Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T07:46:11.498Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Food and the gut: relevance to some of the autisms

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2017

Paul Whiteley*
Affiliation:
ESPA Research, 2A Hylton Park, Hylton Park Road, Sunderland SR5 3HD, UK
*
Corresponding author: P. Whiteley, email paul.whiteley@espa-research.org.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Complex, diverse and rarely appearing without comorbidity, the autism spectrum disorders continue to be a source of research interest. With core symptoms variably impacting on social communication skills, the traditional focus of many research efforts has centred on the brain and how genetic and environmental processes impact on brain structure, function and/or connectivity to account for various behavioural presentations. Alongside emerging ideas on autistic traits being present in various clinical states, the autisms, and the overrepresentation of several comorbid conditions impacting on quality of life, other research avenues have opened up. The central role of the brain in relation to autism may be at least partially influenced by the functions of other organs. The gastrointestinal (GI) tract represents an important biological system pertinent to at least some autism. The notion of a gut–brain–behaviour axis has garnered support from various findings: an overrepresentation of functional and pathological bowel states, bowel and behavioural findings showing bidirectional associations, a possible relationship between diet, GI function and autism and recently, greater focus on aspects of the GI tract such as the collected gut microbiota in relation to autism. Gaps remain in our knowledge of the functions of the GI tract linked to autism, specifically regarding mechanisms of action onward to behavioural presentation. Set however within the context of diversity in the presentation of autism, science appears to be moving towards defining important GI-related autism phenotypes with the possibility of promising dietary and other related intervention options onward to improving quality of life.

Type
Conference on ‘Diet, nutrition and mental health and wellbeing’
Copyright
Copyright © The Author 2017 

It would not be out of place to suggest that the cumulative results of the huge research efforts dedicated to the autism spectrum disorders down the years have probably created less certainty about what we know about the condition(s). Autism continues to be diagnosed solely on the basis of observable behaviours and recorded/remembered developmental history similar to when first formally described over 80 years ago( Reference Kanner 1 ). Identifying core symptoms in the areas of social and communicative functions remains one of the few facts known about autism( 2 ) as proposed genetic and biological tests have come and gone. In light of the increasingly popular notion of plurality( Reference Whitehouse and Stanley 3 ) where autistic traits are seemingly present in various different conditions, the autisms, it is perhaps becoming less and less likely that a universal biological test for autism will ever be successfully developed despite multiple lay media headlines intimating how close science is to achieving such a goal based on measures of brain function or eye tracking for example.

What is becoming clear is that autism or autism spectrum disorders as a diagnostic label serves a purpose in identifying those people presenting with the cluster of symptoms included under it but seemingly has little function (validity) as a starting point when it comes to determining underlying genetics or biology( Reference Waterhouse, London and Gillberg 4 ). The heterogeneity present throughout the autism spectrum is further complicated by a myriad of overrepresented comorbid labels accompanying a diagnosis( Reference Aldinger, Lane and Veenstra-VanderWeele 5 ). Said comorbidity variably impact on quality of life, sometimes more significantly than the autism diagnosis itself. Coupled also to extensive evidence on differing developmental trajectories being present in autism( Reference Szatmari, Georgiades and Duku 6 ) and the problems facing autism research are multiple and complex when specifically reliant on the use of the singular-label ‘autism’ as a research starting point.

The brain and autism

The structure and function of the brain in relation to autism has enjoyed a privileged research position down the years. It is natural to assume that the brain plays an important role in the presentation of autism given its behavioural focus. Such research has spurred a revolution in our thinking about autism, as the concept of ‘neurodiversity’ has risen in prominence highlighting heterogeneity. It has also witnessed the increasing use of more problematic terms such as ‘neurotypical’ to somehow differentiate brain or thinking styles in autism from not-autism in a binary fashion. Such terminology runs counter to the understanding that ‘typical’ is not something that can yet be suitably defined in relation to the brain in any population; also taking no account of fluidity in behaviour and brain function as a consequence of maturation or the presence of comorbid conditions or other allied factors.

Alongside the rise and rise of technological advances providing insights into the inner workings of the brain, significant resources continue to be ploughed into examination of this important organ with autism in mind( Reference Nickl-Jockschat, Habel and Michel 7 ). Challenges remain however in interpreting data derived from such imaging studies of the brain with a focus on autism, also not helped by sweeping generalised theories of brain structure or function in relation to autism( Reference Baron-Cohen 8 ). The idea of an ‘autistic brain’( Reference Grecucci, Rubicondo and Siugzdaite 9 ) to denote generalised differences in structure or function to account for autistic behaviours again offers only a simplistic explanation for an issue of significant complexity. This and many other areas of autism science it seems, continue to ignore the quite remarkable heterogeneity present alongside the label, including also the fact that conditions such as epilepsy/seizure-disorder also known to affect brain functioning are among one of the most important comorbidities accompanying autism( Reference Viscidi, Triche and Pescosolido 10 ).

The gastrointestinal tract and autism

Of the many scientific advances that are being made in relation to autism (i.e. that the presence of comorbidity is the rule rather than the exception), there is an increasing realisation that the brain may not be the only organ relevant to (some) autism. The gastrointestinal (GI) tract has been discussed with reference to autism for many years. Some of the most prominent people central to the history of autism spectrum disorders have talked about how facets of GI functioning may not be totally unrelated to at least some cases of autism( Reference Asperger 11 ). In more recent times, discussions have focused on several aspects of GI functioning as being potentially relevant to (some) autism based on various strands of peer-reviewed evidence( Reference Whiteley 12 ). This includes: the overrepresentation of functional and pathological bowel states in cases of autism( Reference McElhanon, McCracken and Karpen 13 , Reference Doshi-Velez, Avillach and Palmer 14 ), the impact of various dietary interventions for autism( Reference Whiteley, Shattock and Knivsberg 15 ) and the new triad comprising GI immune function–intestinal barrier permeability–gut microbiota( Reference Bischoff, Barbara and Buurman 16 ) potentially being relevant to some of the autisms.

The gut–brain–behaviour axis and (some) autism

The idea of a gut–brain–behaviour axis in relation to autism derives evidence from various different sources. The GI tract is at its most basic an energy-converting device. Using food and drink as fuel, this complicated organ performs countless duties to release energy from diet to drive/maintain the various biological systems of the body including the brain. Food therefore represents an important variable in any discussions about the GI tract.

It has long been known that certain foods when meeting certain GI tracts can cause issues in relation to physical health as per the example of the diet-related autoimmune condition coeliac disease. No less important is the cumulative evidence suggesting that under particular circumstances, food can also affect mental health as noted in the inborn error of metabolism called phenylketonuria. Where specific offending foods are removed from the diet in conditions such as phenylketonuria, remarkable benefits are noted in relation to behaviour and cognition. Ergo, science has a template for suggesting that a gut–brain axis exists and food is a potentially modifying variable. Such a template may also be pertinent to some autism; not only on the basis that various important inborn errors of metabolism may be overrepresented in cases of autism( Reference Simons, Eyskens and Glazemakers 17 ) (including phenylketonuria) but that the interest in the GI tract in relation to autism similarly models a potential role for dietary factors in some instances.

Drawing on earlier research hinting at a role for the GI tract and diet in cases of schizophrenia spectrum disorders( Reference Vlissides, Venulet and Jenner 18 ), research focus has shifted to specific dietary elements as being potentially important to autism. Wheat or more specifically gluten, has received considerable research attention on the basis of the pharmacology of the protein and its breakdown metabolites and their similarity to other biologically-active agents( Reference Zioudrou, Streaty and Klee 19 ). The focus on opioid peptide metabolites has similarly ‘pulled in’ other foodstuffs such as milk and dairy products on the basis of their proposed similar chemical activity( Reference Ul Haq, Kapila and Kapila 20 ).

Various studies have reported on the effects of removal of gluten and casein containing foods from the diets of people on the autism spectrum( Reference Whiteley, Shattock and Knivsberg 15 ). By no means a universal effect( Reference Hyman, Stewart and Foley 21 ), discussions have turned to the possibility that within the autisms there may be one or more phenotypes( Reference Whiteley 12 ) sensitive to such dietary elements. Such a notion opens up the possibility of identifying potential best- and non-responders to dietary intervention impacting on some of the core and peripheral aspects of autism( Reference Pedersen, Parlar and Kvist 22 ).

There is still confusion about what specific elements may be at work when it comes to examining the effects (or not) of a gluten- and casein-free diet in relation to autism. Five key areas stand out in the research literature potentially pertinent to effects: (i) the biological activity and pharmacological effects of the specific foods; (ii) the role of enzyme function or conditions for enzyme functions acting on the metabolism of foods; (iii) altered intestinal barrier function as a means of any food-derived biological activity reaching the wider central nervous system; (iv) a role for immune function and specific responses to dietary elements; (v) a role for the collected gut microbiota.

Dietary elements as biologically active entities linked to autism?

With the requirement for greater research inspection, the suggestion that the opioid-like qualities of peptide species derived from gluten and casein( Reference Zioudrou, Streaty and Klee 19 , Reference Ul Haq, Kapila and Kapila 20 ) may impact on the presentation of autism has a long history. The notion that there is overlap in the behaviours noted in situations of long-term opioid exposure (in animals and human subjects) and cases of autism provided a basis for early explanations of how such food elements might affect behaviour. The inclusion of other potentially important effects linking GI symptoms (e.g. constipation) and opioid-based drugs( Reference Müller-Lissner, Bassotti and Coffin 23 ) also coincided with some of the functional bowel findings noted in autism( Reference McElhanon, McCracken and Karpen 13 ). Independent evidence citing the potential effectiveness of certain anti-opioid medication (naltrexone) in relation to facets of autism( Reference Roy, Roy and Deb 24 ) and studies querying enzymatic function pertinent to gluten and casein protein metabolism( Reference Vojdani, Bazargan and Vojdani 25 ) also added to the feasibility of an opioid-excess hypothesis of autism. Similarly, dietary interventions probably affecting gluten and casein intake but not exclusively labelled as gluten-free casein-free have also provided surrogate evidence for potential effects. The use of a ketogenic diet, high fat, low carbohydrate, in cases of autism for example, has some supporting evidence of effect( Reference Castro, Slongo Faccioli and Baronio 26 ).

Intestinal barrier function(s) and autism (but not ‘leaky gut’)

Leaky gut’ in the context of autism still invokes ridicule and charges of pseudo-science in some quarters. Despite increasing evidence implicating alterations to the permeability of the intestinal barrier in relation to various conditions( Reference Julio-Pieper, Bravo and Aliaga 27 ) including autism( Reference de Magistris, Familiari and Pascotto 28 , Reference Fiorentino, Sapone and Senger 29 ), the term remains contentious. Such a response is not helped by the implications that: (a) the intestinal barrier is an impenetrable barrier deflecting anything and everything away from contact with the wider central nervous system; (b) by labelling the gut as ‘leaky’, there is an oversimplification of the complex structure and workings of the intestinal barrier in relation to its large surface area and lack of uniformity when it comes to permeability.

It is perhaps therefore more accurate to use the term intestinal hyperpermeability to denote a more realistic scenario where appreciation for intestinal permeability exists; not least that typical GI permeability represents a key process in diverting energy (nutrition) from the digestion of dietary elements into wider circulation. The focus is therefore on atypical permeability and how this manifests in relation to autism.

As mentioned, there is evidence for intestinal hyperpermeability in cases of autism( Reference de Magistris, Familiari and Pascotto 28 , Reference Fiorentino, Sapone and Senger 29 ) based on both direct measurement and also as a function of other observations such as evidence of bacterial translocation( Reference Williams, Hornig and Parekh 30 ). The precise reason(s) for such a state are not yet fully understood but diet has been observed to be a potential factor; specifically the use of a gluten-free casein-free diet( Reference de Magistris, Familiari and Pascotto 28 ) mirroring research in relation to other labels( Reference Drago, El Asmar and Di Pierro 31 ). The possibility of a direct effect of dietary elements, specifically gluten and casein metabolites, on gut barrier integrity provides an additional strand to the notion that such foods can affect some autism. Not only may opioid peptides originating from foods containing gluten and casein have potential direct pharmacological activity on the central nervous system (brain) but also they could be key moderators of the means to enter into general circulation. Such effects require further investigation. Specifically how such entities may impact on the enteric nervous system (i.e. within the GI tract) and their action on key barrier proteins such as zonulin( Reference Sturgeon and Fasano 32 ).

The gut–‘bug’–brain–behaviour axis

The collected bacteria and other species that populate the human GI tract has become big research business in recent years. Not a day seemingly goes by without a specific species or general measure of bacterial diversity being implicated in all-manner of conditions, labels and states. What is becoming clear from the science so far is that the functions of the gut microbiome do seem to be more diverse than merely aiding digestion or the production of nutrients. No better example of this extended role is evidenced by the notion of psychobiotics( Reference Dinan, Stanton and Cryan 33 ) denoting how elements of the gut microbiota may carry influence on aspects of human and animal behaviour and development. The production of peripheral serotonin in the GI tract by enterochromaffin cells as potentially being mediated by the gut microbiota represents one example of psychobiotics in action.

Still a research area in its infancy, autism (whether modelled in animals or studied directly) has provided some key information about a possible relationship between the gut microbiota and behaviour and/or development. Rodent studies, for example( Reference Hsiao, McBride and Hsien 34 ), have linked behaviour, gut bacteria and intestinal permeability. Various human studies have detailed differences in gut bacterial constitution in relation to autism( Reference Ding, Taur and Walkup 35 ) based on both individual species and overall bacterial diversity. More preliminary data on how specific probiotics, bacterial species thought to confer some health advantage, may affect the presentation of autism( Reference Navarro, Liu and Rhoads 36 ) have also been published. Research on the potential effectiveness of faecal microbiota transplant in relation to autism( Reference Kang, Adams and Gregory 37 ) has similarly been undertaken.

More investigations are required as to the importance of the gut microbiome in relation to autism. The mechanism of effect, from gut bacteria to behaviour, in particular requires further explanation( Reference Grenham, Clarke and Cryan 38 ) and how diet and other factors will influence gut bacterial populations for example. It is however, getting harder to discount the idea that the gut is truly a ‘thinking organ’ and, alongside producing various neurotransmitters and hormones, the cross-talk between gut bacteria and the central nervous system may be important for various labels/conditions/states including some cases of autism.

Where next?

Research does not happen in a social or political vacuum. Autism is a prime example of this notion, as within the complexity and diversity of the label, various viewpoints exist on issues such as the gut–brain axis and the acceptability of interventions related to diet or other GI-affected issues. The question of ‘where next?’ therefore is not one simply driven by science but also an understanding of the wants and wishes of those on the spectrum and their significant others.

It is logical to assume that given the presence of GI issues in cases of autism (sometime severe and life-changing) moves to alleviate such issues should be accelerated. If by altering the pattern or severity of such GI issues corresponding positive changes are noted in behaviours linked to autism that negatively affect quality of life, this should be welcomed. The various processes already noted (dietary elements, intestinal barrier functions, gut microbiota) separately and cumulatively lend themselves to intervention. The use of artificial enzymes to aid digestive processes( Reference Saad, Eltayeb and Mohamad 39 ) represents one intervention avenue in addition to those mentioned in relation to the use of probiotics and/or faecal microbiota transplant. Dietary changes also remain a possibility in light of the evidence of effect (for some) already produced. Early findings in relation to the expression of the barrier protein zonulin in relation to autism( Reference Esnafoglu, Cırrık and Ayyıldız 40 ) require replication and lend themselves to possible intervention.

To correct one generalisation already mentioned in this commentary, that all milk sources are chemically the same in terms of their release of opioid peptides during digestion( Reference Pal, Woodford and Kukuljan 41 ), other areas of intervention are also opening up. Studies highlighting short-term positive behavioural effects following the use of alternative mammalian milk sources (alternative to cow milk) in relation to autism( Reference Bashir and Al-Ayadhi 42 ) have offered potential evidence of effect. Taking into account the existing literature on how GI issues may be overrepresented when it comes to autism( Reference McElhanon, McCracken and Karpen 13 , Reference Doshi-Velez, Avillach and Palmer 14 ) the idea that not all cow milk may provoke the same GI issues( Reference Jianqin, Leiming and Lu 43 ) provides a platform for additional studies specifically in relation to the use of a1 β-casein free milk (a2 milk) and autism( Reference Allison and Clarke 44 ). Research is currently underway examining whether, under double-blind, placebo-controlled conditions, use of a2 milk might impact on some of the core and peripheral behavioural presentations of autism( 45 ).

Conclusions

The case for the diagnosis of autism reflecting a complex, diverse and rarely stand-alone condition has been proven beyond doubt. The idea that the brain, although central to the behavioural presentation of autism, is not the only organ important to autism is gaining scientific momentum. Within the diversity of autism, the plural autisms, the GI tract is being implicated in multiple cases potentially pointing to one or more autistic phenotypes being characterised by GI involvement. Evidence is accumulating to suggest that various facets of GI function may exert an important influence on the presentation of behaviours linked to autism. Interventions targeting adverse GI conditions in relation to autism may also show some promise in terms of positively affecting aspects of autism in light of a growing interest in a gut–brain–behaviour relationship.

Acknowledgements

The author wishes to acknowledge Rick Miller and The a2 Milk Company for putting forward the author and sponsoring the session that the author presented at during the 2016 Winter meeting of the Nutrition Society. The author also acknowledges the contribution of the Robert Luff Foundation in providing funds pertinent to the author's research discussed in this manuscript.

Financial Support

The author received no financial funding or other recompense from The a2 Milk Company or any other external organisation for attending and presenting at the 2016 Winter meeting of the Nutrition Society or authoring this paper. The author is currently in receipt of a grant from The a2 Milk Company in relation to an ongoing collaborative study examining the potential effectiveness of a2 milk on the behavioural presentation of autism and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in children (see: https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT02911194). Alongside being employed by ESPA Research, the author is also a director at Analutos, a commercial analytical company providing mass spectrometric and other analytical services to individuals and organisations. The a2 Milk Company and partner organisations are customers of Analutos.

Conflicts of Interest

None.

Authorship

P. W. is the sole author of this paper.

References

1. Kanner, L (1943) Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nerv Child 2, 217250.Google Scholar
2. World Health Organization (1993) Mental Disorders: a Glossary and Guide to Their Classification in Accordance with the 10th Revision of the International Classification of Diseases – Research Diagnostic Criteria (ICD-10). Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
3. Whitehouse, AJO & Stanley, FJ (2013) Is autism one or multiple disorders? Med J Aust 198, 302303.Google Scholar
4. Waterhouse, L, London, E & Gillberg, C (2016) ASD Validity. Rev J Autism Dev Disord 3, 302329.Google Scholar
5. Aldinger, KA, Lane, CJ, Veenstra-VanderWeele, J et al. (2015) Patterns of risk for multiple co-occurring medical conditions replicate across distinct cohorts of children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res 8, 771781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6. Szatmari, P, Georgiades, S, Duku, E et al. (2015) Developmental trajectories of symptom severity and adaptive functioning in an inception cohort of preschool children with autism spectrum disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 276283.Google Scholar
7. Nickl-Jockschat, T, Habel, U, Michel, TM et al. (2012) Brain structure anomalies in autism spectrum disorder–a meta-analysis of VBM studies using anatomic likelihood estimation. Hum Brain Mapp 33, 14701489.Google Scholar
8. Baron-Cohen, S (2010) Empathizing, systemizing, and the extreme male brain theory of autism. Prog Brain Res 186, 167175.Google Scholar
9. Grecucci, A, Rubicondo, D, Siugzdaite, R et al. (2016) Uncovering the social deficits in the autistic brain. A source-based morphometric study. Front Neurosci 10, 388.Google Scholar
10. Viscidi, EW, Triche, EW, Pescosolido, MF et al. (2013) Clinical characteristics of children with autism spectrum disorder and co-occurring epilepsy. PLoS ONE 8, e67797.Google Scholar
11. Asperger, H (1961) Psychopathology of children with coeliac disease. Ann Paediatr 197, 346351.Google Scholar
12. Whiteley, P (2015) Nutritional management of (some) autism: a case for gluten- and casein-free diets? Proc Nutr Soc 74, 202207.Google Scholar
13. McElhanon, BO, McCracken, C, Karpen, S et al. (2014) Gastrointestinal symptoms in autism spectrum disorder: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics 133, 872883.Google Scholar
14. Doshi-Velez, F, Avillach, P, Palmer, N et al. (2015) Prevalence of inflammatory bowel disease among patients with autism spectrum disorders. Inflamm Bowel Dis 21, 22812288.Google Scholar
15. Whiteley, P, Shattock, P, Knivsberg, AM et al. (2013) Gluten- and casein-free dietary intervention for autism spectrum conditions. Front Hum Neurosci 6, 344.Google Scholar
16. Bischoff, SC, Barbara, G, Buurman, W et al. (2014) Intestinal permeability–a new target for disease prevention and therapy. BMC Gastroenterol 14, 189.Google Scholar
17. Simons, A, Eyskens, F, Glazemakers, I et al. (2017) Can psychiatric childhood disorders be due to inborn errors of metabolism? Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 26, 143154.Google Scholar
18. Vlissides, DN, Venulet, A & Jenner, FA (1986) A double-blind gluten-free/gluten-load controlled trial in a secure ward population. Br J Psychiatry 148, 447452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
19. Zioudrou, C, Streaty, RA & Klee, WA (1979) Opioid peptides derived from food proteins. The exorphins. J Biol Chem 254, 24462449.Google Scholar
20. Ul Haq, MR, Kapila, R & Kapila, S (2015) Release of β-casomorphin-7/5 during simulated gastrointestinal digestion of milk β-casein variants from Indian crossbred cattle (Karan Fries). Food Chem 168, 7079.Google Scholar
21. Hyman, SL, Stewart, PA, Foley, J et al. (2016) The gluten-free/casein-free diet: a double-blind challenge trial in children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 46, 205220.Google Scholar
22. Pedersen, L, Parlar, S, Kvist, K et al. (2014) Data mining the ScanBrit study of a gluten- and casein-free dietary intervention for children with autism spectrum disorders: behavioural and psychometric measures of dietary response. Nutr Neurosci 17, 207213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23. Müller-Lissner, S, Bassotti, G, Coffin, B et al. (2016) Opioid-induced constipation and bowel dysfunction: a clinical guideline. Pain Med (Epublication ahead of print version).Google Scholar
24. Roy, A, Roy, M, Deb, S et al. (2015) Are opioid antagonists effective in attenuating the core symptoms of autism spectrum conditions in children: a systematic review. J Intell Disabil Res 59, 293306.Google Scholar
25. Vojdani, A, Bazargan, M, Vojdani, E et al. (2004) Heat shock protein and gliadin peptide promote development of peptidase antibodies in children with autism and patients with autoimmune disease. Clin Diagn Lab Immunol 11, 515524.Google Scholar
26. Castro, K, Slongo Faccioli, L, Baronio, D et al. (2015) Effect of a ketogenic diet on autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review. Res Autism Spectr. Disord 20, 3138.Google Scholar
27. Julio-Pieper, M, Bravo, JA, Aliaga, E et al. (2014) Review article: intestinal barrier dysfunction and central nervous system disorders–a controversial association. Alim Pharmacol Ther 40, 11871201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28. de Magistris, L, Familiari, V, Pascotto, A et al. (2010) Alterations of the intestinal barrier in patients with autism spectrum disorders and in their first-degree relatives. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 51, 418424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29. Fiorentino, M, Sapone, A, Senger, S et al. (2016) Blood-brain barrier and intestinal epithelial barrier alterations in autism spectrum disorders. Mol Autism 7, 49.Google Scholar
30. Williams, BL, Hornig, M, Parekh, T et al. (2012) Application of novel PCR-based methods for detection, quantitation, and phylogenetic characterization of Sutterella species in intestinal biopsy samples from children with autism and gastrointestinal disturbances. MBio; available at http://mbio.asm.org/content/3/1/e00261-11.full.Google Scholar
31. Drago, S, El Asmar, R, Di Pierro, M et al. (2006) Gliadin, zonulin and gut permeability: effects on celiac and non-celiac intestinal mucosa and intestinal cell lines. Scand J Gastroenterol 41, 408419.Google Scholar
32. Sturgeon, C & Fasano, A (2016) Zonulin, a regulator of epithelial and endothelial barrier functions, and its involvement in chronic inflammatory diseases. Tissue Barriers 4, e1251384.Google Scholar
33. Dinan, TG, Stanton, C & Cryan, JF (2013) Psychobiotics: a novel class of psychotropic. Biol Psychiatry 74, 720726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34. Hsiao, EY, McBride, SW, Hsien, S et al. (2013) Microbiota modulate behavioral and physiological abnormalities associated with neurodevelopmental disorders. Cell 155, 14511463.Google Scholar
35. Ding, HT, Taur, Y & Walkup, JT (2017) Gut microbiota and autism: key concepts and findings. J Autism Dev Disord 47, 480489.Google Scholar
36. Navarro, F, Liu, Y & Rhoads, JM (2016) Can probiotics benefit children with autism spectrum disorders? World J Gastroenterol 22, 1009310102.Google Scholar
37. Kang, DW, Adams, JB, Gregory, AC et al. (2017) Microbiota Transfer Therapy alters gut ecosystem and improves gastrointestinal and autism symptoms: an open-label study. Microbiome 23, 10.Google Scholar
38. Grenham, S, Clarke, G, Cryan, JF et al. (2011) Brain-gut-microbe communication in health and disease. Front Physiol 2, 94.Google Scholar
39. Saad, K, Eltayeb, AA, Mohamad, IL et al. (2015) A randomized, placebo-controlled trial of digestive enzymes in children with autism spectrum disorders. Clin Psychopharmacol Neurosci 13, 188193.Google Scholar
40. Esnafoglu, E, Cırrık, S, Ayyıldız, SN et al. (2017) Increased serum zonulin levels as an intestinal permeability marker in autistic subjects. J Pediatr 188, 240244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41. Pal, S, Woodford, K, Kukuljan, S et al. (2015) Milk intolerance, beta-casein and lactose. Nutrients 7, 72857297.Google Scholar
42. Bashir, S & Al-Ayadhi, LY (2014) Effect of camel milk on thymus and activation-regulated chemokine in autistic children: double-blind study. Pediatr Res 75, 559563.Google Scholar
43. Jianqin, S, Leiming, X, Lu, X et al. (2016) Effects of milk containing only A2 beta casein versus milk containing both A1 and A2 beta casein proteins on gastrointestinal physiology, symptoms of discomfort, and cognitive behavior of people with self-reported intolerance to traditional cows’ milk. Nutr J 15, 35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44. Allison, AJ & Clarke, AJ (2006) Further research for consideration in ‘the A2 milk case’. Eur J Clin Nutr 60, 921924.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45.ClinicalTrials.gov (2016) a2 Milk for Autism and Attention-deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT02911194. (accessed March 2017).Google Scholar