Introduction
Stigma, described as ignorance, prejudice and discrimination (Thornicroft et al., Reference Thornicroft, Rose, Kassam and Sartorius2007), towards people with mental illness contributes to inequality (Phelan et al., Reference Phelan, Lucas, Ridgeway and Taylor2014), excess mortality (Laursen et al., Reference Laursen, Munk-Olsen, Nordentoft and Mortensen2007; Gissler et al., Reference Gissler, Laursen, Ösby, Nordentoft and Wahlbeck2013; Starace et al., Reference Starace, Mungai, Baccari and Galeazzi2018) and affects help-seeking behaviour (Thornicroft, Reference Thornicroft2008; Henderson et al., Reference Henderson, Evans-Lacko and Thornicroft2013; Schnyder et al., Reference Schnyder, Panczak, Groth and Schultze-Lutter2017). While there is evidence that mental health stigma in England has lessened since 2008, changes are still needed (Evans-Lacko et al., Reference Evans-Lacko, Corker, Williams, Henderson and Thornicroft2014; Henderson et al., Reference Henderson, Robinson, Evans-Lacko, Corker, Rebollo-Mesa, Rose and Thornicroft2016a, Reference Henderson, Potts and Robinson2020; Robinson and Henderson, Reference Robinson and Henderson2019). Newspaper coverage is one influence on public perceptions of mental illness: people exposed to positive stories about mental illness online and in print media are more likely to report less stigma (Thornton and Wahl, Reference Thornton and Wahl1996; Corrigan et al., Reference Corrigan, Watson, Gracia, Slopen, Rasinski and Hall2005, Reference Corrigan, Powell and Michaels2013; Klin and Lemish, Reference Klin and Lemish2008; Schomerus et al., Reference Schomerus, Angermeyer, Baumeister, Stolzenburg, Link and Phelan2016; Ross et al., Reference Ross, Morgan, Jorm and Reavley2019).
UK newspaper coverage of mental illness has been found to be more frequently stigmatising (Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Fatoye and Wibberley2013; Thornicroft et al., Reference Thornicroft, Goulden, Shefer, Rhydderch, Rose, Williams, Thornicroft and Henderson2013; Rhydderch et al., Reference Rhydderch, Krooupa, Shefer, Goulden, Williams, Thornicroft, Rose, Thornicroft and Henderson2016), portraying people with mental illness as hopeless victims or as perpetrators of violence and crime while neglecting discussion of treatment, recovery and personal experiences. Analyses from other European countries show similar patterns (Nawková et al., Reference Nawková, Nawka, Adámková, Vukušić Rukavina, Holcnerová, Rojnić Kuzman, Jovanović, Brborović, Bednárová, Žuchová, Miovský and Raboch2012; Aragonès et al., Reference Aragonès, López-Muntaner, Ceruelo and Basora2014; Ohlsson, Reference Ohlsson2018), finding that newspapers were likely to associate people with mental illness with stigmatising messages.
However, studies of UK and Canadian newspapers have shown improvement in coverage over the past decade (Whitley and Wang, Reference Whitley and Wang2017; Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Robinson, Krooupa and Henderson2018). Both countries have long-running anti-stigma programmes that include work with media companies, which, along with the broader societal shift in perception of mental illness, have likely contributed these improvements (Henderson et al., Reference Henderson, Stuart and Hansson2016b; Whitley and Wang, Reference Whitley and Wang2017; Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Robinson, Krooupa and Henderson2018).
In England, this programme is ‘Time to Change,’ delivered by the national charities Mind and Rethink, that includes social marketing (González-Sanguino et al., Reference González-Sanguino, Potts, Milenova and Henderson2019), community-level projects and work with employers, schools and higher education institutions (Henderson and Thornicroft, Reference Henderson and Thornicroft2009). Work with the media initially involved protesting stigmatising reporting, but now focusses on working with journalists, editors and writers, providing responsible reporting guidelines, workshops and a platform for discussion (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Robinson, Krooupa and Henderson2018).
The Time to Change responsible reporting guidelines (https://www.time-to-change.org.uk/media-centre/responsible-reporting) directly address eating disorders, self-harm and suicide. Other diagnoses are not discussed in detail. Previous analyses indicate that eating disorder stigma can be constructed differently by the media to that of other mental illnesses, but is still harmful (O'Hara and Smith, Reference O'Hara and Smith2007; Shepherd and Seale, Reference Shepherd and Seale2010; MacLean et al., Reference MacLean, Sweeting, Walker, Patterson, Räisänen and Hunt2015). Prior studies suggest that there is generally more stigma towards eating disorders than those with depression (Roehrig and McLean, Reference Roehrig and McLean2010; Ebneter and Latner, Reference Ebneter and Latner2013). This raises the question of whether coverage of eating disorders specifically has improved over time and how this coverage relative to coverage of other disorders has changed over time.
Other evidence suggests that the same question regarding coverage of schizophrenia should be examined, i.e. whether an overall improvement in coverage also applies to this diagnosis.
Surveys in several countries show that schizophrenia was associated with more stigmatising views than depression or bipolar disorder and attitudes have either not improved or worsened (Reavley and Jorm, Reference Reavley and Jorm2012; Schomerus et al., Reference Schomerus, Schwahn, Holzinger, Corrigan, Grabe, Carta and Angermeyer2012; Angermeyer et al., Reference Angermeyer, Matschinger, Carta and Schomerus2014, Reference Angermeyer, van der Auwera, Carta and Schomerus2017). Previous analyses of UK newspaper coverage during the Time to Change programme also focussed on mental illness as a single construct (Thornicroft et al., Reference Thornicroft, Goulden, Shefer, Rhydderch, Rose, Williams, Thornicroft and Henderson2013; Rhydderch et al., Reference Rhydderch, Krooupa, Shefer, Goulden, Williams, Thornicroft, Rose, Thornicroft and Henderson2016; Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Robinson, Krooupa and Henderson2018). However exploratory analysis (2018) indicated that a higher proportion of stories about schizophrenia were stigmatising than those about other diagnoses, in line with other studies of social media (Bowen and Lovell, Reference Bowen and Lovell2019; Li et al., Reference Li, Jiao, Liu and Zhu2020) and newspapers (Goulden et al., Reference Goulden, Corker, Evans-Lacko, Rose, Thornicroft and Henderson2011; Gwarjanski and Parrott, Reference Gwarjanski and Parrott2018; Bowen et al., Reference Bowen, Kinderman and Cooke2019; Ross et al., Reference Ross, Morgan, Jorm and Reavley2019).
This study examines longitudinal trends of mental health coverage in the British press since the 2008 baseline for the whole of Time to Change, and since the 2016 baseline for its third phase. In addition, we compare coverage of each of eating disorders, schizophrenia and depression with coverage of all other disorders and examine for changes over time in these comparisons. Depression is a frequently covered condition allowing comparison with schizophrenia and eating disorders. Our hypotheses build on previous iterations of this study (Thornicroft et al., Reference Thornicroft, Goulden, Shefer, Rhydderch, Rose, Williams, Thornicroft and Henderson2013; Rhydderch et al., Reference Rhydderch, Krooupa, Shefer, Goulden, Williams, Thornicroft, Rose, Thornicroft and Henderson2016; Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Robinson, Krooupa and Henderson2018): that there will be an increase in the odds that articles are anti-stigmatising, with a decrease in the odds that articles are stigmatising when comparing the findings from 2019 to 2008 and comparing 2019 to the findings from 2016. We will examine the variation in different stigmatising and anti-stigmatising themes reported on over the period from 2008 to 2019. We hypothesise that the odds that articles discussing depression were stigmatising would be lower than articles discussing other diagnoses; the odds that articles discussing schizophrenia or eating disorders were stigmatising would be higher than articles discussing other diagnoses and the trends relating to these three diagnoses would interact with the year an article was published.
Method
This study utilises data previously collected relating to newspaper articles published from 2008 to 2016 (Thornicroft et al., Reference Thornicroft, Goulden, Shefer, Rhydderch, Rose, Williams, Thornicroft and Henderson2013; Rhydderch et al., Reference Rhydderch, Krooupa, Shefer, Goulden, Williams, Thornicroft, Rose, Thornicroft and Henderson2016; Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Robinson, Krooupa and Henderson2018). The data for 2019 was collected using the same methods used in previous data collection rounds, to allow for direct comparison between 2019 and the previous years.
Search strategy
The Lexis Nexis Professional UK electronic newspaper database was used to search articles from 18 local and national newspapers on two randomly chosen days each month which referred to mental illness. We ensured that there was a proportional representation of weekdays and weekend reports were included in the study, as per the data collection protocol used for previous data collection rounds. Ten national mass-circulation (>1 00 000 copies/day), daily newspapers and the eight highest circulation regional newspapers in England at the start of Time to Change were used. Only one newspaper per town/city was used to ensure geographical diversity. The Sun on Sunday is used from 2011 onwards to replace ‘News of the World’ which went out of print in July 2011. Only print news articles were included in the sample to allow for comparison between the different data collection rounds.
The following newspapers were included: Daily/Sunday Telegraph, Daily/Sunday Mail, Daily/Sunday Star, Daily/Sunday Express, Daily/Sunday Mirror, Times/Sunday Times, Sun/Sun on Sunday, Guardian/Observer, Independent/Independent on Sunday, Birmingham Evening mail, Eastern Daily Press (Norwich), Evening Chronicle (Newcastle), The Evening Standard (London), Hull Daily Mail, Leicester Mercury, Liverpool Echo, Manchester Evening News and The Sentinel (Stoke).
Newspaper articles from 2019 were retrieved on 24 randomly selected days using the Lexis Nexis database (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Robinson, Krooupa and Henderson2018). The search strategy included 35 general and diagnostic terms covering a wide range of mental disorders and descriptions of mental health services (Wahl, Reference Wahl1992): The full text of articles in the selected newspapers were searched using the following terms (* = wildcard): ‘mental health OR mental illness OR mentally ill OR mental disorder OR mental patient OR mental problem OR (depression NOT W/1 economic OR great) OR depressed OR depressive OR schizo! OR psychosis OR psychotic OR eating disorder OR anorexi! OR bulimi! OR personality disorder OR dissociative disorder OR anxiety disorder OR anxiety attack OR panic disorder OR panic attack OR obsessive compulsive disorder OR OCD OR post-traumatic stress OR PTSD OR social phobia OR agoraphobi! OR bipolar OR ADHD OR attention deficit OR psychiatr! OR mental hospital OR mental asylum OR mental home OR secure hospital’.
Inclusion and exclusion criteria
Only articles that referred to clinical mental illness were included in the sample analysed, even if the reference was brief. Articles were excluded if they referenced a search term in a context unrelated to mental health (e.g. ‘the economy is depressed’); used in a non-clinical way (e.g. ‘Brexit is making me anxious’), or if a diagnostic or slang term was used metaphorically (e.g. ‘he is driving me nuts’). Articles relating primarily to developmental disorders (e.g. autism), neurodegenerative disorders (e.g. Alzheimer's disease), or substance use disorders alone were excluded as these were not the focus of Time to Change. Only articles published in the UK were included.
There was an increase in articles meeting inclusion criteria from 2016. A random sample of 50% of the articles for each day sampled in 2016 was coded and 67% of the articles for each day sampled in 2019 were coded. This ensured a similar sample size as for previous years and hence a manageable workload for coders.
Coding
Articles were coded using content analysis (Krippendorff, Reference Krippendorff, Barnouw, Gerbner, Schramm, Worth and Gross1989). Articles were first given a unique identifier derived from the date they were published, then coded for the newspaper of origin, diagnoses mentioned and the elements present in the article. An article may contain more than one type of diagnosis, or it may not contain any. If more than one diagnosis was present and discussed in different ways, the article was only coded for the dominant disorder. In response to the introduction of DSM-5, we included binge-eating disorder in the ‘eating disorders’ diagnostic category, which had previously been excluded.
Each article was read and analysed for the presence of specific elements, using the same coding criteria for previous work (Thornicroft et al., Reference Thornicroft, Goulden, Shefer, Rhydderch, Rose, Williams, Thornicroft and Henderson2013; Rhydderch et al., Reference Rhydderch, Krooupa, Shefer, Goulden, Williams, Thornicroft, Rose, Thornicroft and Henderson2016; Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Robinson, Krooupa and Henderson2018). The elements describe the primary and/or secondary message conveyed by the article. Elements were derived from a combination of existing literature about mental health stigma and reporting, and the process of inductive coding (Thornicroft et al., Reference Thornicroft, Goulden, Shefer, Rhydderch, Rose, Williams, Thornicroft and Henderson2013). Articles were also given a summary ‘overall’ code as stigmatising, anti-stigmatising, mixed or neutral. If an article contained stigmatising and anti-stigmatising elements that were given equal weight, the article was coded as mixed. If an article met the inclusion criteria, but none of the elements was present, the article was coded as neutral. If an article had a stigmatising element present, but this was overshadowed by anti-stigmatising elements, the article was only coded with the anti-stigmatising elements. Table 1 details the elements included in the analysis, and further details, including the coding framework, can be found in the online supplementary documents.
The researcher coding the articles published in 2019 sampled for this iteration of the study was trained in the same way as those who coded previous years, other than the codebook developer (Thornicroft et al., Reference Thornicroft, Goulden, Shefer, Rhydderch, Rose, Williams, Thornicroft and Henderson2013; Rhydderch et al., Reference Rhydderch, Krooupa, Shefer, Goulden, Williams, Thornicroft, Rose, Thornicroft and Henderson2016; Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Robinson, Krooupa and Henderson2018). All researchers were trained using articles from 2008 coded by the codebook developer and then coded another sample to derive the kappa value. The researcher coding the 2019 sample (RH) coded a sample of 92 articles from 2014 to derive the kappa value; this allowed him/her to discuss his/her results with the previous coder (CA) who had used the same sample. The agreement between coders was analysed using a κ analysis (Kirkwood and Sterne, Reference Kirkwood, Sterne, Kirkwood and Sterne2003), and when a score higher than 0.7, indicating substantial agreement, was obtained, the coder was considered trained. Areas of discrepancy or uncertainty were discussed with CH and previous researchers until a consensus was reached.
Analysis
First, the proportions of articles containing the various elements, diagnoses and overall category (stigmatising, anti-stigmatising, neutral or mixed) were calculated and compared. Univariate logistic regression models were used to estimate the odds that an article was stigmatising, anti-stigmatising, neutral or mixed in 2019 compared to 2008 and 2016 and the odds that an element would occur in 2019 compared to 2008 and 2016. Assumptions for the logistic regression models were checked for multicollinearity and linearity of the independent variables with the log odds was confirmed. Goodness of fit, outliers and appropriateness of the link function were checked using the deviance residuals. A Wald (χ 2) test was used to assess the overall statistical significance of the year variable as a predictor in each model.
Three logistic regression models were constructed, one for each diagnosis, to compare the odds that an article was stigmatising if the diagnosis was present, adjusted for the year published and accounting for the hypothesised interaction. A Wald (χ 2) test was used to assess the significance of the interaction between diagnosis and year published. Articles that did not discuss any named diagnosis were dropped from this part of the analysis.
Holm-Bonferroni sequential adjustments were used to account for 42 hypothesis tests: 14 individual elements plus four overall categories for the comparisons between 2008 and 2019 and 2016 and 2019; three tests assessing the association between stigmatising coverage and diagnosis and three tests describing interaction with year. The unadjusted level of statistical significance (α) was set as p = 0.05. All analyses were carried out using Stata version 16.0.
Results
The sample
The sample included 6731 articles, with: 880 from 2008, 794 from 2009, 626 from 1010, 694 from 2011, 1043 from 2013, 941 from 2014, 869 from 2019 and 880 from 2019.
Changes in stigmatising and anti-stigmatising coverage
The frequencies and proportions of elements and overall categories by year are shown in Table 2. Stigmatising articles accounted for 46% of the coded articles published in 2008, 35% in 2016 and 23% in 2019. Anti-stigmatising articles accounted for 31% of the coded articles published in 2008, 50% in 2016 and 59% in 2019.
a Data reported for 2008, 2009, 2010, 2011, 2013, 2014 and 2016 were collected in previous iterations of the study, using the same data collection protocols. The previous study iterations are reported in Thornicroft et al. (Reference Thornicroft, Goulden, Shefer, Rhydderch, Rose, Williams, Thornicroft and Henderson2013), Rhydderch et al. (Reference Rhydderch, Krooupa, Shefer, Goulden, Williams, Thornicroft, Rose, Thornicroft and Henderson2016) and Anderson et al. (Reference Anderson, Robinson, Krooupa and Henderson2018).
The results of the logistic regression models relating to changes in stigmatising and anti-stigmatising coverage over time are presented in Table 3. In support of the hypotheses that there was an increase in anti-stigmatising content and decrease in stigmatising content, the odds that an article was anti-stigmatising was 3.16 times higher in 2019 compared to 2008 (OR 3.16 (2.60–3.84), p < 0.001). There was a 40% increase in the odds that an article was anti-stigmatising between 2016 and 2019 (OR 1.40 (1.16–1.69), p < 0.001). Between 2008 and 2019, the odds that an article was stigmatising reduced significantly (OR 0.35 (0.28–0.43) p < 0.001). The odds that an article was stigmatising reduced significantly between 2016 and 2019 (0.56 (0.46–0.70), p < 0.001). In all cases, the Wald (χ 2) tests (reported in Table 3) were positive for the overall statistical significance of the year variable as the predictor in each model.
a Wald test assessing the significance of the year variable in the model, with 7 degrees of freedom.
*Odds ratio is statistically significant at the 5% level after Holm Bonferroni adjustment.
There was a significant increase in the anti-stigmatising elements ‘recovery/successful treatment of mental illness’ (OR 2.89 (2.05–4.07), p < 0.001,) and ‘injustice’ (OR 1.62 (1.21–2.19), p = 0.001) between 2016 and 2019, and a statistically significant increase in all anti-stigmatising elements except for ‘sympathetic portrayal’ and ‘prevalence’ between 2008 and 2019, shown in Table 3. There was a statistically significant decrease in all stigmatising elements except for ‘sceptical of seriousness’ and ‘problem for others’ between 2008 and 2019. There was a significant decrease in the stigmatising elements ‘danger to others,’ (OR 0.57 (0.43–0.75), p < 0.001) ‘hopeless victim,’ (OR 0.36 (0.26–0.51) p < 0.001) and ‘strange behaviour’ (0.53 (0.37–0.75), p < 0.001) between 2016 and 2019.
Diagnosis, stigmatising coverage and changes over time
As shown in Table 4, depression was the most common diagnosis and was discussed in 31% of articles in the sample; with 3% of articles in the sample discussing schizophrenia and 7% discussed eating disorders. Of the total, 32% did not specify a diagnosis, so were removed from further analysis, leaving 5142 articles.
a Total number of times the diagnosis is mentioned.
b Number of articles included in the analysis.
c “Eating disorder category includes but is not limited to Bulimia nervosa and Anorexia nervosa.
Note: Data reported for 2008, 2009, 2010, 2011, 2013, 2014 and 2016 were collected in previous iterations of the study, using the same data collection protocols. The previous study iterations are reported in Thornicroft et al. (Reference Thornicroft, Goulden, Shefer, Rhydderch, Rose, Williams, Thornicroft and Henderson2013), Rhydderch et al. (Reference Rhydderch, Krooupa, Shefer, Goulden, Williams, Thornicroft, Rose, Thornicroft and Henderson2016) and Anderson et al. (Reference Anderson, Robinson, Krooupa and Henderson2018)
Articles about schizophrenia were 6.37 times more likely to be stigmatising than articles discussing any other diagnosis (OR: 6.37 (3.05–13.29) p < 0.001) and a Wald test indicated that there was a significant (p = 0.01) interaction between the year an article was published and the odds that an article about schizophrenia was stigmatising. The results of the regression analysis are shown in Table 5 and Fig. 1a shows that in 2008, articles about schizophrenia were more likely to be stigmatising than those that were not, but this discrepancy became insignificant between 2010 and 2014. In 2016 and 2019, the probability that a stigmatising article was about schizophrenia remained comparable to the probability a stigmatising article was about schizophrenia in 2008 and 2009. Over the same period, the probability that articles about other diagnoses were stigmatising dropped significantly.
a Wald test assessing the significance of the year variable in the model, with 7 degrees of freedom.
b Eating disorder category includes but is not limited to Bulimia nervosa and Anorexia nervosa.
*Significant with 95% confidence after Holm Bonferroni adjustment.
** Odds ratio indicating that the diagnosis is associated with an article being stigmatising (baseline: neutral/anti-stigmatising/mixed).
Articles about depression were significantly less likely to be stigmatising than articles about other diagnoses (OR 0.59 (0.69–0.85) p = 0.018). The Wald test indicated that the interaction between the year an article was published and the odds that an article about depression was stigmatising was not statistically significant (p = 0.07). Figure 1b shows that the pattern of change over the study period was similar for both the depression group and the ‘other diagnosis’ group. These results are shown in Table 5.
There was no evidence for a difference in stigmatising coverage of eating disorders v. other diagnoses (OR 1.37 (0.67–2.80) p = 0.386). The Wald test indicated that the interaction between the year variable and stigmatising coverage of eating disorders was not statistically significant (p = 0.08). Figure 1c shows the pattern of this change and while eating disorders were discussed in a less stigmatising way than other diagnoses in 2008, this gap closed as coverage of other diagnoses improved.
Discussion
The study provides the first evidence of a sustained improvement in the discourse around mental illness in print media, following initial findings of improvement by Anderson et al. (Reference Anderson, Robinson, Krooupa and Henderson2018). The number of articles retrieved for 2019 was higher than most previous years, except for 2016, supporting previous findings that coverage of stories relating to mental illness is generally increasing (Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Fatoye and Wibberley2013; Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Robinson, Krooupa and Henderson2018). Thus, there is an increase and an improvement in reporting about mental illness, with a reduction in the proportion of stigmatising articles and an approximately proportional increase in anti-stigmatising articles.
Improvements in knowledge about and attitudes towards mental illness showed improvements since 2014 (Henderson et al., Reference Henderson, Robinson, Evans-Lacko, Corker, Rebollo-Mesa, Rose and Thornicroft2016a) and with the continuation of improvement in 2017 (Robinson and Henderson, Reference Robinson and Henderson2019) and 2019 (Henderson et al., Reference Henderson, Potts and Robinson2020). A similar pattern, albeit delayed, is seen within newspaper reporting: coverage between 2008 and 2014 showed no significant reduction in the proportion of stigmatising coverage (Thornicroft et al., Reference Thornicroft, Goulden, Shefer, Rhydderch, Rose, Williams, Thornicroft and Henderson2013; Rhydderch et al., Reference Rhydderch, Krooupa, Shefer, Goulden, Williams, Thornicroft, Rose, Thornicroft and Henderson2016), followed by a significant reduction in stigmatising coverage in 2016 (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Robinson, Krooupa and Henderson2018) that was sustained in 2019. There was a reduction in the proportion of stigmatising articles in 2013, but this change was not sustained and was not associated with an increase in anti-stigmatising articles.
While it has been previously shown that stigmatising articles effect population attitudes towards mental illness (Thornton and Wahl, Reference Thornton and Wahl1996; Corrigan et al., Reference Corrigan, Watson, Gracia, Slopen, Rasinski and Hall2005, Reference Corrigan, Powell and Michaels2013; Klin and Lemish, Reference Klin and Lemish2008; Schomerus et al., Reference Schomerus, Angermeyer, Baumeister, Stolzenburg, Link and Phelan2016; Ross et al., Reference Ross, Morgan, Jorm and Reavley2019), it is possible that the causal pathway is not unidirectional. The public may have had more access to positive stories about mental illness via the internet, often relating to recovery or treatment of mental illness, which may then affect their perceptions and the views of traditional journalists (Betton et al., Reference Betton, Borschmann, Docherty, Coleman, Brown and Henderson2015; Carmichael et al., Reference Carmichael, Adamson, Sitter and Whitley2019; González-Sanguino et al., Reference González-Sanguino, Potts, Milenova and Henderson2019).
The increase in stories discussing recovery from mental illness is particularly encouraging. Research suggests that stories portraying individuals constructively coping with mental illness can benefit others who are similarly struggling (Niederkrotenthaler and Till, Reference Niederkrotenthaler and Till2019; Til Reference Til2019). However, while social media and web-based forums can reach hard-to-engage populations, the lack of accountability in social media can allow the spread of misinformation about mental illness and cause harm (for example, through cyberbullying), to vulnerable people (Daine et al., Reference Daine, Hawton, Singaravelu, Stewart, Simkin and Montgomery2013; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Cox, Bailey and Herrman2016).
The finding that schizophrenia is associated with more stigmatising newspaper coverage is in line with other studies (Clement and Foster, Reference Clement and Foster2008; Bowen et al., Reference Bowen, Kinderman and Cooke2019; Ross et al., Reference Ross, Morgan, Jorm and Reavley2019). This study shows that the proportion of stigmatising articles about schizophrenia has recently increased. The disproportionate proportion of stigmatising coverage associated with schizophrenia could be for several reasons. Schizophrenia is frequently associated with violence and criminality when discussed in newspapers (Clement and Foster, Reference Clement and Foster2008; Goulden et al., Reference Goulden, Corker, Evans-Lacko, Rose, Thornicroft and Henderson2011; Aoki et al., Reference Aoki, Aoki, Goulden, Kasai, Thornicroft and Henderson2016; Rodrigues-Silva et al., Reference Rodrigues-Silva, Falcão de Almeida, Araújo, Molodynski, Venâncio and & Bouça2017; Gwarjanski and Parrott, Reference Gwarjanski and Parrott2018; Bowen et al., Reference Bowen, Kinderman and Cooke2019), either in a metaphorical or literal sense. Newspapers focus on criminality and mission to report topics that are ‘newsworthy’ may create a selection bias towards only publishing stories about people with schizophrenia that have committed a criminal act. However, reports of criminal behaviour can discuss the role of an individual's mental disorder in a neutral or anti-stigmatising way. Population prevalence of psychotic disorders is much lower than that of depression (McManus S et al., Reference McManus, Bebbington, Jenkins and Brugha2016). As not knowing someone with a mental disorder is associated with more stigmatising views (Henderson et al., Reference Henderson, Potts and Robinson2020), there may not be the same demand for sensitive, anti-stigmatising reports of schizophrenia in the way that there is for other disorders and people with schizophrenia may be less likely to be asked to contribute their experiences to stories due to this lower prevalence and unchallenged prejudice.
This study showed that articles discussing depression were consistently less likely to be stigmatising than other articles, consistent with findings that population attitudes to depression tend to be less stigmatising than those towards other disorders (Reavley and Jorm, Reference Reavley and Jorm2012; Schomerus et al., Reference Schomerus, Schwahn, Holzinger, Corrigan, Grabe, Carta and Angermeyer2012; Angermeyer et al., Reference Angermeyer, Matschinger, Carta and Schomerus2014).
While eating disorders are much less common than depression and other common mental disorders (Micali et al., Reference Micali, Hagberg, Petersen and Treasure2013; McManus et al., Reference McManus, Bebbington, Jenkins and Brugha2016), the reporting of eating disorders was found to be no more or less stigmatising than that regarding other disorders. It is discussed more than all other diagnoses apart from depression, indicating that despite the relative rarity of eating disorders, they are widely discussed. The reporting guidelines provided for reporters by Time to Change and other mental health charities may have also improved the quality of coverage relating to eating disorders, making them no less stigmatised than other disorders.
Strengths and limitations
A major strength of this study is that it is an ongoing longitudinal dataset, which is a detailed and consistent analysis of newspaper coverage of mental illness for over a decade. While adhering to the protocol developed for the initial round of data collection has limited the scope of this study (i.e. the exclusion of online news sources and exclusion of certain diagnoses), this consistency has allowed for an in-depth understanding of the way that portrayals of mental illness have changed during the period. However, while newspapers still play a significant role in shaping national attitudes towards mental illness, this influence has declined since this study started in 2008 as more people use social media as a source of news and information.
Headlines and photographs were not analysed in this study. The exclusion of photographs may have disproportionately reduced sensitivity of the study in identifying eating disorder stigma in comparison to other mental illnesses (Bowen et al., Reference Bowen, Lovell and Waller2020). The coding framework was, however, carefully designed, referencing a wide range of sources and the use of inductive coding to assess stigma in a wide range of mental illnesses, including eating disorders.
The analysis of changes in stigma associated with articles discussing schizophrenia was a novel addition to this study. However, further insight into the details of, for example, the variation in the stigmatising or anti-stigmatising elements was not possible in this dataset, as the sample did not have the power to support such a granular analysis. Further, we could not examine changes over time for all diagnoses due to low frequencies within the dataset.
The decision by the study to exclude articles relating to neurodegenerative, neurodevelopmental and substance use disorders further limits the scope of the study, although this omission was integral to the overall aim of the study to assess the impact of the Time to Change programme on the stigma associated with mental illness in UK media.
Implications for anti-stigma programmes
Our findings suggest that the work by Time to Change is associated with a reduction in the proportion of stigmatising newspaper articles about mental illness in the UK. However, a wide range of factors may have contributed to this change, interventions such as Time to Change must continue to work with journalists and the media, although the focus could be updated.
Specific guidelines about reporting on schizophrenia should be developed, as for those on eating disorders (Angermeyer and Matschinger, Reference Angermeyer and Matschinger2003). That the difference in frequency of stigmatising reports relating to schizophrenia and those about other disorders widening is cause for concern. As has been observed with eating disorder stigma, the stigma associated with schizophrenia can have different features to that related to depression, and this will need to be accounted for in future work by Time for Change (Angermeyer and Matschinger, Reference Angermeyer and Matschinger2003). To gain further insight, future evaluations of Time to Change could include outcomes relating to specific mental illnesses. Current interventions may not be helping all people with mental illness equally, so it will be essential to assess knowledge, attitudes and behaviours relating to different diagnoses.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S204579602000089X
Availability of data and materials
The dataset of newspaper articles and the coding dataset are available upon reasonable request.
Acknowledgements
The study was funded by the UK Department of Health, Big Lottery Fund and Comic Relief through their funding of the Time to Change programme. Graham Thornicroft included newspaper analysis in the Time to Change Phase 1 evaluation proposal. The coding framework was developed by Robert Goulden, who analysed article content for 2008 and 2009. Articles for subsequent years to 2013 were coded by Guy Shefer, Amalia Thornicroft and Danielle Rhydderch. We thank Sue Baker, Maggie Gibbons and Paul Farmer, Mind; Paul Corry and Mark Davies, Rethink Mental Illness, for their collaboration.
Financial Support
None.
Conflict of interest
CH has received an honorarium from Lundbeck for consultancy on an anti-stigma programme and an honorarium from Janssen for teaching on stigma to mental health professionals. RH, LP and CA have no interests to declare.
Ethical standards
None.