Randomised controlled trials (RCTs) are considered the reference methodology to assess the beneficial and harmful effects of therapeutic interventions. The key aspect in RCTs is random allocation of individuals to competitive interventions. This procedure generates groups that are similar in terms of known and unknown characteristics (prognostic factors) (Cipriani et al. Reference Cipriani, Nose and Barbui2008). Similar groups of individuals who are exposed to different interventions can be compared at study end in terms of outcome measures, with the aim of establishing a relationship between the intervention under study and the outcome of interest.
In some circumstances, however, random allocation of individuals may be unsuitable. For example, it may be difficult to evaluate the efficacy of a set of clinical guidelines by randomising individuals to be treated according to the clinical guidelines or not. It would imply that each doctor should adopt and follow the set of clinical guidelines for those patients allocated to the intervention group, while simultaneously ignoring it for those allocated to the usual care group. Clearly, this approach is not only counterintuitive but also scientifically unsound, as doctors would inevitably be directly or indirectly influenced by the clinical guidelines even when treating the patients allocated to the control group, a phenomenon called ‘contamination’ across interventions. Other examples of interventions where random allocation of individuals may be inappropriate include organisational, educational or lifestyle interventions. In these cases, interventions naturally occur in groups of individuals, such as those followed by different doctors, medical practices, health care facilities and hospital wards. The groups may also be schools, villages or families. If interventions naturally occur in groups then the unit of random allocation may be the group or cluster, rather than the individual. Clinical trials that randomly allocate groups or clusters of individuals are called cluster randomised trials (Puffer et al. Reference Puffer, Torgerson and Watson2005).
Random allocation of clusters of individuals is graphically described in Fig. 1. A typical scenario involves groups of individuals followed by different doctors. In the example there are six doctors who are randomly allocated to two different interventions (new treatment v. control treatment). Doctor No. 1, 5 and 6 are allocated to the new treatment arm and will therefore provide this intervention to all patients meeting the trial entry criteria (in this example, eight patients per doctor). These patients will constitute three clusters receiving the experimental intervention. Doctor No. 2, 3 and 4 are allocated to the control treatment arm and will therefore provide this intervention to all patients meeting the trial entry criteria (in this example, eight patients per doctor). These patients will constitute three clusters receiving the control intervention. At the end of the trial primary and secondary outcomes will be measured on all patients, which means 24 patients per treatment arm.
Intuitively, random allocation of clusters of patients is different from random allocation of individual participants, with implications in terms of study design, analysis and interpretation (Cipriani & Barbui, Reference Cipriani and Barbui2010; Purgato et al. Reference Purgato, Barbui and Cipriani2010). The main implications of cluster randomisation are briefly discussed below (Puffer et al. Reference Puffer, Torgerson and Watson2003; Hahn et al. Reference Hahn, Puffer, Torgerson and Watson2005; Eldridge et al. Reference Eldridge, Ashby, Bennett, Wakelin and Feder2008).
(1) Generalisability. In cluster trials, this aspect is related not only to the characteristics of the individuals included, as in individually randomised trials, but also to the characteristics of clusters. These may be judged by analysing the cluster entry criteria, which means, in the example reported above, information on number of doctors contacted, approached, recruited and analysed.
(2) Concealment of cluster allocation status. A major problem with cluster trials is that identification and recruitment of individuals occur after random allocation of clusters has been carried out. It is therefore possible that doctors enrol patients without being blind to the allocation status, and this might introduce a potential source of bias, as the knowledge of whether each cluster is an ‘intervention’ or ‘control’ cluster could affect the types of participants recruited. Hence, the potential for selection bias within clusters is particularly high (Eldridge et al. Reference Eldridge, Kerry and Torgerson2009). In some cluster trials, in order to minimise the risk of selection bias, all individuals within a cluster are included, or, alternatively, patient recruitment may be carried out by professionals who are masked to the cluster allocation status.
(3) Comparability at baseline. Random allocation of individuals aims at generating samples of individuals who are similar in group characteristics. This is not the case in cluster trials, as the unit of allocation is the cluster and not the individual. In cluster trials, there are two levels, the cluster and the individual, and random allocation should ensure comparability at baseline for clusters only. It is therefore of paramount importance to check the baseline summary information for both clusters (where comparability is expected) and individuals (where comparability may not be straightforward).
(4) Blindness. The characteristics of the intervention under study in cluster trials often do not allow blinding those delivering components of the intervention and, similarly, do not allow blinding individual participants receiving the intervention. Although this inability to blind is a distinctive feature of cluster trials, it is possible to assess outcomes blind to allocation status, for example by employing outcome assessors who are not involved in the conduct of the study and are masked to the allocated interventions.
(5) Loss of cluster. Loss of individual participants is a compelling issue in individually randomised trials. In cluster trials, in addition to this possibility, it is occasionally possible that complete clusters are lost, and have to be omitted from the analysis. In the example reported above, it is possible that one doctor is lost and, consequently, the corresponding cluster of eight patients is similarly lost. Clearly, just as for missing outcome data in individually randomised trials, this may lead to bias. In cluster trials, the issue is of high relevance, as groups of patients, rather than individual cases, may be lost.
(6) Intra-cluster correlation. In the example reported above, outcomes may be compared between the group of 24 patients who received the experimental intervention and the 24 patients who received the control intervention. One compelling issue, however, is that it cannot be assumed that observations on individuals belonging to the same cluster are independent, because it is possible that participants within any one cluster may share some known or unknown characteristics, and consequently may tend to respond in a similar manner. In cluster trials, observations on individuals tend to be correlated. This has implications in terms of sample size and analyses (Campbell et al. Reference Campbell, Thomson, Ramsay, MacLennan and Grimshaw2004c, Reference Campbell, Fayers and Grimshaw2005). Unfortunately, many of these studies are incorrectly analysed as though the unit of allocation had been the individual participants (Campbell et al. Reference Campbell, Grimshaw and Elbourne2004b; Eldridge et al. Reference Eldridge, Ashby, Feder, Rudnicka and Ukoumunne2004). This is often referred to as a ‘unit-of-analysis error’. If outcomes are measured at the individual patient level, the analysis needs to be adjusted for potential clustering in the data. In contrast, if cluster trials are analysed as if individuals had been randomised, resulting p values will be artificially small, resulting in false-positive conclusions that the intervention had an effect.
Reports of cluster randomised trials should include additional information to allow readers to interpret them accurately. The CONSORT statement has recently been extended to cluster randomised trials, offering this way a useful tool for investigators reporting the results of cluster trials and for readers of cluster trial reports (Campbell et al. Reference Campbell, Elbourne and Altman2004a). Readers may adopt the CONSORT statement to check the quality of reporting, making an informed judgement on the potential risk of bias associated with cluster randomised trials.