Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-29T11:23:04.107Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Semantic relations and Alzheimer's disease: An early and disproportionate deficit in functional knowledge

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 February 2009

Marcia K. Johnson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey, 08544
Allison M. Hermann
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey, 08544

Abstract

This experiment explored knowledge of four types of semantic relations (superordinate category, part, property, and function) in Alzheimer's disease (AD) subjects and age- and education-matched controls. Moderate AD subjects showed the greatest disruption on functional relations, intermediate disruption on part and property relations, and the least disruption on category relations; mild AD subjects showed a similar pattern but significant deficits only on functions. We suggest that the disproportionate deficit on functions reflects a greater cognitive complexity of functions than other semantic relations that renders them more vulnerable either to disrupted processing or to structural degradation of the network of associations among semantic concepts. (JINS, 1995, 1, 568–574.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Balota, D.A. & Duchek, J.M. (1991). Semantic priming effects, lexical repetition effects, and contextual disambiguation effects in healthy aged individuals and individuals with senile dementia of the Alzheimer Type. Brain and Language, 40, 181201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayles, K.A., Tomoeda, C.K., Kaszniak, A.W., & Trosset, M.W. (1991). Alzheimer's disease effects on semantic memory: Loss of structure or impaired processing? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 166182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonilla, J.L. & Johnson, M.K. (1995). Semantic space in Alzheimer's disease patients. Neuropsychology, 9, 345353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, A.S., Butters, N., Paulsen, J.S., Salmon, D.P., Swenson, M.R., & Maloney, L.T. (1993). An assessment of the semantic network in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 254261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chertkow, H., Bub, D., & Seidenberg, M. (1989). Priming and semantic memory loss in Alzheimer's disease. Brain and Language, 36, 420446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., & McHugh, P.R. (1975). Mini-mental state: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Francis, W.N. & Kucera, H. (1982). Frequency analysis of English usage. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Funnell, E. (1992). Progressive loss of semantic memory in a case of Alzheimer's disease. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 249, 287291.Google Scholar
Funnell, E. & Sheridan, J. (1992). Categories of knowledge? Unfamiliar aspects of living and nonliving things. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 9, 135153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gainotti, G. (1993). Mechanisms underlying semantic-lexical disorders in Alzheimer's disease. In Boller, F. & Grafman, J. (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 8, pp. 283294). New York: Elsevier Science.Google Scholar
Hartman, M. (1991). The use of semantic knowledge in Alzheimer's disease: Evidence for impairments of attention. Neuropsychologia, 29, 213228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hasher, L. & Zacks, R.T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In Bower, G.M. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 22, pp. 193225). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hodges, J.R., Patterson, K., Oxbury, S., & Funnell, E. (1992). Semantic dementia. Brain, 115, 17831806.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M.K. (1992). MEM: Mechanisms of recollection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 268280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M.K., Hermann, A.M., & Bonilla, J.L. (1995). Semantic relations and Alzheimer's disease: Typicality and direction of testing. Neuropsychology, 9, 18.Google Scholar
Martin, A. & Fedio, P. (1983). Word production and comprehension in Alzheimer's disease: The breakdown of semantic knowledge. Brain and Language, 19, 124141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, L., Price, D., AND Stadlan, E. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer disease. Neurology, 34, 939944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milberg, W., Blumstein, S.E., Katz, D., & Gershberg, F., & Brown, T. (1995). Semantic facilitation in aphasia: Effects of time and expectancy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 3350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, G.A. (1991). Lexical echoes of perceptual structure. In Lockhead, G. & Pomerantz, J.R. (Eds.), The perception of structure: Essays in honor of W. R. Garner (pp. 249261). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nebes, R.D. (1990). Semantic-memory function and dysfunction in Alzheimer's disease. In Hess, T.M. (Ed.), Aging and cognition: Knowledge organization and utilization (pp. 265296). New York: Elsevier Science.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nebes, R.D. (1992). Cognitive dysfunction in Alzheimer's disease. In Craik, F.I.M. & Salthouse, T.A. (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (pp. 373446). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nebes, R.D. & Brady, C.B. (1988). Integrity of semantic fields in Alzheimer's disease. Cortex, 24, 291300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nebes, R.D., Martin, D.C., & Horn, L.C. (1984). Sparing of semantic memory in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 321330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roy, E.A. & Square, P.A. (1985). Common considerations in the study of limb, verbal and oral apraxia. In Roy, E.A. (Ed.), Advances in psychology: Neuropsychological studies of apraxia and related disorders (Vol. 23, pp. 111161). New York: Elsevier Science.Google Scholar
Schwartz, M.F., Marin, O.S., & Saffran, E.M. (1979). Dissociations of language function in dementia: A case study. Brain and Language, 7, 277306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tversky, B. & Hemenway, K. (1984). Objects, parts, and categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 169197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warrington, E. (1975). The selective impairment of semantic memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 635657.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed