Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T07:58:19.328Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The relationship between personal and interpersonal mental health experiences and stigma-related outcomes in Hong Kong

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 April 2023

Stephanie Ng
Affiliation:
School of English, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
Hannah Reidy
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong; and Mind HK, Hong Kong
Paul Wai-Ching Wong
Affiliation:
Department of Social Work and Social Administration, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
Olga Zayts-Spence*
Affiliation:
School of English, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
*
Correspondence: Olga Zayts-Spence. Email: zayts@hku.hk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Previous research indicates that personal mental health experiences (e.g. one's current mental health status) and interpersonal mental health experiences (e.g. one's familiarity with someone with mental illness) are associated with stigma-related outcomes. These outcomes include knowledge, attitudes and desire for social distance from people with mental illness.

Aims

To explore the extent to which current personal mental health status and familiarity with mental illness predict stigma-related outcomes in Hong Kong.

Method

Data were drawn from a larger research project examining mental well-being in Hong Kong citizens. Citizens (N = 1010) aged ≥18 years were surveyed between August and September 2021.

Results

Multiple regression analyses revealed that immediate family and friends showed better attitudinal outcomes and lower desire for social distance compared with people who did not know anyone with mental illness (all β > 1.00, all P < 0.05), whereas people with personal experience of mental illness showed higher prejudicial attitudes compared with people who did not know anyone with mental illness (β = −0.744, P = 0.016). Better current personal mental health predicted lower prejudicial attitudes (β = 0.488, P < 0.001) and mixed outcomes on different realms of mental health knowledge.

Conclusions

Cultural concerns surrounding ‘saving face’ and emphasis on collectivistic values may explain the nonlinear relationship between personal and interpersonal mental health experiences and stigma-related outcomes. Future anti-stigma interventions should tailor their approaches to the needs of people with different levels of familiarity with mental illness and include efforts to support the mental health of the overall population.

Type
Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Royal College of Psychiatrists

Mental health stigma has been shown to have deleterious effects on the mental and physical health of people with mental health problems globally.Reference Volkow, Gordon and Koob1Reference Corrigan and Watson3 The issue of stigma has been a topic of concern in the mental health field since the publication of Erving Goffman's seminal work, Stigma: Notes on the Management of Spoiled Identity. Reference Goffman4 Despite such longstanding interest in this topic, there is a lack of consensus surrounding which mechanisms form and perpetuate stigma. On the one hand, stigma has been as an issue stemming from inaccurate or insufficient knowledge about mental health issues, or unfamiliarity with these issues leading to the labelling of people with mental health problems as ‘other’.Reference Walsh and Foster5 On the other hand, there is evidence to suggest that mental health stigma is not merely a product of lacking knowledge or opportunities for contact, with studies suggesting that even people with high levels of knowledge and intimate relationships with people with mental illness can endorse high levels of stigma.Reference Corrigan and Nieweglowski6 The effects of these conceptual inconsistencies are evident in practical efforts to address stigma. Specifically, despite the implementation of numerous large-scale, anti-stigma efforts, these interventions have only been found to result in short-term behavioural and attitudinal change,Reference Thornicroft, Mehta, Clement, Evans-Lacko, Doherty and Rose7 and the ‘active ingredients’ (i.e. causal mechanisms leading to these changes) remain poorly understood.Reference Jorm and Ross8

The relationship between personal and interpersonal mental health experiences and stigma-related outcomes

Existing research demonstrates that mental health stigma is not merely a product of lacking knowledge or opportunities for contact. For instance, studies suggest that the relationship between familiarity and stigma-related outcomes is better represented as a U-shaped curve, as opposed to a linear relationship, whereby the highest levels of stigma are observed in people who do not know anyone with mental illness and people with the most intimate relationships with these individuals, such as immediate family members and mental healthcare providers.Reference Corrigan and Nieweglowski6 Furthermore, a recent review study of existing anti-stigma campaigns has presented the argument that stigma may resist change because individuals are motivated to sustain perceptions of individuals with mental illness as ‘other’.Reference Walsh and Foster5 One way that these perceptions may be sustained is by drawing on subjective personal mental health experiences to justify these views, which allows the person to continuously revise their attitudes and perceptions based on situational factors.Reference Choudhry, Mani, Ming and Khan9

Taken together, these findings suggest that stigma is a complex and dynamic phenomenon that is meaningfully associated with factors such as personal and interpersonal experiences with mental health issues. Exploring the nuanced influences of these factors is, therefore, essential to enhance future theoretical and practical approaches to stigma.

The present study

The present study explores the relationship between two variables related to personal and interpersonal mental health experiences (familiarity with mental illness and current personal mental health status) and stigma-related outcomes (knowledge, attitudes and desire for social distance) in the context of Hong Kong, using a representative survey sample of 1010 citizens administered in 2021. This investigation responds to a research direction that a growing number of scholars have begun to advocate for, which calls for a greater focus on ‘the context and variability of health-related behaviours’ as opposed to ‘programmatic or unified theories of change’.Reference Walsh and Foster5 That is, it is essential for effective anti-stigma approaches to account for contextually specific factors that may influence how and why people have stigma.

Considerations surrounding one's influence and relative position within interpersonal relationships are particularly salient in Chinese-majority societies such as Hong Kong. Here, collectivistic values emphasise the preservation of one's interpersonal relationships, particularly with one's family members, which can have a significant impact on attitudes and behaviours toward people with mental illness.Reference Wong, Kong, Tu and Frasso10 Specifically, people with family members with mental illness are likely to experience associative stigma, which refers to the phenomenon of someone being discriminated against because of their relationship with a stigmatised individual.Reference Mehta and Farina11 Accordingly, we expect that immediate family members of people with mental illness in our survey sample will endorse the highest levels of stigma. Furthermore, research in Chinese contexts suggest that people with mental illness are likely to experience self-stigma because of the perceived burden on their family members.Reference Wong, Kong, Tu and Frasso10 Thus, we expect that personal experience with mental illness will be correlated with higher attitudinal stigma, and that current mental health status and stigma-related outcomes will be inversely correlated in our sample; that is, people in better states of mental health will endorse lower stigma, and vice versa.

Although previous studies have investigated the topic of stigma in Chinese contexts,Reference Wong, Kong, Tu and Frasso10,Reference Ran, Zhang, Wong, Yang, Liu and Liu12 to our knowledge, no study has explicitly explored the relationship between personal and interpersonal mental health experiences and stigma-related outcomes within a large population sample. Furthermore, although a considerable number of past studies have operationalised stigma solely in terms of its attitudinal component,Reference Oliveira, Machado, Fonseca, Palha, Silva Moreira and Sousa13,Reference Sandhu, Arora, Brasch and Streiner14 our study acknowledges the multifaceted nature of stigma by separately examining the influence of personal and interpersonal mental health experience variables on knowledge, attitudes and desire for social distance. We also retain the distinction between different familiarity categories during our analysis (i.e. self, immediate family, extended family, friend, colleague and do not know anyone) in contrast with previous studies that collapsed these categories (i.e. self, other, none) during analysis procedures.Reference Potts and Henderson15

Method

Data source

The population survey used in this study was administered as part of a larger research project entitled ‘Mental Health in Hong Kong: Assessing Mental Well-Being, Mind HK Programs and Resources, and Mental Health Literacy, Support, and Stigma’ conducted in collaboration with a local mental health charity, Mind Mental Health Hong Kong Limited (Mind HK; charity number 91/16471). The project aims to examine measures of mental well-being in the Hong Kong population, to understand the factors that affect these measures (e.g. mental health stigma and resource availability) and to inform the development of improved mental health support systems and resources. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. This project has been approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of The University of Hong Kong for the time period of 7 May 2020 to 31 December 2023 (reference number EA2006029).

Respondents and data collection procedures

Survey data were collected using a random sampling method to avoid selection bias. In this approach, two lists of telephone numbers were generated. The first list included randomly selected numbers from telephone directories, and the second list was created based on the first list using the plus-minus one-two method. Duplicated numbers were removed, and all remaining numbers were ordered randomly in the final sample. Approximately 70% of numbers were mobile numbers and 30% were landline numbers.

Social Policy Research Limited, a Hong Kong-based research firm, was commissioned to conduct the surveys. The target population were Hong Kong citizens aged 18 or above who were able to speak Cantonese, Mandarin, or English. Interviews were conducted anonymously by interviewers who had undergone training to administer survey questions. Telephone calls were made on both weekdays and weekends. Informed consent was considered given following the respondent's verbal agreement to participate in the interview. When more than one eligible household member was available for the interview, the member whose birth month was the most proximal to the time of the call was interviewed. Interviews were considered successful when at least 90% of questions were answered. Each number was called five times before it was dropped as an unsuccessful case. Data quality was ensured using on-site monitoring and voice recording procedures.

The survey included 56 questions and took 20–30 min to complete with Web-CATI (Computer-Assisted Telephone Interview) technology. The branching of questions was guided by computer programs. On-site validation procedures, such as range and consistency checks, were utilised during the interviews. Data collection and validation procedures were conducted using SurveyCake, a cloud-based survey solution for enterprises (SurveyCake, Taipei, Taiwan; https://www.surveycake.com/en/why-surveycake/enterprise).

The survey was conducted between 23 August and 15 September 2021. The brief period of data collection was determined necessary because of the availability of funding. A total of 4000 numbers were sampled, with 1987 invalid numbers (i.e. fax/data line, non-residence line, non-working line) and 2013 valid numbers. Among the 2013 valid numbers, interviews were successfully completed for 1010 respondents, with a response rate of 50.2%. Using a 95% confidence level, the maximum sampling error of percentages based on the 1020 effective sample size would be less than ±3.1%.

Sample weighting of age and gender was applied to the data before the analysis, to correct for underrepresentation of sociodemographic groups. These weights were calculated based on 2021 mid-year census data (i.e. the data collected most proximally to the time of survey data collection), as reported by the Census and Statistics Department of The Government of the Hong Kong Special Administrative Region.16

Measures

Measures of mental health knowledge, attitudes and behaviours

Three psychometrically validated scales measuring knowledge, attitudes and behaviours toward mental illness in community settings were included in the survey. All measurement scales have been empirically evaluated for reliability and validity for use in English.Reference Evans-Lacko, Little, Meltzer, Rose, Rhydderch and Henderson17Reference Evans-Lacko, Rose, Little, Flach, Rhydderch and Henderson19 These measures are the same as the ones used in the ‘Time to Change’ survey in the UK, a collaborative project between an anti-stigma initiative (‘Time to Change’) and King's College London.Reference Henderson and Thornicroft20 The decision to use the same measures was taken to ensure that survey findings were commensurate with international research standards. To account for the dominant languages used in Hong Kong (English and Cantonese), items in these measures were translated and back-translated into Traditional Chinese by the research team members. All measures have been validated for use in Chinese-speaking populations.Reference Li, Li, Thornicroft and Huang21,Reference Kuo, Hung, Huang, Hsu, Su and Lin22

The first scale was the Mental Health Knowledge Schedule (MAKS), which is a 12-item measure assessing mental health knowledge that is separated into parts A and B.Reference Evans-Lacko, Little, Meltzer, Rose, Rhydderch and Henderson17 Part A assesses stigma-related mental health knowledge (e.g. appropriate forms of help-seeking for someone with a mental health problem) and part B asks respondents to classify whether various conditions are diagnosable mental illnesses or not. Responses are indicated on a five-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Total scores on the MAKS can range from 12 to 60, with higher scores indicating greater mental health literacy. The internal consistency of the MAKS is acceptable (α = 0.65). Scores on parts A and B were analysed separately.

The second scale was the Community Attitudes towards the Mentally Ill (CAMI), which is a 12-item measure examining attitudes toward mental illness, evaluating endorsements of statements surrounding sentiments of social exclusion, benevolence, tolerance and support for community mental healthcare.Reference Taylor and Dear18 This scale can be separated into two subscales, namely the ‘Prejudice and Exclusion (CAMI-P/E)’ subscale and the ‘Tolerance and Support (CAMI-T/S)’ subscale. Responses are indicated on a five-point Likert scale from 1 (disagree strongly) to 5 (agree strongly). The highest possible total score on the CAMI is 60, with higher scores indicating more favourable attitudes toward individuals with mental illness. The internal consistency for both subscales are acceptable to good (α = 0.836 for the CAMI-P/E subscale, α = 0.729 for the CAMI-T/S subscale).Reference Sampogna, Bakolis, Evans-Lacko, Robinson, Thornicroft and Henderson23 Scores on the CAMI-P/E and CAMI-T/S subscales were analysed separately.

The third measure was the ‘Intended Behaviours’ subscale of the Reported and Intended Behaviour Scale (RIBS-IB), which consists of four questions assessing a respondent's willingness to engage with someone with mental illness in four key domains: living with, working with, living nearby and continuing a relationship with this person.Reference Evans-Lacko, Rose, Little, Flach, Rhydderch and Henderson19 The Intended Behaviours subscale has been used in past studies exploring the relationship between familiarity and desire for social distance.Reference Potts and Henderson15 These questions are scored on a five-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly unwilling) to 5 (strongly willing), and consist of questions about intended future behaviours toward a person with mental illness (e.g. ‘In the future, I would be willing to live with someone with a mental health problem.’) The possible range of scores for the scale is between 1 and 6, with higher scores indicating higher willingness to approach someone with a mental illness. The internal consistency of the scale is good (α = 0.85).

Familiarity with mental illness

Respondents were asked to disclose their level of familiarity with mental illness by indicating their most proximal relationship with someone with a past or present mental illness (‘Who is the person closest to you who has or has had some kind of mental illness?’). Responses were coded numerically in descending order of familiarity: (1) immediate family, (2) partner (living together), (3) extended family, (4) partner (not living together), (5) friend, (6) colleague and (7) do not know anyone. For the purposes of analysis, the ‘partner (living together)’ category was collapsed with the ‘immediate family’ category, and the ‘partner (not living together)’ category was collapsed with the ‘extended family’ category. The decision to collapse these categories was made in alignment with the ‘level of contact’ report described by Holmes et al,Reference Holmes, Corrigan, Williams, Canar and Kubiak24 which includes separate categories for people living with someone with mental illness, and people with a familial relationship with someone with mental illness but who do not live with that individual. Of note, people in the Hong Kong context usually live with their immediate family members because of high population density and limited housing space.Reference Wong and Chan25 For the purposes of analysis, we combined the ‘familiarity with mental illness’ variable with another question asking respondents to disclose whether they had ever been diagnosed with mental illness, and created a new variable with six familiarity categories, namely self, immediate family, extended family, friend, colleague and do not know anyone.

Current mental health status

Respondents were asked to disclose their current level of mental well-being (‘How would you describe your current mental health?’) Responses were scored on a five-point Likert scale from 1 (very poor) to 5 (very good). Current mental health status was treated as a numeric variable for analysis. Self-report measures of mental health have been used in past studies,Reference Moodie, Ilie, Rutledge, Andreou and Kirkland26 and responses to these questions have been found to be associated with clinically significant symptoms and access to mental health treatment.Reference Nguyen, Chan and Keeler27

Sociodemographic information

Demographic variables collected for the present study are gender, age, ethnicity and highest educational attainment. Gender was coded into a binary variable (female/male). Age was coded into six categories (18–19, 20–29, 30–39, 40–49, 50–59 and ≥60 years). Ethnicity was coded into two categories (Chinese or non-Chinese). Educational attainment was coded into seven categories (no schooling, primary, lower secondary, upper secondary, college diploma/certificate/associate degree, Bachelor's degree and Postgraduate). The ‘no schooling’ and ‘primary’ categories were collapsed into one category (‘primary and below’) for the purposes of analysis because of the small sample size of the ‘no schooling’ category (n = 6).

Analysis

Descriptive statistics were first generated to explore the characteristics of the survey sample, reporting unweighted frequencies and weighted percentages for sociodemographic and personal mental health experience variables. Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests of normality were performed to determine whether subgroup scores for each outcome measure were normally distributed. These tests demonstrated that scores for most subgroups were not normally distributed (P < 0.001). Thus, the median was selected as the most appropriate measure of central tendency,Reference Sainani28 and was reported along with s.d. and 95% confidence intervals for scores on each of the five stigma measures across subgroups of age, gender, educational attainment and the three personal mental health experience variables. These details were not reported for ethnicity subgroups because of the small sample size of non-Chinese respondents (n = 1).

Multiple regression analysis was used to examine the relationship between the two independent variables (familiarity with mental illness and current mental health status) and five stigma measures, controlling for age, gender and educational attainment. In block 1, age, gender and educational attainment were inputted as control variables. For the ‘familiarity with mental illness’ variable, dummy variables representing each of the subcategories were inputted in block 2, with the ‘do not know anyone’ category designated as the reference group. This category was selected as the reference group in line with previous literature that this group represents the lowest level of familiarity,Reference Holmes, Corrigan, Williams, Canar and Kubiak24 and constitutes a baseline from which other familiarity groups can be compared against. ‘Current mental health status’ was treated as a numeric variable and inputted directly into block 2.

Statistical significance was evaluated using two-sided tests with a cut-off P-value of 0.05. Adjusted R 2 was used as the measure of effect size, indicating the degree to which the overall regression model explained the amount of variance in stigma measure scores. Unstandardised β-coefficients were used to examine the strength of the effect of each predictor variable (i.e. subcategories of personal mental health variables) on stigma measure scores. Statistical analyses were performed with SPSS for Mac OS version 27.

Results

Unweighted frequencies and weighted percentages of the sample across sociodemographic and personal mental health experience variables, as well as the median, s.d. and 95% confidence interval for each of the five stigma measures, are reported in Table 1. The majority of the sample identified as ethnically Chinese (99.8%, n = 1009). Most of the sample had completed upper secondary school or above (73.4%, n = 818). A vast majority of individuals indicated that they did not know anyone with a mental illness (70.1%, n = 701), and a notable portion of the sample reported personal experience with mental illness (15.4%, n = 157). Almost 80% of respondents reported that their current mental health status was ‘okay’ to ‘very good’ (79.2%, n = 816).

Table 1 Descriptive statistics for scores on stigma measures across sociodemographic and personal and interpersonal mental health experience variable subgroups

RIBS-IB, ‘Intended Behaviours’ subscale of the Reported and Intended Behaviour Scale; CAMI-P/E, ‘Prejudice and Exclusion’ subscale of the Community Attitudes towards the Mentally Ill; CAMI-T/S, ‘Tolerance and Support’ subscale of the Community Attitudes towards the Mentally Ill; MAKS, Mental Health Knowledge Schedule.

Table 2 shows the results of multiple regression analyses used to examine the relationship between each of the personal and interpersonal mental health experience variables and each of the five stigma measures, controlling for age, gender and educational attainment. Although all regression models were highly significant (P < 0.001), effect sizes for these models were small (R2 adjusted = 0.021–0.095). However, unstandardised β-coefficients indicated statistically significant relationships between the independent variables (familiarity with mental illness and current mental health status) and stigma-related outcome scores.

Table 2 Results of multiple regression analyses of the relationship between personal and interpersonal mental health experience variables and stigma outcome measures, controlling for age, gender and educational attainment

P-values are two sided. RIBS-IB, ‘Intended Behaviours’ subscale of the Reported and Intended Behaviour Scale; CAMI-P/E, ‘Prejudice and Exclusion’ subscale of the Community Attitudes towards the Mentally Ill; CAMI-T/S, ‘Tolerance and Support’ subscale of the Community Attitudes towards the Mentally Ill; MAKS, Mental Health Knowledge Schedule.

*P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.

Familiarity with mental illness significantly predicted variance in desire for social distance (RIBS-IB subscale), prejudice and exclusion attitudes (CAMI-P/E) and tolerance and support attitudes (CAMI-T/S), but not in levels of mental health knowledge (MAKS parts A and B). Individuals with immediate family members with mental illness showed lower desire for social distance (β = 1.991, P < 0.001), lower prejudice and exclusion attitudes (β = 1.110, P = 0.012) and higher tolerance and support attitudes (β = 1.368, P = 0.001) compared with the reference group (do not know anyone). Individuals with friends with mental illness showed lower desire for social distance (β = 1.790, P < 0.001) and higher tolerance and support attitudes compared with the reference group (β = 1.048, P = 0.008). People with personal experience of mental illness showed higher prejudice and exclusion attitudes compared with the reference group (β = −0.744, P = 0.016). The overall regression models testing the relationship between familiarity with mental illness and mental health knowledge (MAKS parts A and B) were significant, but the β-coefficients indicated that variance in scores was better explained by differences in educational attainment (P < 0.001).

Current mental health status significantly predicted variance in scores on prejudice and exclusion attitudes (CAMI-P/E) and mental health knowledge (MAKS parts A and B), but not on tolerance and support attitudes (CAMI-T/S) or desire for social distance (RIBS-IB). Higher levels of current mental health were associated with lower prejudice and exclusion attitudes (β = 0.488, P < 0.001), higher scores on the MAKS part A (β = 0.205, P = 0.008) and lower scores on the MAKS part B (β = −0.170, P = 0.021). The overall regression models testing the relationship between current mental health status and desire for social distance (RIBS-IB) and tolerance and support attitudes (CAMI-T/S) were significant, but the β-coefficients indicated that variance in scores was better explained by differences in educational attainment (P < 0.001).

Discussion

The present study explored the relationship between personal and interpersonal mental health experiences and stigma-related outcomes in a large population sample, thus contributing to conceptual and practical approaches to mental health stigma. In contrast with results from the Attitudes to Mental Illness Survey conducted in the UK, which showed that higher familiarity is associated with improved stigma-related outcomes,Reference Potts and Henderson15 our findings suggest a more nuanced relationship whereby immediate family members and friends of people with mental illness show lower levels of stigma, whereas people with personal experience of mental illness show significantly higher levels of stigma, compared with people who do not know anyone with mental illness. Our analysis of current mental health status and stigma-related outcomes suggest that better mental health status predicts lower prejudice and exclusion attitudes and mixed results on measures of mental health knowledge. Finally, although not the focus of our analysis, we found that educational attainment is a significant predictor of all stigma-related outcomes explored here. Overall, our findings indicate that the relationship between personal and interpersonal mental health experiences is not linear, and that future anti-stigma efforts would benefit from addressing the nuances of this relationship.

Contrary to our hypothesis that individuals with immediate family members with mental illness exhibit high levels of stigma because of factors such as associative stigma, our findings demonstrated that immediate family members have a lower desire for social distance, lower prejudice and exclusion attitudes and higher tolerance and support attitudes compared with people who do not know anyone with mental illness. Although it is possible that these findings corroborate previous findings that greater familiarity predicts lower stigma,Reference Potts and Henderson15 another possible explanation is that these favourable responses reflect cultural values emphasising ‘saving face’ (i.e. preserving a respectable image in front of others).Reference Hwang29 That is, these values may motivate immediate family members of people with mental illness to report more positive attitudes and behaviours toward people with mental illness, to protect the reputation of family members as well as their family name.

Our study also showed that intergroup differences in attitudes and desire for social distance are independent of levels of mental health knowledge, which were not significantly different between individuals from different familiarity categories. These findings are in line with past research on the relationship between contact and decreased prejudice, which indicates that increased empathy and reduced anxiety are more potent mediators of this relationship compared with increased knowledge about individuals in the outgroup.Reference Pettigrew and Tropp30 It is also aligned with the self-disclosure literature, which suggests that disclosing personal information and one's emotions (in contrast with merely sharing facts about oneself) are essential ingredients in developing relational intimacy and social connectedness.Reference Luo and Hancock31 In the context of anti-stigma interventions, fostering a sense of familiarity and emotional intimacy can help target audience members to appreciate the ‘essential humanity of the individual with mental illness’.Reference Corrigan and Nieweglowski6

In support of our hypothesis, we found that personal experience with mental illness is associated with stronger attitudes of prejudice and exclusion. This is in line with previous research on self-stigma, which refers to the tendency for people with personal experiences of mental illness to internalise stigmatising attitudes and beliefs at the cost of their own well-being.Reference Corrigan, Watson and Barr32 Self-stigma may be prevalent in Hong Kong because of frequent experiences of mental health-related discrimination in interpersonal settings,Reference Lee, Lee, Chiu and Kleinman33 combined with collectivistic values that increase the likelihood of one's internalisation of societal expectations and attitudes.Reference Young and Ng34

In partial support of our hypothesis that current mental health status would be inversely correlated with stigma-related outcomes, our findings indicate that better personal mental health is associated with lower levels of prejudice and exclusion attitudes, and mixed results on subscales of mental health knowledge. In line with previous research that people with poor mental health may hold stronger attitudes of therapeutic pessimism,Reference Potts and Henderson15 our findings suggest that better personal mental health is associated with a stronger belief that people with mental illness can recover and reintegrate into the community (as reflected in lower prejudice and exclusion attitudes). It is also likely that this relationship is bidirectional: people with greater confidence that recovery from mental illness is possible may be more likely to access mental health support resources themselves, thus reporting better states of current mental health.

Our findings regarding prejudice and exclusion attitudes can also explain our mixed findings surrounding the relationship between current mental health status and different forms of mental health knowledge. Specifically, the MAKS part A subscale assesses mental health knowledge related to the stigma of mental illness (e.g. whether people with mental illness can fully recover, whether most people with mental illness seek therapy), whereas the MAKS part B subscale assesses knowledge surrounding mental health diagnoses (i.e. whether people can accurately identify names of mental illness diagnostic categories). These findings suggest that although current mental health status is not associated with levels of factual knowledge surrounding mental illness, it is associated with a person's awareness of mental health stigma within the community.

Finally, our finding that educational attainment is a significant predictor of stigma-related outcomes is aligned with previous research.Reference Lo, Suen, Chan, Sum, Charlton and Hui35 Previous findings have been equivocal, with some studies showing that lower education levels predict worse stigma-related outcomes,Reference Girma, Tesfaye, Froeschl, Möller-Leimkühler, Müller and Dehning36 and others showing that stigma-related outcomes are worse among people with higher levels of education.Reference Chan, Tam, Lee, Hui, Chang and Lee37 Of note, previous research has discussed the role of cultural differences in explaining the differential influences of educational attainment on stigma-related outcomes,Reference Lo, Suen, Chan, Sum, Charlton and Hui35 further underlining a need for future research to explore the relationship between culture and other correlates of stigma-related outcomes.

These findings suggest the need for anti-stigma interventions to use different approaches for populations with different levels of familiarity with mental illness.Reference Corrigan and Nieweglowski6 Interventions targeting the general public should aim to increase a sense of familiarity and emotional intimacy with people with mental illness, such as through facilitating positive and meaningful interactions with these individuals. Given the possibility that immediate family members of people with mental illness may be suppressing experiences of associative stigma or caregiver burden because of concerns surrounding 'saving face', it is also important for anti-stigma efforts to advocate for the development of supportive resources to address the affective and practical burdens affecting this population.Reference Tsang, Tam, Chan and Chang38 Anti-stigma efforts targeted toward people with personal experiences of mental illness should support them in developing a sense of self beyond their diagnostic labels, such as by giving them opportunities to share their stories of lived experience in supportive contexts.Reference Nurser, Rushworth, Shakespeare and Williams39 Finally, given that current mental health status can have a notable influence on stigma-related outcomes, efforts to support the mental health of the general population should be considered an indispensable component of the anti-stigma agenda.

Limitations and future directions

Several limitations of the present study can be noted. The ethnic distribution of our sample was biased toward ethnically Chinese respondents (99.8%), and is thus not representative of the overall Hong Kong population, which is composed of 91.6% ethnically Chinese individuals.16 Our study also only focused on two possible types of personal and interpersonal mental health experiences, which may explain the small adjusted R 2 values of our regression models despite statistically significant β-coefficients. There were likely other explanatory factors at play that we were not able to explore in the present study.

Another limitation is that we did not conduct pairwise comparisons between familiarity groups, which may explain the insignificant results observed for certain groups (i.e. extended family and colleagues) in the regression model testing the relationship between familiarity with mental illness and stigma-related outcomes. That is, the differences between individual groups may have been overshadowed by larger effects within the overall regression model. Thus, future research should aim to conduct such pairwise comparisons (e.g. between family and friends, friends and colleagues, immediate and extended family members) so as to continue building a more nuanced depiction of the relationship between familiarity with mental illness and stigma-related outcomes. The cross-sectional design of the survey used in this study is also a limitation, as it does not allow for causal interpretations to be made from the current data. Finally, since self-report questions were used to inquire about respondents’ personal mental health experiences and mental health knowledge, attitudes and behavioural intentions, it is possible that social desirability and response biases affected the authenticity of responses. Future studies can use semi-structured interviews and focus groups to explore the rationales for participants’ responses via multiple-choice questions, and to explore findings that were contrary to our hypotheses (e.g. immediate family members showing lower levels of stigma).

Despite its limitations, our study meaningfully expands on existing theoretical and practical perspectives on mental health stigma. Moreover, although the details of study findings are specific to Hong Kong, the implications of these findings are applicable to global contexts. From a theoretical standpoint, we found that levels of mental health knowledge can operate independently from mental health attitudes and desire for social distance, and that factors such as personal and interpersonal mental health experiences and cultural norms can be associated with stigma-related outcomes. Future research should continue exploring how different factors operating at individual, interpersonal and societal levels affects different facets of stigma. From a practical standpoint, our findings suggest that first, it is crucial for anti-stigma interventions to foster emotional intimacy and a sense of familiarity in addition to providing factual information; second, that different approaches toward improving stigma-related outcomes should be used for people with different levels of familiarity with mental illness, and third, that anti-stigma interventions should be carried out in tandem with efforts to improve the mental health of the population. Of note, findings from this study will be incorporated into Mind HK's future programme direction, thus maximising the practical potential of the present research.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author, O.Z.-S. The data are not publicly available due to their containing information that could compromise the privacy of research participants.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to the team at Mind HK and Time to Change UK for their support in designing the survey used in this study, and to the team at Social Policy Research Limited for their role in collecting the survey data. We also sincerely thank the survey respondents who contributed their time and insights.

Author contributions

S.N. contributed to the devising of survey questions, the conception and design of this study, data analysis and interpretation, and writing the manuscript. P.W.-C.W. and H.R. contributed to the devising of survey questions, revising the article for important intellectual content and reviewing the final version before publication. O.Z.-S. contributed to writing the manuscript, revising the article for important intellectual content and reviewing the final version before publication. All authors have approved this manuscript for publication and have agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work described here.

Funding

The survey analysed in this study was commissioned by Mind HK and is part of their research study aiming to examine the state of mental health and well-being in the Hong Kong population, to understand the factors that affect it (e.g. mental health stigma and resource availability) and to inform the development of improved mental health support systems and resources in the community. Funding for Mind HK's anti-stigma programming and research is generously provided by MINDSET Hong Kong and Jardine Matheson.

Declaration of interest

S.N.'s PhD scholarship is funded by a grant from Mind HK. H.R. is the former CEO and a current Board Member for Mind HK. P.W.-C.W. is a Board Advisor. O.Z.-S. is a Board Member for Mind HK.

References

Volkow, ND, Gordon, JA, Koob, GF. Choosing appropriate language to reduce the stigma around mental illness and substance use disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 2021; 46(13): 2230–2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, KJ, Lewis, C, Roberts, A, Richards, NA, Evison, C, Pearce, HA, et al. The effect of the COVID-19 pandemic on mental health in individuals with pre-existing mental illness. BJPsych Open 2022; 8(2): e59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corrigan, PW, Watson, AC. Understanding the impact of stigma on people with mental illness. World Psychiatry 2002; 1(1): 16.Google ScholarPubMed
Goffman, E. Stigma: Notes on the Management of Spoiled Identity. Prentice-Hall, 1963.Google Scholar
Walsh, DAB, Foster, JLH. A call to action. a critical review of mental health related anti-stigma campaigns. Front Public Health 2021; 8: 569539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corrigan, PW, Nieweglowski, K. How does familiarity impact the stigma of mental illness? Clin Psychol Rev 2019; 70: 4050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thornicroft, G, Mehta, N, Clement, S, Evans-Lacko, S, Doherty, M, Rose, D, et al. Evidence for effective interventions to reduce mental-health-related stigma and discrimination. Lancet 2016; 387(10023): 1123–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorm, AF, Ross, AM. Guidelines for the public on how to provide mental health first aid: narrative review. BJPsych Open 2018; 4(6): 427–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choudhry, FR, Mani, V, Ming, LC, Khan, TM. Beliefs and perception about mental health issues: a meta-synthesis. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2016; 12: 2807.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong, Y-LI, Kong, D, Tu, L, Frasso, R. “My bitterness is deeper than the ocean”: understanding internalized stigma from the perspectives of persons with schizophrenia and their family caregivers. Int J Ment Health Syst 2018; 12: 14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehta, SI, Farina, A. Associative stigma: perceptions of the difficulties of college-aged children of stigmatized fathers. J Soc Clin Psychol 1988; 7(2): 192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ran, M-S, Zhang, T-M, Wong, IY-L, Yang, X, Liu, C-C, Liu, B, et al. Internalized stigma in people with severe mental illness in rural China. Int J Soc Psychiatry 2018; 64(1): 916.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oliveira, AM, Machado, D, Fonseca, JB, Palha, F, Silva Moreira, P, Sousa, N, et al. Stigmatizing attitudes toward patients with psychiatric disorders among medical students and professionals. Front Psychiatry 2020; 11: 326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandhu, HS, Arora, A, Brasch, J, Streiner, DL. Mental health stigma: explicit and implicit attitudes of Canadian undergraduate students, medical school students, and psychiatrists. Can J Psychiatry 2019; 64(3): 209–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potts, LC, Henderson, C. Moderation by socioeconomic status of the relationship between familiarity with mental illness and stigma outcomes. SSM Popul Health 2020; 11: 100611.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Census and Statistics Department. Hong Kong 2021 Population Census - Summary Results. The Government of the Hong Kong Special Administrative Region, 2022 (https://www.census2021.gov.hk/en/publications.html).Google Scholar
Evans-Lacko, S, Little, K, Meltzer, H, Rose, D, Rhydderch, D, Henderson, C, et al. Development and psychometric properties of the Mental Health Knowledge Schedule. Can J Psychiatry 2010; 55(7): 440–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, SM, Dear, MJ. Scaling community attitudes toward the mentally ill. Schizophr Bull 1981; 7(2): 225–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans-Lacko, S, Rose, D, Little, K, Flach, C, Rhydderch, D, Henderson, C, et al. Development and psychometric properties of the Reported and Intended Behaviour Scale (RIBS): a stigma-related behaviour measure. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci 2011; 20(3): 263–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henderson, C, Thornicroft, G. Stigma and discrimination in mental illness: time to change. Lancet 2009; 373(9679): 1928–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, J, Li, J, Thornicroft, G, Huang, Y. Levels of stigma among community mental health staff in Guangzhou, China. BMC Psychiatry 2014; 14: 231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuo, Y-P, Hung, J-T, Huang, T-W, Hsu, P-C, Su, H-C, Lin, EC-L. Developing and testing the validity and reliability of the Chinese version Attitudes Toward Mental Illness Scale in a sample of senior high school students. Hu Li Za Zhi 2014; 61(6): 48.Google Scholar
Sampogna, G, Bakolis, I, Evans-Lacko, S, Robinson, E, Thornicroft, G, Henderson, C. The impact of social marketing campaigns on reducing mental health stigma: results from the 2009–2014 Time to Change programme. Eur Psychiatry 2017; 40: 116–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, EP, Corrigan, PW, Williams, P, Canar, J, Kubiak, MA. Changing attitudes about schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1999; 25(3): 447–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong, H, Chan, SM. The impacts of housing factors on deprivation in a world city: the case of Hong Kong. Soc Policy Adm 2019; 53(6): 872–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moodie, L, Ilie, G, Rutledge, R, Andreou, P, Kirkland, S. Assessment of current mental health status in a population-based sample of Canadian men with and without a history of prostate cancer diagnosis: an analysis of the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA). Front Psychiatry 2020; 11: 586260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nguyen, MT, Chan, WY, Keeler, C. The association between self-rated mental health status and total health care expenditure: a cross-sectional analysis of a nationally representative sample. Medicine (Baltimore) 2015; 94(35): e1410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sainani, KL. Dealing with non-normal data. PM R 2012; 4(12): 1001–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hwang, K-k. Face and favor: the Chinese power game. Am J Sociol 1987; 92(4): 944–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pettigrew, TF, Tropp, LR. How does intergroup contact reduce prejudice? Meta-analytic tests of three mediators. Eur J Soc Psychol 2008; 38(6): 922–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luo, M, Hancock, JT. Self-disclosure and social media: motivations, mechanisms and psychological well-being. Curr Opin Psychol 2020; 31: 110–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corrigan, PW, Watson, AC, Barr, L. The self-stigma of mental illness: implications for self-esteem and self-efficacy. J Soc Clin Psychol 2006; 25(8): 875.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, S, Lee, MT, Chiu, MY, Kleinman, A. Experience of social stigma by people with schizophrenia in Hong Kong. Br J Psychiatry 2005; 186(2): 153–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, DK-W, Ng, PY-N. The prevalence and predictors of self-stigma of individuals with mental health illness in two Chinese cities. Int J Soc Psychiatry 2016; 62(2): 176–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lo, LLH, Suen, YN, Chan, SKW, Sum, MY, Charlton, C, Hui, CLM, et al. Sociodemographic correlates of public stigma about mental illness: a population study on Hong Kong's Chinese population. BMC Psychiatry 2021; 21: 274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Girma, E, Tesfaye, M, Froeschl, G, Möller-Leimkühler, AM, Müller, N, Dehning, S. Public stigma against people with mental illness in the Gilgel Gibe Field Research Center (GGFRC) in Southwest Ethiopia. PLoS One 2013; 8(12): e82116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chan, SKW, Tam, WWY, Lee, KW, Hui, CLM, Chang, WC, Lee, EHM, et al. A population study of public stigma about psychosis and its contributing factors among Chinese population in Hong Kong. Int J Soc Psychiatry 2016; 62(3): 205–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsang, HW, Tam, PK, Chan, F, Chang, W. Sources of burdens on families of individuals with mental illness. Int J Rehabil Res 2003; 26(2): 123–30.Google ScholarPubMed
Nurser, KP, Rushworth, I, Shakespeare, T, Williams, D. Personal storytelling in mental health recovery. Ment Health Rev J 2018; 23: 2536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Descriptive statistics for scores on stigma measures across sociodemographic and personal and interpersonal mental health experience variable subgroups

Figure 1

Table 2 Results of multiple regression analyses of the relationship between personal and interpersonal mental health experience variables and stigma outcome measures, controlling for age, gender and educational attainment

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.