Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T00:50:41.121Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Positive symptoms and the organization within and between ideas in schizophrenic speech

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

Heidelinde A. Allen*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Clinical Research Centre, Harrow, Middlesex
Derek S. Allen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Clinical Research Centre, Harrow, Middlesex
*
1Address for correspondence: Dr H. A. Allen, Psychological Services, Department of Psychiatry, The Royal Free Hospital, Pond Street, London NW3 2QG.

Synopsis

The study examined the performance of positive symptom schizophrenics vis à vis that of demographically well-matched negative symptom schizophrenics and normals in organizing words into ideas and ideas into integrated spoken discourse. This involved applying a series of analyses to speech produced when describing pictures in an unconstrained way. The results showed that, for the most part, positive symptom schizophrenics organized their speech, both within and between ideas, as well as negative symptom schizophrenics and normals. The results did, however, show an increased tendency by the positive speech disordered schizophrenics to omit referents for noun phrases requiring referents. This constitutes a specific failure to connect ideas and does, at least in part, explain what makes positive speech disorder (incoherence of speech) unintelligible. However, taken altogether, the results do not support the extant view that positive symptom schizophrenics suffer from a general loss of control in producing speech.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1985

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, H. A. (1983). Do positive symptom and negative symptom subtypes of schizophrenia show qualitative differences in language production? Psychological Medicine 13, 787797.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, H. A. (1984). Positive and negative symptoms and the thematic organisation of schizophrenic speech. British Journal of Psychiatry 144, 611617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreasen, N. C. (1979). Thought, language and communication disorders. II. Diagnostic significance. Archives of General Psychiatry 36, 13251330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreasen, N. C. (1982). Negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 39, 784788.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bleuler, E. (1950). Dementia Praecox or the Group of Schizophrenias (transl. Zinkin, J.). International Universities Press: New York.Google Scholar
Bransford, J. D. & Johnson, M. K. (1972). Contextual prerequisites for understanding: some investigations of comprehension and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour 11, 717726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, N. (1944). Experimental analysis of schizophrenic thinking. In Language and Thought in Schizophrenia (ed. Kasanin, J. S.). University of California Press: Berkeley.Google Scholar
Chaika, E. (1982). A unified explanation for the diverse structural deviations reported for adult schizophrenics with disrupted speech. Journal of Communication Disorders 15, 167189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, L. J., Chapman, J. P. & Miller, G. A. (1977). The theory of verbal behaviour in schizophrenia. In Contributions to the Psychopathology of Schizophrenia (ed. Maher, B. A.). Academic Press: New York.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1977). Inferences in comprehension. In Basic Processes in Reading: Perception and Comprehension (ed. La Berge, D. and Samuels, S. J.), pp. 243263. John Wiley & Sons: New York.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1978). Inferring what is meant. In Studies in the Perception of Language (ed. Levett, W. J. and Flores d'Arcais, G. B.), pp. 295322. John Wiley & Sons: New York.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. & Haviland, S. E. (1974). Psychological processes as linguistic explanation. In Explaining Linguistic Phenomena (ed. Cohen, D.), pp. 91124. John Wiley & Sons: New York.Google Scholar
Cohen, B. D. (1978). Self-editing deficits in schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research 14, 267273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crow, T. J. (1980). Molecular pathology of schizophrenia: more than one disease process? British Medical Journal 280, 6668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerver, D. (1967). Linguistic rules and the perception and recall of speech by schizophrenic patients. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 6, 204211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feighner, J. P., Robins, E., Guze, S., Woodruff, R. A., Winokur, G. & Munoz, R. (1972). Diagnostic criteria for use in psychiatric research. Archives of General Psychiatry 26, 5762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldstein, S. & Jaffe, J. (1962). Vocabulary diversity of schizophrenics and normals. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 5, 7678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, C. D. (1979). Consciousness, information processing and schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 134, 225235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halliday, M. A. K. & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. Longman: London.Google Scholar
Hart, D. S. & Payne, R. W. (1973). Language structure and predictability in over-inclusive patients. British Journal of Psychiatry 123, 643662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kraepelin, E. (1919). Dementia Praecox and Paraphrenia (transl. Barclay, R. M.). E. & S. Livingstone: Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Krawiecka, M., Goldberg, D. & Vaughan, M. (1977). Standardised psychiatric assessment scale for rating chronic patients. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 55, 299308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawson, J. S., McGhie, A. & Chapman, J. (1964). Perception of speech in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 110, 375380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, R. & Maxwell, A. E. (1968). The effect of verbal context on the recall of schizophrenics and other psychiatric patients. British Journal of Psychiatry 114, 311316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewinsohn, P. M. & Elwood, D. L. (1961). The role of contextual constraint in the learning of language samples in schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 133, 7981.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maher, B. A., Manschreck, T. C. & Rucklos, M. E. (1980). Contextual constraint and the recall of verbal material in schizophrenia: the effect of thought disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry 137, 6973.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manschreck, T. C., Maher, B. A. & Rucklos, M. E. (1980). Cloze procedure and written language in schizophrenia. Language and Speech 23, 323328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manschreck, T. C., Maher, B. A. & Ader, D. N. (1981). Formal thought disorder, the type–token ratio, and disturbed voluntary motor movement in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 139, 715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morice, R. & Ingram, J. C. L. (1982). Language analysis in schizophrenia: diagnostic implications. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 16, 1121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, H. A. (1943). Thematic Apperception Test Manual. Harvard University Press: Cambridge, Mass.Google Scholar
Neale, J. M. & Oltmanns, T. F. (1980). Schizophrenia. John Wiley & Sons: New York.Google Scholar
Rausch, M. A., Prescott, T. E. & DeWolfe, A. S. (1980). Schizophrenic and aphasie language: discriminable or not? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 48, 6370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raulin, R. B. & Chapman, L. J. (1976). Schizophrenic recall and contextual constraint. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 85, 151155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rochester, S. R. (1978). Are language disorders in acute schizophrenia actually information processing problems? Journal of Psychiatric Research 14, 275283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rochester, S. R. & Martin, J. R. (1979). Crazy Talk. A Study of the Discourse of Schizophrenic Speakers. Plenum Press: New York.Google Scholar
Salzinger, K., Portnoy, S. & Feldman, R. S. (1964). Verbal behaviour of schizophrenic and normal subjects. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 105, 845860.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shimkunas, A. (1978). Hemispheric asymmetry and schizophrenic thought disorder. In Language and Cognition in Schizophrenia (ed. Schwartz, S.). John Wiley & Sons: New York.Google Scholar
Straube, E., Barth, U. & Koenig, B. (1979). Do schizophrenics use linguistic rules in speech recall? British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 18, 407415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wing, J. K., Cooper, J. E. & Sartorius, N. (1974). The Measurement and Classification of Psychiatric Symptoms. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge.Google Scholar
Withers, E. & Hinton, J. (1971). Three forms of the clinical tests of the sensorium and their reliability. British Journal of Psychiatry 119, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wykes, T. & Leff, J. (1982). Disordered speech: differences between manics and schizophrenics. Brain and Language 15, 117124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed