home empty-handed than to be known as a hunter who must pursue species that are widely disliked because of bad taste of the meat in order to secure an adequate amount of meat.

3.6 The ‘Ecologically Noble Savage’ Debate

From the 1970s to the 1990s the issue of the ‘ecologically noble savage’ was controversially debated but the discussion has abated since the predictions from the thesis were tested using foraging theory for the prey choice by Piro shotgun hunters of Amazonian Peru, demonstrating that there is little evidence supporting the hypothesis (Alvard Reference Alvard1993). Moreover, the hypothesis has become more and more redundant because most hunter-gatherer societies do not practice their original lifestyle any longer. Thus, the debate appears dated, but we present it here for completion and also to introduce the related concept of ‘autonomous conservation’.

Indigenous, small-scale societies have often been portrayed as natural conservationists who ‘live in harmony with their environment’ (Alvard Reference Alvard1993). For example, McDonald (Reference McDonald1977) argues that food taboos facilitate conservation of prey species among South American tropical forest groups. Jean Jacques Rousseau’s romantic view of a ‘noble savage’, the Indigenous person who has not been corrupted by civilization, was extended and adapted to the concept of the ‘ecological noble savage’, the popular belief assuming that ‘primitive’ humans existed in a state of equilibrium with the surrounding natural resources (for overview, see Alvard Reference Alvard1993). Amongst biologists, the hypothesis gained support by Wynne-Edwards’ (Reference Wynne-Edwards1962) assumption of group selection whereby social species evolved adaptations that prevented them from degrading their habitat. Specifically, he assumed that hunter-gatherer remained in balance with their natural resources (Wynne-Edwards Reference Wynne-Edwards1965). Since, the hypothesis has been thoroughly debunked (Alvard Reference Alvard1993; Krech Reference Krech1999; Harkin & Lewis Reference Harkin and Lewis2007; Smith & Wishnie Reference Smith and Wishnie2000). For example, McDonald’s (Reference McDonald1977) argument that food taboos facilitate conservation does not hold as shown by the numerous examples outlined above. Nevertheless, the hypothesis periodically appears in the academic literature, such as in Stoffle (Reference Stoffle2005): ‘For tens of thousands of years, the people of the New World sustainably used and managed these very old human ecosystems. Those peoples who remained in place for long periods coadapted with their ecosystems causing a new ecological order to emerge – an order often accompanied by increases in biodiversity and biocomplexity.’ The strongest evidence against the ‘ecological noble savage’ hypothesis comes from the test of foraging theory on the prey choice by Piro shotgun hunters (Alvard Reference Alvard1993) and from mammal extinctions over the past 126,000 years (Chapter 6.1; Andermann et al. Reference Andermann, Faurby, Turvey, Antonelli and Silvestro2020). Foraging theory assumes that foragers will maximize their short-term harvesting rate rather than be driven by a concern for the sustainability of the harvest (Chapter 4). The data show that Piro hunters pursued prey consistent with predictions of foraging theory. They did not show any restraint from harvesting species vulnerable to overhunting and local extinction. For example, howler and spider monkeys, which are most vulnerable to extinction, were always pursued. One could argue that hunter-gatherers in South America had less time to adapt to the local environment and that, consequently, conservation behaviour should be strongest in Africa, the cradle of humanity. However, the widespread adoption of firearms and snares by Aka Pygmies, Baka Pygmies and San and the connected increased extraction rates and unsustainability of hunting speak against this notion. The exception is the Mbuti Pygmies, but all studies are from the last century and Mbuti readily adopted the more efficient net hunting after contact with agriculturalist neighbours (see Section 3.3.2).

Much of the debate appears to be centred on the definitions of conservation. It is important to distinguish proactive conservation from epiphenomenal conservation, which Hames (Reference Hames2007) defines as a consequence of a human population’s inability to cause resource degradation or a simple observation about long-term equilibrium with resources. In contrast, proactive conservation does not only prohibit or mitigate resource depletion and damage, but it is designed to do so. An extensive review suggests that proactive conservation amongst hunter-gatherers is rare (Smith & Wishnie Reference Smith and Wishnie2000). However, it is clear that many small-scale hunter-gatherers have extracted prey as sustainable levels, thus fulfilling the definition of epiphenomenal conservation. In general, this appears an epiphenomenon of population density where demand remains sustainable. Where effective population density increases, for example by hunting for the market economy through selling wild meat outside their own group, increased extraction rate occurs and unsustainability is the norm rather than the exception even within hunter-gatherer societies (Chapter 8). In other words, hunter-gatherer societies exhibit all the ingenuity and self-interest as societies elsewhere, except that they belong to the most disenfranchised people of the world (The Lancet Reference Anderson, Robson and Connolly2016).

Related to, but clearly distinct from the ‘ecologically noble savage’ hypothesis is the recently emerging concept of rural communities taking responsibilities for managing their resources in their own lands (Franco et al. Reference Franco, El Bizri and de Souza2021) and that recognizing Indigenous Peoples’ rights to land, benefit sharing and institutions is essential to meeting local and global conservation goals (Garnett et al. Reference Garnett, Burgess and Fa2018), often even forming alliances with conservation bodies (Schwartzman & Zimmerman Reference Schwartzman and Zimmerman2005). The new approach argues that ‘local people are effective in protecting large areas in a relatively natural state ‘ (Sheil et al. Reference Sheil, Boissière and Beaudoin2015), ensuring that autonomous management can lead to effective conservation. We discuss this in more detail in Chapter 8.

Figure 0

Figure 3.1 Comparison of main attributes of subsistence, commercial and recreational hunting.

(modified from Ojasti 1996)
Figure 1

Figure 3.2 Drawings found in Leang Bulu’Sipong 4 cave, south of Sulawesi, showing a buffalo being hunted by part-human, part-animal creatures holding spears and possibly ropes.

(from Aubert et al. 2019; reprinted with permission from Nature Springer and M. Aubert)
Figure 2

Figure 3.3 Backed tools from Twin Rivers.

(from Barham 2002 reprinted with permission from Elsevier)
Figure 3

Table 3.1

Figure 4

Table 3.1

Figure 5

Table 3.1

Figure 6

Table 3.1

Figure 7

Table 3.1

Figure 8

Table 3.1

Figure 9

Table 3.1

Figure 10

Table 3.1

Figure 11

Table 3.1

Figure 12

Table 3.1

Figure 13

Figure 3.4 (a) Asurini do Tocantins tribe member from the region between the Xingu and Tocantins rivers holding bow and arrow (WIN-Initiative/The Image Bank Unreleased/Getty Images); (b) Huaorani hunter from Ecuador using blowgun in forest (WIN-Initiative/The Image Bank Unreleased/Getty Images); (c) Bayaka or Aka tribesman holding spear in a forest in the Central African Republic (Timothy Allen/The Image Bank Unreleased/Getty Images); (d) Yangoru Boiken bat catcher retrieves a giant fruit bat from his net in the foothills of Mount Turu, East Sepik Province, Papua New Guinea.

(Timothy Allen/The Image Bank Unreleased/Getty Images)
Figure 14

Figure 3.5 Blue duiker caught using nylon rope snares in Bioko Island, Equatorial Guinea.

(photo: M. Grande-Vega)
Figure 15

Figure 3.6 Spear II. The 2.30 m long spear is shown to the left of an incomplete pelvis of a horse, and the base has been broken off. Inset shows a detail of the tip of spear II. Scale in cm.

(From Thieme 1997; reprinted with permission from Springer Nature.)
Figure 16

Figure 3.7 (a) Local group area size versus percent hunting contribution to diet; (b) population density versus percent hunting contribution to diet.

(data from Binford 2001)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×