Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T06:31:26.633Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - Intrinsic Temporality in Depression

Classical Phenomenological Psychiatry, Affectivity, and Narrative

from Part IV - Depression, Schizophrenia, and Dementia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 October 2020

Christian Tewes
Affiliation:
Heidelberg University Hospital
Giovanni Stanghellini
Affiliation:
Chieti University
Get access

Summary

Studies of depression indicate the existence of temporal abnormalities, particularly as related to the perspectival and agentive aspects of lived experience in persons who undergo depression. With reference to these anomalies, there is a long-standing view in phenomenological psychopathology, reinforced by empirical studies, that depression involves a disruption of “intrinsic temporality.” This view is widely incorporated into contemporary enactivist approaches to cognition, which link such a disruption to disorders involving affect, affordances, and narrative aspects of experience. We argue that classical phenomenological accounts of temporal abnormalities in depression do not warrant the supposition of a disruption to “intrinsic temporality,” and that this term itself is ambiguous. Instead, we understand depressive experience primarily in terms of affectivity, which does indeed feedback into the experience of lived time. Our view demonstrates a deeper consensus between phenomenological psychopathology and “4E” approaches, including work on both Gibsonian affordances and narrative, than is typically recognized.

Type
Chapter
Information
Time and Body
Phenomenological and Psychopathological Approaches
, pp. 289 - 310
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aho, K. A. (2013). Depression and embodiment: Phenomenological reflections on motility, affectivity, and transcendence. Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy, 16(4), 751759. doi:10.1007/s11019-013-9470-8Google Scholar
Binswanger, L. (2005). Mélancolie et manie: Etudes phénoménologiques [Melancholy and mania: Phenomenological studies]. Paris, France: Presses Universitaires de France. (Original work published 1960)Google Scholar
Blewett, A. E. (1992). Abnormal subjective time experience in depression. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 161(2), 195200. doi:10.1192/bjp.161.2.195Google Scholar
Bloc, L., Souza, C., & Moreira, V. (2016). Phenomenology of depression: Contributions of Minkowski, Binswanger, Tellenbach and Tatossian. Estudos de Psicologia (Campinas), 33(1), 107116. doi:10.1590/1982-027520160001000011Google Scholar
Bruineberg, J., & Rietveld, E. (2014). Self-organization, free energy minimization, and optimal grip on a field of affordances. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 114. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00599Google Scholar
Bschor, T., Ising, M., Bauer, M., Lewitzka, U., Skerstupeit, M., Mueller-Oerlinghausen, B., & Baethge, C. (2004). Time experience and time judgment in major depression, mania and healthy subjects. A controlled study of 93 subjects. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 109(3), 222229. doi:10.1046/j.0001-690X.2003.00244.xGoogle Scholar
Bucci, W., & Freedman, N. (1981). The language of depression. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 45(4), 334358.Google ScholarPubMed
Cardinalli, I. E. (2012). Daseinsanalyse e esquizofrenia: um estudo da obra de Medard Boss [Daseinsanalyse and schizophrenia: A study of Medard Boss' work]. São Paulo, Brasil: Escuta.Google Scholar
Colombetti, G. (2014). The feeling body: Affective science meets the enactive mind. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press.Google Scholar
De Haan, S., Rietveld, E., Stokhof, M., & Denys, D. (2013). The phenomenology of deep brain stimulation-induced changes in OCD: An enactive affordance-based model. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7(653), 114. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00653Google Scholar
Delafield-Butt, J. T., & Trevarthen, C. (2015). The ontogenesis of narrative: From moving to meaning. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 116. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01157Google Scholar
Depraz, N. (1994). Temporalité et affection dans les manuscrits tardifs sur la temporalité (1929–1935) de Husserl [Temporality and affection in Husserl's late manuscripts on temporality (1929–1935)]. Alter, 2, 6386.Google Scholar
Depraz, N., Varela, F. J., & Vermersch, P. (2000). The gesture of awareness: An account of its structural dynamics. In Velmans, M. (Ed.), Investigating phenomenal consciousness: New methodologies and maps (pp. 121136). Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Droit-Volet, S. (2013). Time perception, emotions and mood disorders. Journal of Physiology – Paris, 107(4), 255264. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2013.03.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fingelkurts, A. A., & Fingelkurts, A. A. (2017). Three-dimensional components of selfhood in treatment-naive patients with major depressive disorder: A resting-state qEEG imaging study. Neuropsychologia, 99, 3036. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.02.020Google Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2001). Melancholia as a desynchronization: Towards a psychopathology of interpersonal time. Psychopathology, 34(4), 179186. doi:10.1159/000049304Google Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2005). Corporealized and disembodied minds: A phenomenological view of the body in melancholia and schizophrenia. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 12(2), 95107. doi:10.1353/ppp.2005.0040Google Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2013). Temporality and psychopathology. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 12(1), 75104. doi:10.1007/s11097-010-9189-4Google Scholar
Fuchs, T., & Schlimme, J. E. (2009). Embodiment and psychopathology: A phenomenological perspective. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 22(6), 570575. doi:10.1097/YCO.0b013e3283318e5cGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, S., & Cole, J. (2011). Dissociation in self-narrative. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(1), 149155. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.10.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallagher, S., & Daly, A. (2018). Dynamical relations in the self-pattern. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 113. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00664Google Scholar
Gallagher, S., & Varela, F. J. (2003). Redrawing the map and resetting the time: Phenomenology and the cognitive sciences. Canadian Journal of Philosophy, 33(Suppl. 1), 93132. doi:10.1080/00455091.2003.10717596Google Scholar
Gallagher, S., & Zahavi, D. (2014). Primal impression and enactive perception. In Lloyd, D. & Arstila, V. (Eds.), Subjective time: The philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience of temporality (pp. 8399). Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1986). The ecological approach to visual perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (Original work published 1979)Google Scholar
Greimas, A. J. (1970). Du sens II. Essais sémiotiques [Meaning II. Essays on semiotic theory]. Paris, France: Seuil.Google Scholar
Heidegger, M. (1957). Sein und Zeit [Being and Time]. Tübingen, Germany: Max Niemeyer Verlag. (Original work published 1927)Google Scholar
Hsu, T. Y., Lee, H. C., Lane, T. J., & Missal, M. (2019). Temporal preparation and short-term temporal memory in depression. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 13, 112. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00258Google Scholar
Husserl, E. (1970). Logical investigations (Findlay, J. N., Trans.). London, UK: Routledge & Kegan Paul. (Original work published 1900–1901)Google Scholar
Husserl, E. (1991). On the phenomenology of the consciousness of internal time (Brough, J. B., Trans.). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Husserl, E. (2001). Analyses concerning passive and active synthesis (Steinbock, A. J., Trans.). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Jaspers, K. (1997). General psychopathology: Volume I and volume II (Hoenig, J. & Hamilton, M. W., Trans.). Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press. (Original work published 1913)Google Scholar
Junghaenel, D., Smyth, J. M., & Santner, L. (2008). Linguistic dimensions of psychopathology: A quantitative analysis. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 27(1), 3655. doi:10.1521/jscp.2008.27.1.3Google Scholar
Luft, S. (2008). Zur phänomenologischen Methode in Karl Jaspers’ Allgemeine Psychopathologie [On the phenomenological method in Karl Jaspers' General Psychopathology]. In Rinofner-Kreidl, S. & Wiltsche, H. A. (Eds.), Karl Jaspers’ Allgemeine Psychopathologie zwischen Wissenschaft, Philosophie und Praxis [Karl Jaspers' general psychopathology between science, philosophy, and practice] (pp. 3151). Würzburg, Germany: Königshausen & Neumann.Google Scholar
Mahlberg, R., Kienast, T., Bschor, T., & Adli, M. (2008). Evaluation of time memory in acutely depressed patients, manic patients, and healthy controls using a time reproduction task. European Psychiatry, 23(6), 430433. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.07.001Google Scholar
Maiese, M. (2016). Affective scaffolds, expressive arts, and cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 111. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00359Google Scholar
McTaggart, J. E. (1908). The unreality of time. Mind, 17, 457474. doi:10.1093/mind/XVII.4.457Google Scholar
Minkowski, E. (1970). Lived time: Phenomenological and psychopathological studies (Metzel, N., Trans.). Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Moskalewicz, M. (2018). Toward a unified view of time: Erwin W. Straus’ phenomenological psychopathology of temporal experience. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 17(1), 6580. doi:10.1007/s11097-016-9494-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Brien, S. (2004). The family silver: A memoir of depression and inheritance. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Paolucci, C. (2019). Social cognition, mindreading and narratives. A cognitive semiotics perspective on narrative practices from early mindreading to autism spectrum disorder. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 18(2), 375400. doi:10.1007/s11097-018-9575-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paskaleva, A. (2011). A phenomenological assessment of depression narratives. PICS – Publications of the Institute of Cognitive Science (Vol. 3, Master's thesis, University of Osnabrück , Germany). Retrieved from https://portal.ikw.uni-osnabrueck.de/en/system/files/03-2011.pdfGoogle Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W., Mehl, M. R., & Niederhoffer, K. G. (2003). Psychological aspects of natural language use: Our words, our selves. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 547577. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145041Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W., & Seagal, J. D. (1999). Forming a story: The health benefits of narrative. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55(10), 12431254. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-4679(199910)55:10%3C1243::AID-JCLP6%3E3.0.CO;2-NGoogle Scholar
Ramstead, M. J. D., Veissière, S. P. L., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2016). Cultural affordances: Scaffolding local worlds through shared intentionality and regimes of attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 121. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01090Google Scholar
Ratcliffe, M. (2008). Feelings of being: Phenomenology, psychiatry, and the sense of reality. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ratcliffe, M. (2010). Depression, guilt and emotional depth. Inquiry, 53(6), 602626. doi:10.1080/0020174X.2010.526324Google Scholar
Ratcliffe, M. (2012). Varieties of temporal experience in depression. The Journal of Medicine and Philosophy, 37(2), 114138. doi:10.1093/jmp/jhs010Google Scholar
Ratcliffe, M. (2013). The world of depression. Unpublished manuscript. Retrieved from www.academia.edu/6054108/The_World_of_DepressionGoogle Scholar
Ratcliffe, M. (2015). Experiences of depression: A study in phenomenology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rietveld, E. (2008). Situated normativity: The normative aspect of embodied cognition in unreflective action. Mind, 117(468), 9731001. doi:10.1093/mind/fzn050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rietveld, E., & Kiverstein, J. (2014). A rich landscape of affordances. Ecological Psychology, 26(4), 325352. doi:10.1080/10407413.2014.958035Google Scholar
Rottenberg, J. (2005). Mood and emotion in major depression. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(3), 167170. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00354.xGoogle Scholar
Slaby, J., & Stephan, A. (2008). Affective intentionality and self-consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(2), 506513. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.007Google Scholar
Solomon, A. (2001). The noonday demon: An atlas of depression. New York, NY: Scribner.Google Scholar
Stirman, S. W., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2001). Word use in the poetry of suicidal and non-suicidal poets. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63(4), 517522. doi:10.1097/00006842-200107000-00001Google Scholar
Straus, E. W. (1947). Disorders of personal time in depressive states. Southern Medical Journal, 40(3), 254259. doi:10.1097/00007611-194703000-00011Google Scholar
Tatossian, A. (1979). La phénoménologie des psychoses [The phenomenology of psychoses]. Paris, France: Masson.Google Scholar
Thoma, S. (2014). Karl Jaspers' criticism of anthropological and phenomenological psychiatry. In Fuchs, T., Breyer, T., & Mundt, C. (Eds.), Karl Jaspers’ philosophy and psychopathology (pp. 8598). New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Thompson, E. (2007). Mind in life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C., & Delafield-Butt, J. T. (2013). Biology of shared meaning and language development: Regulating the life of narratives. In Legerstee, M., Haley, D., & Bornstein, M. (Eds.), The infant mind: Origins of the social brain (pp. 167199). New York, NY: Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Wyrick, R. A., & Wyrick, L. C. (1977). Time experience during depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 34(12), 14411443. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1977.01770240067005Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×