Hostname: page-component-65f69f4695-22cgb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-06-27T08:08:51.941Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Stress dynamics that maintain posttraumatic stress disorder across 20 years

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 May 2025

Whitney R. Ringwald*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
Scott Feltman
Affiliation:
Department of Applied Mathematics, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
Sean A.P. Cloutson
Affiliation:
Program in Public Health and Department of Family, Population, and Preventive Medicine, Renaissance School of Medicine, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
Frank Mann
Affiliation:
Program in Public Health and Department of Family, Population, and Preventive Medicine, Renaissance School of Medicine, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA Department of Medicine, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
Camilo Ruggero
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of North Texas, Denton, TX, USA
Evelyn Bromet
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
Benjamin J. Luft
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA World Trade Center Health Program, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
Roman Kotov
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
*
Corresponding author: Whitney Ringwald; Email: wringwald@umn.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is often chronic and impairing. Mechanisms that maintain symptoms remain poorly understood because of heterogenous presentation. We parsed this heterogeneity by examining how individual differences in stress-symptom dynamics relate to the long-term maintenance of PTSD.

Methods

We studied 7,308 trauma-exposed World Trade Center responders who self-reported PTSD symptoms and stressful life events at annual monitoring visits for up to 20 years (average = 8.8 visits; [range = 4–16]). We used multilevel structural equation models to separate the stable and time-varying components of symptoms and stressors. At the within-person level, we modeled stress reactivity by cross-lagged associations between stress and future symptoms, stress generation by cross-lagged associations between symptoms and future stress, and autoregressive effects represented symptom persistence and stress persistence. The clinical utility of the stress-symptom dynamics was evaluated by associations with PTSD chronicity and mental health care use.

Results

Stress reactivity, stress generation, and symptom persistence were significant on average (bs = 0.03–0.16). There were significant individual differences in the strength of each dynamic (interquartile ranges = 0.06–0.12). Correlations among within-person processes showed some dynamics are intertwined (e.g. more reactive people also generate stress in a vicious cycle) and others represent distinct phenotypes (e.g. people are reactive or have persistent symptoms). Initial trauma severity amplified some dynamics. People in the top deciles of most dynamics had clinically significant symptom levels across the monitoring period and their health care cost 6–17× more per year than people at median levels.

Conclusions

Individual differences in stress-symptom dynamics contribute to the chronicity and clinical burden of PTSD.

Information

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Many people with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) experience symptoms for years after the initial trauma (Bryant et al., Reference Bryant, Nickerson, Creamer, O’Donnell, Forbes, Galatzer-Levy, McFarlane and Silove2015; Galatzer-Levy et al., Reference Galatzer-Levy, Huang and Bonanno2018; Marmar et al., Reference Marmar, Schlenger, Henn-Haase, Qian, Purchia, Li, Corry, Williams, Ho, Horesh, Karstoft, Shalev and Kulka2015; Solomon & Mikulincer, Reference Solomon and Mikulincer2006). Indeed, the majority of people with PTSD have symptoms even after undergoing gold standard treatment (Hoskins et al., Reference Hoskins, Pearce, Bethell, Dankova, Barbui, Tol, van Ommeren, de Jong, Seedat, Chen and Bisson2015; Steenkamp, Litz, & Marmar, Reference Steenkamp, Litz and Marmar2020; Steenkamp, Litz, Hoge, & Marmar, Reference Steenkamp, Litz, Hoge and Marmar2015). This often prolonged course of PTSD, and ongoing need for mental health services, comes at a considerable economic cost to the individual and society (Von Der Warth, Dams, Grochtdreis, & König, Reference Von Der Warth, Dams, Grochtdreis and König2020). Identifying mechanisms that determine a chronic symptom course is imperative to develop more targeted and effective interventions and to reduce socioeconomic burden. Psychological mechanisms involving transactions between stressful life events and symptoms are among the more promising candidates for explaining the long-term maintenance of PTSD, and they can be directly translated into intervention targets (Chiu, Low, Chan, & Meiser-Stedman, Reference Chiu, Low, Chan and Meiser-Stedman2024; Hammen, Reference Hammen1991; Monroe & Harkness, Reference Monroe and Harkness2005). Next, we review four dynamics reflecting mechanisms that have been proposed to help explain PTSD symptoms over time, which we term: (1) Stress Reactivity, (2) Stress Generation, (3) Symptom Persistence, and (4) Stress Persistence.

Stress-symptom dynamics and underlying maintenance mechanisms PTSD

One candidate dynamic is stress reactivity, which refers to how stressful events occurring after the initial trauma can exacerbate symptoms. Stressors may be especially potent for people with PTSD because of pre-trauma vulnerabilities associated with the condition (Brewin, Andrews, & Valentine, Reference Brewin, Andrews and Valentine2000; Chiu et al., Reference Chiu, Low, Chan and Meiser-Stedman2024; Nievergelt et al., Reference Nievergelt, Maihofer, Klengel, Atkinson, Chen, Choi, Coleman, Dalvie, Duncan, Gelernter, Levey, Logue, Polimanti, Provost, Ratanatharathorn, Stein, Torres, Aiello, Almli and Koenen2019). Longitudinal studies of trauma-exposed populations support the role of stress reactivity, with many finding that post-trauma stressors predict subsequent increases in PTSD symptoms and that people exposed to more traumatic experiences are especially reactive (Galea et al., Reference Galea, Ahern, Tracy, Hubbard, Cerda, Goldmann and Vlahov2008; Lowe et al., Reference Lowe, Joshi, Galea, Aiello, Uddin, Koenen and Cerdá2017; Schmied, Larson, Highfill-McRoy, & Thomsen, Reference Schmied, Larson, Highfill-McRoy and Thomsen2016). Furthermore, according to stress sensitization models, trauma exposure may actually cause biological, cognitive, and behavioral changes that amplify responses to stressful life events (Post & Weiss, Reference Post and Weiss1998). Some mechanisms that have been proposed to explain heightened reactivity after trauma include abnormalities in neurobiological systems involved with memory (Careaga, Girardi, & Suchecki, Reference Careaga, Girardi and Suchecki2016; Foa, Steketee, & Rothbaum, Reference Foa, Steketee and Rothbaum1989), selective attention to threat (Ehlers & Clark, Reference Ehlers and Clark2000), and engagement in avoidant coping strategies that prevent adaptive fear conditioning (Seligowski, Lee, Bardeen, & Orcutt, Reference Seligowski, Lee, Bardeen and Orcutt2015). In support of the possibility trauma contributes to stress reactivity, there is also evidence that the more traumatic events a person is exposed to, the more reactive they are to subsequent stressors (Smid et al., Reference Smid, Van Der Velden, Lensvelt-Mulders, Knipscheer, Gersons and Kleber2012, Reference Smid, Kleber, Rademaker, Van Zuiden and Vermetten2013; Zvolensky et al., Reference Zvolensky, Farris, Kotov, Schechter, Bromet, Gonzalez, Vujanovic, Pietrzak, Crane, Kaplan, Moline, Southwick, Feder, Udasin, Reissman and Luft2015a). Thus, regardless of whether reactivity emerges before or after a trauma, PTSD pathology may be prolonged by the continual re-activation of symptoms in response to stressful life events.

A second candidate mechanism is stress generation. Stress generation refers to how symptoms may actually create more stressful conditions that perpetuate a person’s problems (Hammen, Reference Hammen1991; Rnic et al., Reference Rnic, Santee, Hoffmeister, Liu, Chang, Chen and LeMoult2023). In support of stress generation mechanisms in PTSD, longitudinal studies have shown that PTSD symptoms prospectively predict stressful life events (Lowe et al., Reference Lowe, Walsh, Uddin, Galea and Koenen2014; Maniates et al., Reference Maniates, Stoop, Miller, Halberstadt and Wolf2018; Milan, Zona, Acker, & Turcios-Cotto, Reference Milan, Zona, Acker and Turcios-Cotto2013; Sadeh, Miller, Wolf, & Harkness, Reference Sadeh, Miller, Wolf and Harkness2015; Schmied et al., Reference Schmied, Larson, Highfill-McRoy and Thomsen2016; Zvolenskyet al., Reference Zvolensky, Kotov, Schechter, Gonzalez, Vujanovic, Pietrzak, Crane, Kaplan, Moline, Southwick, Feder, Udasin, Reissman and Luft2015b). In conjunction with stress reactivity, stress generation may perpetuate a feedback loop that prolongs PTSD in which intense reactions to stress create more stress, that lead to more intense reactions and so on.

A third dynamic is symptom persistence, which could reflect a slowed return to baseline functioning. Recovery may be slowed because of, for example, inadequate treatment, limited social support, or underdeveloped coping skills (Brewin et al., Reference Brewin, Andrews and Valentine2000). Additionally, stress autonomy or ‘kindling’ models propose that neurobiological changes induced by trauma lower a person’s threshold for stress to a point that trauma-related symptoms are sustained in the absence of major life stressors (Monroe & Harkness, Reference Monroe and Harkness2005; Post et al., Reference Post, Weiss, Smith, Li and McCann1997). There is longitudinal evidence for this sort of symptom persistence over and above the effects of stressful events in depression (Kendler & Gardner, Reference Kendler and Gardner2016), but no studies have examined this mechanism in the context of PTSD. Given the common features with depression, it is possible that symptom persistence contributes to the course of PTSD as well.

In addition to the immediate impact of major stressors, being exposed to unremitting stress may contribute to PTSD. Stress persistence is therefore a fourth potential mechanism, which encompasses prolonged stressors (e.g. health problems) or cascades of stressful events that unfold over the course of years (e.g. getting an injury leads to job loss which leads to debt). Such conditions of persistent stress may have an indirect effect on the chronicity of PTSD vis-à-vis the toll on a person’s emotional, biological, and material resources (Davidson & Baum, Reference Davidson and Baum1986; Hilton et al., Reference Hilton, Ham, Rodrigues, Kirsh, Chapovalov and Seto2020).

Could variation in stress-symptom dynamics govern the long-term course of PTSD?

All longitudinal research on symptoms and stressful events to date has focused on average effects, which ignores the wide variability in the clinical presentation, course, and maintenance mechanisms of PTSD (Bryant, Galatzer-Levy, & Hadzi-Pavlovic, Reference Bryant, Galatzer-Levy and Hadzi-Pavlovic2023; Steinert, Hofmann, Leichsenring, & Kruse, Reference Steinert, Hofmann, Leichsenring and Kruse2015). Parsing this heterogeneity would enable identification of characteristics that make a person more likely to have a chronic condition and need mental health services. Previous work has focused on describing and explaining heterogeneity based on static attributes, such as symptom profiles, personality traits, and type of traumatic event (Campbell, Trachik, Goldberg, & Simpson, Reference Campbell, Trachik, Goldberg and Simpson2020, Campbell-Sills et al., Reference Campbell-Sills, Sun, Choi, He, Ursano, Kessler and Stein2022; Kelley et al., Reference Kelley, Weathers, McDevitt-Murphy, Eakin and Flood2009; Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Hopwood, Donnellan, Wright, Sanislow, McDevitt-Murphy and Morey2014). However, no studies have examined dynamic attributes, such as the four mentioned before. Compared with static attributes, stress-symptom dynamics more directly reflect a critical source of heterogeneity in PTSD; namely, mechanisms that maintain a given person’s pathology over time. Identifying mechanisms that account for why one trauma-exposed person has a worse outcome than another is essential for effective and targeted treatment.

We propose a different approach that addresses gaps left by prior work. Specifically, we seek to understand heterogeneity in PTSD by characterizing dynamic rather than static characteristics. To do this, we examine individual differences in stress reactivity, stress generation, symptom persistence, and stress persistence for people exposed to trauma, in contrast to prior longitudinal research that has exclusively focused on the average effect of these dynamics. Our approach preserves heterogeneity in stress-symptom patterns which then allows us to determine which of these maintain trauma-related pathology and whether they could reflect interrelated mechanisms. Knowing the between-person covariation of within-person effects (i.e. dynamics) has important conceptual and practical implications. For example, if stress reactivity and stress generation are uncorrelated between people, this would imply they reflect two pathways to chronic PTSD, with many patients presenting with one primary temporal pattern or the other. In contrast, if stress reactivity and stress generation covary between people, this would suggest they reflect mechanisms that typically feed into one another and maintain PTSD via a ‘vicious cycle’. Because of the focus on average, within-person effects in nearly all prior longitudinal studies of PTSD, these fundamental properties of stress-symptom dynamics are unknown.

Present study

In the present study, we aimed to provide new insights into mechanisms that maintain PTSD, and which give rise to a more chronic and severe course, by examining individual differences in stress reactivity, stress generation, symptom persistence, and stress persistence. We studied a sample of World Trade Center responders (N = 7,308) who were assessed 4–16 times spanning up to 20 years. The frequency of assessment and study duration is unprecedented in research on temporal associations between stress and PTSD symptoms, which have been mostly limited to two or three time points spanning less than 4 years. Our aims were to (1) identify within-person stress-symptom dynamics that characterize the long-term functioning of trauma-exposed people on average, (2) quantify the extent of between-person variation in these dynamics, (3) determine the between-person covariation in these dynamics, and (4) evaluate the validity and clinical utility of these dynamics by testing whether they predict the severity/chronicity of PTSD and mental health care service utilization.

Methods

Participants and procedures

Participants are members of the WTC Health Program Long Island Clinical Center of Excellence. Membership eligibility is determined by qualifying exposure during response to 9/11 terrorist attacks. Data for this study were drawn from annual visits to the health monitoring program. This is not a treatment-seeking sample, but rather the visits were akin to routine primary care check-ups. The program began monitoring responders in 2002 and has continued annual follow-up visits while maintaining open enrollment. To obtain reliable estimates of symptom dynamics, we only included participants with four or more visits (N = 7,308). On average, participants in our analytic sample had 8.8 visits. The distribution of visits per participant are reported in Supplementary Table S2.

Our sample was mostly White (74%; 4% Black/African American; 1% Asian; 5% other; 16% unknown/not reported) and male (90%), with an average age of 37.7 (SD = 8.16) on 9/11. Among responders who were assessed by clinical interview in the full sample from which our data were drawn, 17.7% met criteria for PTSD at some point after 9/11 (Bromet et al., Reference Bromet, Hobbs, Clouston, Gonzalez, Kotov and Luft2016).

Measures

PTSD symptoms

PTSD symptoms were self-reported at each visit using the trauma-specific version of the PTSD Checklist (PCL-17; Blanchard, Jones-Alexander, Buckley, & Forneris, Reference Blanchard, Jones-Alexander, Buckley and Forneris1996), with ratings referencing the WTC disaster. Participants rated past month DSM-IV symptoms on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely). All 17 items were summed for a total symptom severity score at each visit. Scores ranged from 17 to 85, with the average total symptom score across all visits and participants being 27.6 (SD = 13.06). A total of 21.1% of the sample reported clinically significant symptoms, on average, defined as scores over 35 (Terhakopian et al., Reference Terhakopian, Sinaii, Engel, Schnurr and Hoge2008). Internal consistency (ω) for total PCL was 0.98 at the between-person level and 0.89 within-person.

Stressful life events

Life stress was self-reported at each visit using the Disaster Supplement of the Diagnostic Interview Schedule (Robins & Smith, Reference Robins and Smith1983), which includes a checklist of 18 events (e.g. job loss, injury, arrest). Items were summed into a life stress composite. The average number of events reported each year was 1.26 (SD = 1.52). Endorsement rates of specific stressors are in Supplementary Table S2.

PTSD chronicity

Chronicity of PTSD symptoms was indexed by averaging each participants PCL total scale scores across the entire monitoring period.

Mental health care expenditures

The annual cost (in dollars) of participant’s mental health care services were obtained from their electronic medical health records. This included expenditures on psychiatric medications, psychotherapy, medication management, other office visits, and inpatient treatment billed to WTC Health Program for psychiatric disorders. These data were available for years 2018–2022.

Analytic plan

Statistical models were estimated in R Version 4 (R Core Team, 2023) and Mplus Version 8 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2020), in conjunction with the Mplus Automation package in R (Hallquist & Wiley, Reference Hallquist and Wiley2018). We used multi-level structural equation models (MSEMs) for our analyses. The MSEMs used Bayes estimator with non-informative priors that are default in Mplus (Asparouhov & Muthén, Reference Asparouhov and Muthén2021). MSEM was used because it accounts for the nested structure of our data (i.e. time points within people) and it enables variables to be predictors and outcomes in the same model (i.e. bi-directional paths). Missing data was treated as missing at random and models were estimated using full information maximum likelihood techniques. Inferences were made from point estimates drawn from the posterior distribution and associated 95% credibility intervals. We considered parameter coefficients to be significantly different from zero if the 95% credibility interval did not include zero.

With MSEM, we decomposed the repeated measures of PTSD symptom severity and stressful events into between- and within-person latent variables. The between-person variables (i.e. intercepts) reflect individual differences in average levels of symptom severity and average stress exposure during the monitoring period. The within-person variables reflect fluctuations in symptoms and stress exposure away from a person’s average levels at a given monitoring visit.

The MSEM used for our analyses is depicted in Figure 1. Bidirectional processes linking symptoms and stress were estimated as random slopes at the within-person level. Stress reactivity was modeled by the cross-lagged association between stressful events t−1 → symptoms t, stress generation by the cross-lagged association between symptoms t−1 → stressful events t, symptom persistence by the autoregressive effect of symptoms t−1 → symptomst, and stress persistence by the autoregressive effect of stress events t−1 → stress events t. Symptoms and stress at the same visit were allowed to covary. The fixed effects of these within-person processes reflect patterns that characterize functioning on average.

Figure 1. Multi-level structural equation model of stress-symptom dynamics. Note: Stress-symptom dynamics are modeled at the within-person level and individual differences are modeled at the between-person level. Within-person variables reflect deviations in symptoms and stressful events from a person’s average at time t for person i. Between-person latent variables reflect individual differences in the strength of within-person processes and average levels of symptoms/stressors. Single-headed arrows with circles = random slopes. Double-headed arrows = correlations.

Random slopes were then estimated at the between-person level alongside the intercepts for symptom severity and stressful events. The random slopes capture between-person variation in the strength of within-person processes. All random intercepts were freely correlated. Correlations among the random slopes capture between-person covariation in processes. Uncorrelated slopes indicate they are relatively independent processes and people tend to present with one pattern or another (e.g. people are either more reactive or tend to generate stress) whereas correlated slopes suggest they are functionally related (e.g. people who are more reactive also generate more stress).

Finally, we evaluated the clinical utility of stress-symptom processes. To determine whether these processes contribute to the chronicity and severity of PTSD, we first examined between-person correlations among the random slopes and the PTSD symptom intercept (i.e. stable levels of symptom severity) within the MSEM framework. Then to increase the translational value of our results, we quantified clinical outcomes for people with varying levels of each mechanism. To do this, we first extracted individual-level within-person slope estimates. Slopes were estimated from the person-specific posterior distributions obtained as part of the within-person standardization procedure in Mplus (Schuurman et al., Reference Schuurman, Ferrer, de Boer-Sonnenschein and Hamaker2016). Next, we grouped people into deciles based on the strength of each stress-symptom slope. Because our interest was in evaluating outcomes for people with a high standing on these mechanisms, we focused on comparing outcomes for those in the top two deciles to those in the lower deciles. Namely, we compared the average PCL scores (i.e. chronicity) and total dollars spent on mental health services for people in upper and lower deciles of stress reactivity, stress generation, symptom persistence, and stress persistence.

Results

Key parameters for the MSEM are in Table 1.

Table 1. Key parameters of multi-level structural equation model of stress-symptom dynamics

Note: CI, credibility interval; IQR, interquartile range, * = credibility interval does not contain zero.

Average stress-symptom dynamics

The average within-person slopes for stress reactivity, stress generation, and symptom persistence were significant. Stress reactivity was the strongest effect and stress generation was the weakest. Stress persistence was not significant. These results indicate that for people exposed to trauma, symptoms tend to worsen after major life stressors, worsening symptoms lead to more stressors than usual, and it often take months or even years to return to baseline functioning after a symptom flare-up.

Individual variation and covariation of stress-symptom dynamics

There was significant variance in every random slope, with inter quartile ranges between 0.06 and 0.12 (model-estimated variance parameters are reported in Supplementary Table S3). Individual differences in the strength of stress-symptom dynamics are shown in Figure 2. Individual slope estimates ranged from strongly negative (minimum β = −.89) to strongly positive (maximum β = 0.98), although the majority of slopes were positive. This wide variability in the strength of stress-symptom dynamics highlights how the average within-person effects typically reported in longitudinal studies ignore considerable heterogeneity.

Figure 2. Individual differences in within-person stress-symptom dynamics. Note: Each point is an individual slope estimate. Stress reactivity is the individual’s average cross-lagged stress t−1 → symptom t slope; stress generation is the individual’s average cross-lagged symptom t−1 → stress t slope; symptom persistence is the individual’s average autoregressive symptom t−1 → symptom t slope; stress persistence is the individual’s average autoregressive stress t−1 → stress t slope.

Correlations among random slopes indicate that there are related and independent processes. Stress reactivity was correlated with stress generation. Additionally, stress reactivity, stress generation, and symptom persistence were correlated with stress persistence. Scatterplots of individual slope estimates visualize the co-occurrence of processes in Figure 3. As shown in the figure, these processes co-occur across the sample, and there are people in the top deciles of each slope that incur a ‘double hit’ of interlocking dynamics. In contrast to these correlated processes, symptom persistence was not correlated with stress reactivity or stress generation despite all being significant, on average. These results reinforce that finding multiple, significant paths at the sample level does not necessarily mean those processes are typical of any given individual.

Figure 3. Between-person covariation of stress-symptom dynamics. Note: Each point is an individual slope estimate. The x and y-axes are standardized beta coefficients. Stress reactivity is the individual’s average cross-lagged stress t−1 → symptom t slope; stress generation is the individual’s average cross-lagged symptom t−1 → stress t slope; symptom persistence is the individual’s average autoregressive symptom t−1 → symptom t slope; stress persistence is the individual’s average autoregressive stress t−1 → stress t slope.

In sum, we found that people whose symptoms worsen after stressful events, and those whose symptoms lead to more stressors, also tend to experience more enduring stressors, whereas people who tend to have a slow return to baseline after symptom flare-ups are not particularly sensitive to stressful events and their symptoms do not lead to more stress.

Chronicity and clinical burden associated with stress-symptom dynamics

After establishing there is significant variation in these processes, the final step of our analyses was to evaluate whether those individual differences are clinically meaningful. Starting with results in the MSEM (see Table 1), the stress reactivity and symptom persistence slopes were correlated with the symptom intercept (i.e. average levels of symptom severity).

Next, we examined associations between the four dynamics and chronicity of symptoms indexed by the observed, average total PCL score for people in each decile of slope estimates. People in the top two deciles of stress reactivity and symptom persistence, and top decile of stress generation and stress persistence, had clinically significant symptoms when averaged across the monitoring period. Chronicity of PTSD generally decreased in descending deciles of stress-symptom slopes, with people in most lower 8 deciles experiencing symptoms below cut-offs for clinical significance, on average. These results are shown in Figure 4a and reported in full in Supplementary Table S4.

Figure 4(a). (a) Mean PCL scores for people at upper and lower deciles of stress-symptom dynamics. Note: Dotted line = threshold for clinically significant symptoms (i.e. PCL total score > 35; Terhakopian et al., Reference Terhakopian, Sinaii, Engel, Schnurr and Hoge2008). The horizontal line in the box indicates mean level of PCL for that decile. (b) Mental health care expenditures for people at upper and lower deciles of stress-symptom dynamics. Note: Expenditures include cost of mental health care services and psychiatric medications.

Finally, we compared the annual expenditure on mental health services for people across deciles of stress reactivity, stress generation, symptom persistence, and stress persistence. These results are visualized in Figure 4b and reported in full in Supplementary Table S5. Individual differences in stress reactivity and symptom persistence had the most pronounced impact on service utilization; people in the top two deciles spent more per year than everyone in the respective lower 8 deciles combined ($1,019,029 vs. $998,596 for stress reactivity and $1,011,231 vs. $1,006,394 for symptom persistence). Put another way, mental health care for a person in the top decile of stress reactivity will cost over 10 times the amount that services cost for a person in the median decile ($812 vs. $78 per year), and services for those in the top decile of symptom persistence will cost over 15 times the amount than for people in the median decile ($884 vs. $56 per year). Likewise, although lower in absolute terms, services for people in the top deciles of stress generation and stress persistence cost 17% and 6.6% more than the median deciles, respectively.

Overall, these final analyses show that stress-symptom dynamics predict how severe a person’s symptoms will be over many years and how much mental health treatment they will need.

Role of initial trauma exposure severity

In supplementary analyses, we examined the role of WTC exposures (e.g. lost someone, duration on site) in stress-symptom dynamics with two sets of analyses. A full list of exposure frequencies is in the Supplementary Materials. First, given evidence that trauma exposure severity largely dictates the course of PTSD (Boasso et al., Reference Boasso, Steenkamp, Nash, Larson and Litz2015; Jakob et al., Reference Jakob, Lamp, Rauch, Smith and Buchholz2017), we tested whether stress-symptom dynamics predicted our clinical outcomes over and above exposure severity. The R 2 change from hierarchical regression models with 10 WTC exposures in block one and the four stress-dynamics in block two showed that dynamics accounted for an additional 7% of the variance in mental health service expenditures (block one R 2 = 0.01) and 29% of the variance in average PCL (block one R 2 = 0.05).

Next, we tested whether exposure severity moderated stress-symptom dynamics. Two theories make predictions about the effect of exposures on these dynamics: stress sensitization theory predicts trauma increases stress reactivity and kindling/stress autonomy theories posit exposures increase symptom persistence. These analyses extend results from a prior study of this sample showing that exposure severity moderated stress reactivity over 2 years (Zvolensky et al., Reference Zvolensky, Farris, Kotov, Schechter, Bromet, Gonzalez, Vujanovic, Pietrzak, Crane, Kaplan, Moline, Southwick, Feder, Udasin, Reissman and Luft2015a) by testing moderation over a much longer duration of time and for three additional stress-symptom dynamics. In these analyses, we regressed each stress-symptom dynamic on the 10 exposures in separate models, with the multiple R indexing moderation. Replicating prior work, we found that higher exposure severity associated with stronger stress reactivity (R = 0.10; p < 0.001). Additionally, exposure severity associated with stronger symptom persistence (R = 0.14; p < 0.001) and stress persistence (R = 0.10; p < 0.001), but not stress generation (R = 0.05; p = 0.295).

To summarize the role of WTC exposures, we showed that stress-symptom dynamics incrementally predict clinical outcomes over and above exposure severity and people who had more severe trauma tend to be reactive, have symptoms that persistent irrespective of major stressors, and have more enduring stress.

Discussion

We sought to identify dynamics reflecting key psychological maintenance mechanisms that govern the long-term course of PTSD. Our results show that a clinically important source of heterogeneity in PTSD are individual differences in dynamics. Specifically, stress reactivity, stress generation, symptom persistence, and stress persistence may reflect distinct sets of mechanisms that account for why some people exposed to trauma go on to experience a more chronic course of PTSD and need more treatment than others.

First, we established stable stress-symptom dynamics that characterize the long-term symptom trajectories of trauma-exposed individuals, on average. Specifically, we found that PTSD symptoms tend to increase following stressful events, increases in symptoms tend to precipitate stressful events, and symptomatic episodes tend to have a slow return to baseline levels. These extend prior longitudinal studies that have been limited to following participants for much shorter intervals by showing these dynamics continue to be relevant decades after the original traumatic event.

It should be noted that our life events measure lacked some information needed for optimal tests of stress generation and stress persistence, which may partially account for these being the weakest effects. For stress generation, it is typically conceptualized as symptoms preceding dependent stressors (i.e. those that result in part from person’s behavior/characteristics) rather than independent stressors (i.e. those that are fateful and happen irrespective of a person’s actions) (Hammen, Reference Hammen1991), but we were unable to make this distinction with our measure. Meta-analytically, symptoms across diagnoses do indeed predict dependent events most strongly, but they also predict independent events (Rnic et al., Reference Rnic, Santee, Hoffmeister, Liu, Chang, Chen and LeMoult2023). These prior findings suggest there may be indirect ways that symptoms create stress, such as symptom-driven choices in environments that increase exposure to stressors or difficulties with actively avoiding stressors when especially symptomatic (Hammen, Reference Hammen, Harkness and Hayden2020; Rnic, Reference Rnic, Santee, Hoffmeister, Liu, Chang, Chen, Neufeld, Machado, Starr, Dozois and LeMoult2023). Thus, it is likely our stress generation effects would have been stronger had we only included dependent events, but the relative strength of such direct and indirect generation paths related PTSD remains an open question. Likewise, for stress persistence, it is possible that significant effects would be found by focusing on events expected to penetrate multiple domains of life and lead to more stressors. In particular, chronic stressors may be especially likely to have this sort of permeating effect (Pearlin et al., Reference Pearlin, Aneshensel and LeBlanc1997), but our measure only included discrete events. Research using a life events measure with more context, and which include chronic stressors, is needed to more comprehensively test stress generation and stress proliferation effects in PTSD.

There were significant and clinically meaningful individual differences in these core stress-symptom dynamics that allowed us to describe and understand heterogeneity in trauma-related pathology. Each of these dynamics can be interpreted as a PTSD phenotype that reflects different mechanisms. Patterns of covariation between people in these dynamic phenotypes bring clarity to how the underlying mechanisms may actually play out. This type of mechanistic information could not be gleaned from averaged, within-person effects that has been the focus of prior work. We clarified that although stress reactivity and stress persistence are both present on average, they are uncorrelated between people. This result indicates that these often reflect two distinct clinical phenotypes: trauma-exposed individuals who are especially sensitive to stress and those who tend to have a slow return to baseline after an episode. Such separable pathways, in turn, suggest that the underlying mechanisms operate relatively independently.

We also found evidence for mechanisms that may work in concert to maintain PTSD. Our results showed that people who are more reactive to stress also tend to generate stress, which implies their condition is maintained via a maladaptive, self-perpetuating feedback loop. For example, avoidant behaviors could erode social supports or lead to poor job performance (Breslau, Lucia, & Davis, Reference Breslau, Lucia and Davis2004; Solomon & Mikulincer, Reference Solomon and Mikulincer1990; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Chung, Wang, Yu and Kenardy2021), or intrusive memories could lead to secondary depression and anxiety that compound impairments (Lawrence-Wood, Van Hooff, Baur, & McFarlane, Reference Lawrence-Wood, Van Hooff, Baur and McFarlane2016). Our findings also showed that people who experience more persistent stress are more reactive to stress, generate more stress, and have more persistent symptoms. The co-occurrence of these phenotypes may occur because exposure to chronically stressful environments makes it more difficult for people to recover from episodes (i.e. symptom persistence) or makes them more vulnerable to the effects of acute stressors (i.e. stress reactivity) (Cohen, Murphy, & Prather, Reference Cohen, Murphy and Prather2019; McEwen, Reference McEwen2017). Another possibility is that having stronger reactions (i.e. stress reactivity) or more enduring symptoms (i.e. symptom persistence) makes it harder for a person to properly manage stressors, which then prolongs stressful situations (Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Fiorillo, Rothbaum, Ressler and Michopoulos2018). Evidence for the interplay of stress persistence and stress-symptom processes lays the groundwork for future investigations that can home in on the mediating behaviors or circumstances that account for such indirect effects.

Exposure severity may also influence individual differences in stress-symptom dynamics. We found results consistent with two prominent theories of exposure-related mechanisms: consistent with stress sensitivity, exposure severity amplified the effects of stress on symptoms (i.e. stress reactivity) and consistent with kindling/stress autonomy, exposure severity related to a tendency to have symptoms that endure for years independently of major life events (i.e. symptom persistence). Additionally, although not posited by prevailing theory, we found that people exposed to more severe trauma tended to have more persistent stress. This could be because trauma creates conditions for more chronic stressors–for example, perhaps those with more severe exposures experienced more relationship strain after 9/11 (Hammock et al., Reference Hammock, Dreyer, Riaz, Clouston, McGlone and Luft2019) or developed cognitive impairment (Clouston et al., Reference Clouston, Hall, Kritikos, Bennett, DeKosky, Edwards and Luft2022), problems which could in turn make stressors more likely for many years. By connecting initial trauma exposure to individual differences in these dynamics, our provide new support for long-hypothesized etiological mechanisms, and suggest some that may be underexplored.

Clinical utility of stress-symptom dynamics and translation to practice

In our final analyses, we showed that mechanisms underlying variation in stress-symptom dynamics determine, in part, who will go on to have a more chronic and severe condition. There was evidence that all four dynamics we examined relate to clinical outcomes over and above the effects of WTC exposure severity. People in the top decile of the stress-symptom dynamics are at risk of an unremitting course of PTSD, with most people in these groups experiencing clinically significant symptoms on average across the monitoring period. Stress persistence and stress reactivity were most strongly related to the chronicity of PTSD, with clinically significant symptoms present in the top two deciles.

The more severe and recurrent symptoms experienced by people in the upper deciles of stress reactivity, stress generation, and symptom persistence translate into considerably higher need for mental health services. To put our results into perspective, if we assume that the proportion of total costs attributable to each decile of stress reactivity scales up to the United States population level, this group alone uses 36.5 billion dollars’ worth of mental health services every year (51% of the estimated $76.1 billion in annual, direct health care costs; Davis et al., Reference Davis, Schein, Cloutier, Gagnon-Sanschagrin, Maitland, Urganus, Guerin, Lefebvre and Houle2022). Thus, variation in these psychological processes have real-world consequences.

Our results have actionable implications for improving the clinical management of PTSD in the future. Prevailing practices for treating PTSD are essentially one-size-fits-all despite its well-known heterogeneity (Benish, Imel, & Wampold, Reference Benish, Imel and Wampold2008; Cloitre, Reference Cloitre2015), but it would be more effective to deliver interventions targeting the specific mechanisms driving a patient’s pathology. We advance this goal by providing a proof-of-concept for a mechanism-based method of classifying patients. From this starting point, future research can identify specific mechanisms related to each stress-symptom dynamic and develop treatments tailored to address them. If future research supports the clinical utility of these stress-symptom dynamics, and interventions can be designed around them, implementation would be relatively straight-forward. To accomplish this, a stressful life event checklist and the PCL could be regularly assessed at routine health care visits, entered into patient’s electronic records, and longitudinal patterns of stress and symptoms could be automatically analyzed to produce slope scores indexing levels of stress reactivity, stress generation, and symptom persistence. A learning health system could then provide data-driven, individualized intervention recommendations based on the patient’s slope scores updated at each visit. For example, when a patient with a pattern of high stress reactivity reports an uptick in stressors, this could trigger a recommendation for mindfulness-based stress reduction therapy (Polusny et al., Reference Polusny, Erbes, Thuras, Moran, Lamberty, Collins, Rodman and Lim2015). If a patient high on stress generation and reactivity endorsed stressful events, this could prompt a brief assessment of PTSD-related behaviors that lead to stressors (e.g. behavioral avoidance, flashbacks) to then determine a treatment capable of reversing the vicious cycle (e.g. exposure therapy, cognitive processing therapy; Haagen, Smid, Knipscheer, & Kleber, Reference Haagen, Smid, Knipscheer and Kleber2015). A patient high on symptom persistence, on the other hand, may not be especially sensitive to stressors, but instead an uptick in symptoms could trigger a recommendation for a brief, trauma-focused intervention to prevent a prolonged episode (Sloan & Marx, Reference Sloan and Marx2019).

Additionally, given the link we identified between stressor persistence and stress-symptom dynamics, interventions that address environmental/situational factors may be an under-appreciated component of PTSD treatment. If our results are supported by further research, this suggests that connecting patients with community resources targeted to the legal, vocational, social, financial, or housing-related stressors they are experiencing could have direct and indirect effects on their symptoms. Thus, our findings take one step toward a mechanism-based approach to classifying patients, which in turn can eventually inform development of targeted interventions that would allow clinics to allocate resources more efficiently and ultimately steer the long-term trajectory of PTSD.

Limitations

The contributions of our study must be qualified by some limitations. First, the sample may not be representative of all trauma-exposed populations. Demographically, the sample was mostly White and male. Given that the presentation and course of PTSD has been shown to differ across demographic groups (Asnaani & Hall-Clark, Reference Asnaani and Hall-Clark2017; Reynolds et al., Reference Reynolds, Pietrzak, Mackenzie, Chou and Sareen2016; Tolin & Foa, Reference Tolin and Foa2008), the long-term symptom dynamics that we found may not extend to people of other races or genders. Furthermore, the prevalence of post-9/11 PTSD in this sample is higher than what has been reported among trauma-exposed individuals in general population surveys (Bromet et al., Reference Bromet, Atwoli, Kawakami, Navarro-Mateu, Piotrowski, King and Kessler2017). Despite these limits to generalizability, our results would likely generalize to other high-priority populations, like veterans, who have a similar demographic make-up (U.S. Census Bureau, 2019) and rates of PTSD (Dohrenwend et al., Reference Dohrenwend, Turner, Turse, Lewis-Fernandez and Yager2008) as our sample. Second, because life events were self-reported, it is unclear the extent to which the observed effects are influenced by biased appraisals of events or memory biases. This complicates interpretation of mechanisms because it is possible, for example, that stress reactivity actually reflects a tendency to recall more stressors when in a symptomatic state rather than a response to objective increases in stressors. Brief, semi-structured interviews or the reports of close others (e.g. relatives) could help to mitigate potential biases in future work. Such methods could also allow for separating the effects of subjective stress appraisals from objective events, each of which likely play a role in maintaining symptoms (Lazarus & Folkman, Reference Lazarus and Folkman1984). Third, the life event checklist was limited to relatively major events rather than more minor day-to-day stressors. Accumulating evidence supports the role of daily hassles in maintaining psychopathology, including PTSD (e.g. Short, Boffa, Clancy, & Schmidt, Reference Short, Boffa, Clancy and Schmidt2018), thus, it may be useful in clinical practice to collect more frequent assessments of relatively minor stressors alongside annual major event checklists. Research comparing the clinical utility of assessing major events versus daily hassles could help to determine an optimal assessment protocol for identifying at-risk patients.

Conclusion

Our study showed that individual differences in stress-symptom dynamics can explain and predict heterogeneity in the long-term course of PTSD. We propose that these patterns of stress and symptoms can eventually inform the development of clinical protocols tailored to the psychological mechanisms maintaining a person’s problems. A mechanistically informed approach to classification and treatment can ultimately curb the chronicity and severity of PTSD and reduce economic burden on the individual and society.

Supplementary material

Supplementary information, code, and data needed to reproduce our analyses, as well as full model output are on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/crx9y/).

Funding statement

The study was funded by the NIOSH/CDC award 75D301-22-C-15522 to Benjamin J. Luft and R21OH012614 to Roman Kotov. We thank participants for their commitment to the project. The findings and conclusions in this article are those of the authors and do not represent the official position of NIOSH or CDC.

Competing interests

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

References

Asnaani, A., & Hall-Clark, B. (2017). Recent developments in understanding ethnocultural and race differences in trauma exposure and PTSD. Current opinion in psychology, 14, 96101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2021). Bayesian estimation of single and multilevel models with latent variable interactions. Structural Equation Modeling, 28(2), 314328. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2020.1761808CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benish, S. G., Imel, Z. E., & Wampold, B. E. (2008). The relative efficacy of bona fide psychotherapies for treating post-traumatic stress disorder: A meta-analysis of direct comparisons. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(5), 746758. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanchard, E. B., Jones-Alexander, J., Buckley, T. C., & Forneris, C. A. (1996). Psychometric properties of the PTSD checklist (PCL). Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(8), 669673. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(96)00033-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boasso, A. M., Steenkamp, M. M., Nash, W. P., Larson, J. L., & Litz, B. T. (2015). The relationship between course of PTSD symptoms in deployed US Marines and degree of combat exposure. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 28(1), 7378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breslau, N., Lucia, V. C., & Davis, G. C. (2004). Partial PTSD versus full PTSD: An empirical examination of associated impairment. Psychological Medicine, 34(7), 12051214. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291704002594CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., & Valentine, J. D. (2000). Meta-analysis of risk factors for posttraumatic stress disorder in trauma-exposed adults. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68(5), 748766. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.68.5.748CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bromet, E. J., Atwoli, L., Kawakami, N., Navarro-Mateu, F., Piotrowski, P., King, A. J., … & Kessler, R. C. (2017). Post-traumatic stress disorder associated with natural and human-made disasters in the World Mental Health Surveys. Psychological Medicine, 47(2), 227241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bromet, E. J., Hobbs, M. J., Clouston, S. A., Gonzalez, A., Kotov, R., & Luft, B. J. (2016). DSM-IV post-traumatic stress disorder among World Trade Center responders 11–13 years after the disaster of 11 September 2001 (9/11). Psychological Medicine, 46(4), 771783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryant, R. A., Galatzer-Levy, I., & Hadzi-Pavlovic, D. (2023). The heterogeneity of posttraumatic stress disorder in DSM-5. JAMA Psychiatry, 80(2), 189. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2022.4092CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryant, R. A., Nickerson, A., Creamer, M., O’Donnell, M., Forbes, D., Galatzer-Levy, I., McFarlane, A. C., & Silove, D. (2015). Trajectory of post-traumatic stress following traumatic injury: 6-year follow-up. British Journal of Psychiatry, 206(5), 417423. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.114.145516CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, S. B., Trachik, B., Goldberg, S., & Simpson, T. L. (2020). Identifying PTSD symptom typologies: A latent class analysis. Psychiatry Research, 285, 112779.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell-Sills, L., Sun, X., Choi, K. W., He, F., Ursano, R. J., Kessler, R. C., … & Stein, M. B. (2022). Dissecting the heterogeneity of posttraumatic stress disorder: differences in polygenic risk, stress exposures, and course of PTSD subtypes. Psychological Medicine, 52(15), 36463654.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Careaga, M. B. L., Girardi, C. E. N., & Suchecki, D. (2016). Understanding posttraumatic stress disorder through fear conditioning, extinction and reconsolidation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 71, 4857. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiu, H. T. S., Low, D. C. W., Chan, A. H. T., & Meiser-Stedman, R. (2024). Relationship between anxiety sensitivity and post-traumatic stress symptoms in trauma-exposed adults: A meta-analysis. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 103, 102857. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2024.102857CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cloitre, M. (2015). The “one size fits all” approach to trauma treatment: Should we be satisfied? European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 6(1), 27344. https://doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v6.27344CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clouston, S. A., Hall, C. B., Kritikos, M., Bennett, D. A., DeKosky, S., Edwards, J., … & Luft, B. J. (2022). Cognitive impairment and World Trade Centre-related exposures. Nature Reviews Neurology, 18(2), 103116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S., Murphy, M. L. M., & Prather, A. A. (2019). Ten surprising facts about stressful life events and disease risk. Annual Review of Psychology, 70(1), 577597. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-102857CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, L. M., & Baum, A. (1986). Chronic stress and posttraumatic stress disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54(3), 303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, L. L., Schein, J., Cloutier, M., Gagnon-Sanschagrin, P., Maitland, J., Urganus, A., Guerin, A., Lefebvre, P., & Houle, C. R. (2022). The economic burden of posttraumatic stress disorder in the United States from a societal perspective. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 83(3). https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.21m14116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dohrenwend, B. P., Turner, J. B., Turse, N. A., Lewis-Fernandez, R., & Yager, T. J. (2008). War-related posttraumatic stress disorder in Black, Hispanic, and majority White Vietnam veterans: The roles of exposure and vulnerability. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 21(2), 133141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A., & Clark, D. M. (2000). A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38(4), 319345. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00123-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foa, E. B., Steketee, G., & Rothbaum, B. O. (1989). Behavioral/cognitive conceptualizations of post-traumatic stress disorder. Behavior Therapy, 20(2), 155176. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(89)80067-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galatzer-Levy, I. R., Huang, S. H., & Bonanno, G. A. (2018). Trajectories of resilience and dysfunction following potential trauma: A review and statistical evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 63, 4155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2018.05.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galea, S., Ahern, J., Tracy, M., Hubbard, A., Cerda, M., Goldmann, E., & Vlahov, D. (2008). Longitudinal determinants of posttraumatic stress in a population-based cohort study. Epidemiology, 19(1), 4754. https://doi.org/10.1097/EDE.0b013e31815c1dbfCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haagen, J. F. G., Smid, G. E., Knipscheer, J. W., & Kleber, R. J. (2015). The efficacy of recommended treatments for veterans with PTSD: A metaregression analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 40, 184194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2015.06.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallquist, M. N., & Wiley, J. F. (2018). MplusAutomation: An R package for facilitating large-scale latent variable analyses in M plus. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 25(4), 621638. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2017.1402334CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hammen, C. (1991). Generation of stress in the course of unipolar depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100(4), 555561. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.555CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammen, C. (2020). Stress generation and depression. In Harkness, K. L. & Hayden, E. P. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of stress and mental health (pp. 331347). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hammock, AC, Dreyer, RE, Riaz, M, Clouston, SAP, McGlone, A, Luft, B. (2019). Trauma and relationship strain: Oral histories with World Trade Center disaster responders. Qualitative Health Research, 29(12):17511765. https://doi.org/10.1177/1049732319837534CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hilton, N. Z., Ham, E., Rodrigues, N. C., Kirsh, B., Chapovalov, O., & Seto, M. C. (2020). Contribution of critical events and chronic stressors to PTSD symptoms among psychiatric workers. Psychiatric Services, 71(3), 221227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoskins, M., Pearce, J., Bethell, A., Dankova, L., Barbui, C., Tol, W. A., van Ommeren, M., de Jong, J., Seedat, S., Chen, H., & Bisson, J. I. (2015). Pharmacotherapy for post-traumatic stress disorder: Systematic review and meta-analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 206(2), 93100. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.114.148551CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jakob, J. M., Lamp, K., Rauch, S. A., Smith, E. R., & Buchholz, K. R. (2017). The impact of trauma type or number of traumatic events on PTSD diagnosis and symptom severity in treatment seeking veterans. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 205(2), 8386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelley, L. P., Weathers, F. W., McDevitt-Murphy, M. E., Eakin, D. E., & Flood, A. M. (2009). A comparison of PTSD symptom patterns in three types of civilian trauma. Journal of Traumatic Stress: Official Publication of The International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies, 22(3), 227235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., & Gardner, C. O. (2016). Depressive vulnerability, stressful life events and episode onset of major depression: A longitudinal model. Psychological Medicine, 46(9), 18651874. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716000349CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawrence-Wood, E., Van Hooff, M., Baur, J., & McFarlane, A. C. (2016). Re-experiencing phenomena following a disaster: The long-term predictive role of intrusion symptoms in the development of post-trauma depression and anxiety. Journal of Affective Disorders, 190, 278281. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.10.029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. Springer.Google Scholar
Lowe, S. R., Joshi, S., Galea, S., Aiello, A. E., Uddin, M., Koenen, K. C., & Cerdá, M. (2017). Pathways from assaultive violence to post-traumatic stress, depression, and generalized anxiety symptoms through stressful life events: Longitudinal mediation models. Psychological Medicine, 47(14), 25562566. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717001143CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lowe, S. R., Walsh, K., Uddin, M., Galea, S., & Koenen, K. C. (2014). Bidirectional relationships between trauma exposure and posttraumatic stress: A longitudinal study of Detroit residents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(3), 533544. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037046CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maniates, H., Stoop, T. B., Miller, M. W., Halberstadt, L., & Wolf, E. J. (2018). Stress-generative effects of posttraumatic stress disorder: Transactional associations between posttraumatic stress disorder and stressful life events in a longitudinal sample. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 31(2), 191201. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.22269CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marmar, C. R., Schlenger, W., Henn-Haase, C., Qian, M., Purchia, E., Li, M., Corry, N., Williams, C. S., Ho, C.-L., Horesh, D., Karstoft, K.-I., Shalev, A., & Kulka, R. A. (2015). Course of posttraumatic stress disorder 40 years after the Vietnam War: Findings from the National Vietnam Veterans Longitudinal Study. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(9), 875. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0803CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McEwen, B. S. (2017). Neurobiological and systemic effects of chronic stress. Chronic Stress, 1, 2470547017692328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milan, S., Zona, K., Acker, J., & Turcios-Cotto, V. (2013). Prospective risk factors for adolescent PTSD: Sources of differential exposure and differential vulnerability. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(2), 339353. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9677-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monroe, S. M., & Harkness, K. L. (2005). Life stress, the “kindling” hypothesis, and the recurrence of depression: Considerations from a life stress perspective. Psychological Review, 112(2), 417445. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.112.2.417CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L., & Muthén, B. (2020). Mplus (8.5) [Computer software].Google Scholar
Nievergelt, C. M., Maihofer, A. X., Klengel, T., Atkinson, E. G., Chen, C.-Y., Choi, K. W., Coleman, J. R. I., Dalvie, S., Duncan, L. E., Gelernter, J., Levey, D. F., Logue, M. W., Polimanti, R., Provost, A. C., Ratanatharathorn, A., Stein, M. B., Torres, K., Aiello, A. E., Almli, L. M., … Koenen, K. C. (2019). International meta-analysis of PTSD genome-wide association studies identifies sex- and ancestry-specific genetic risk loci. Nature Communications, 10(1), 4558. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-12576-wCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearlin, L. I., Aneshensel, C. S., & LeBlanc, A. J. (1997). The forms and mechanisms of stress proliferation: the case of AIDS caregivers. Journal of health and social behavior, 38(3), 223236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polusny, M. A., Erbes, C. R., Thuras, P., Moran, A., Lamberty, G. J., Collins, R. C., Rodman, J. L., & Lim, K. O. (2015). Mindfulness-based stress reduction for posttraumatic stress disorder among veterans: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA, 314(5), 456. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2015.8361CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Post, R. M., & Weiss, S. R. (1998). Sensitization and kindling phenomena in mood, anxiety, and obsessive-compulsive disorders: the role of serotonergic mechanisms in illness progression. Biological Psychiatry, 44(3), 193206. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00144-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Post, R. M., Weiss, S. R., Smith, M., Li, H., & McCann, U. (1997). Kindling versus quenching. Implications for the evolution and treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 821, 285295. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1997.tb48287.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2023). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Reynolds, K., Pietrzak, R. H., Mackenzie, C. S., Chou, K. L., & Sareen, J. (2016). Post-traumatic stress disorder across the adult lifespan: findings from a nationally representative survey. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 24(1), 8193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rnic, K., Santee, A. C., Hoffmeister, J.-A., Liu, H., Chang, K. K., Chen, R. X., Neufeld, R. W. J., Machado, D. A., Starr, L. R., Dozois, D. J. A., & LeMoult, J. (2023). The vicious cycle of psychopathology and stressful life events: A meta-analytic review testing the stress generation model. Psychological Bulletin, 149(5-6), 330369. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000390CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rnic, K., Santee, A. C., Hoffmeister, J. A., Liu, H., Chang, K. K., Chen, R. X., … & LeMoult, J. (2023). The vicious cycle of psychopathology and stressful life events: A meta-analytic review testing the stress generation model. Psychological Bulletin, 149(5–6), 330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robins, L. N., and Smith, E. M. (1983). The Diagnostic Interview Schedule/Disaster Supplement. Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis.Google Scholar
Sadeh, N., Miller, M. W., Wolf, E. J., & Harkness, K. L. (2015). Negative emotionality and disconstraint influence PTSD symptom course via exposure to new major adverse life events. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 31, 2027. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.01.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmied, E. A., Larson, G. E., Highfill-McRoy, R. M., & Thomsen, C. J. (2016). Reciprocal relationships between stressors and mental health problems in military veterans. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 35(9), 705721. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2016.35.9.705CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuurman, N. K., Ferrer, E., de Boer-Sonnenschein, M., & Hamaker, E. L. (2016). How to compare cross-lagged associations in a multilevel autoregressive model. Psychological Methods, 21(2), 206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seligowski, A. V., Lee, D. J., Bardeen, J. R., & Orcutt, H. K. (2015). Emotion regulation and posttraumatic stress symptoms: A meta-analysis. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 44(2), 87102. https://doi.org/10.1080/16506073.2014.980753CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Short, N. A., Boffa, J. W., Clancy, K., & Schmidt, N. B. (2018). Effects of emotion regulation strategy use in response to stressors on PTSD symptoms: An ecological momentary assessment study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 230, 7783. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.12.063CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sloan, D. M., & Marx, B. P. (2019). Written exposure therapy for PTSD: A brief treatment approach for mental health professionals. American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Smid, G. E., Kleber, R. J., Rademaker, A. R., Van Zuiden, M., & Vermetten, E. (2013). The role of stress sensitization in progression of posttraumatic distress following deployment. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 48(11), 17431754. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-013-0709-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smid, G. E., Van Der Velden, P. G., Lensvelt-Mulders, G. J. L. M., Knipscheer, J. W., Gersons, B. P. R., & Kleber, R. J. (2012). Stress sensitization following a disaster: A prospective study. Psychological Medicine, 42(8), 16751686. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291711002765CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solomon, Z., & Mikulincer, M. (1990). Life events and combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder: The intervening role of locus of control and social support. Military Psychology, 2(4), 241256. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327876mp0204_4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, Z., & Mikulincer, M. (2006). Trajectories of PTSD: a 20-year longitudinal study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(4), 659666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steenkamp, M. M., Litz, B. T., Hoge, C. W., & Marmar, C. R. (2015). Psychotherapy for military-related PTSD: A review of randomized clinical trials. JAMA, 314(5), 489500. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2015.8370CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steenkamp, M. M., Litz, B. T., & Marmar, C. R. (2020). First-line psychotherapies for military-related PTSD. JAMA, 323(7), 656657. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2019.20825CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinert, C., Hofmann, M., Leichsenring, F., & Kruse, J. (2015). The course of PTSD in naturalistic long-term studies: high variability of outcomes. A systematic review. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 69(7), 483496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Terhakopian, A., Sinaii, N., Engel, C. C., Schnurr, P. P., & Hoge, C. W. (2008). Estimating population prevalence of posttraumatic stress disorder: An example using the PTSD checklist. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 21(3), 290300. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.20341CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, K. M., Hopwood, C. J., Donnellan, M. B., Wright, A. G., Sanislow, C. A., McDevitt-Murphy, M. E., … & Morey, L. C. (2014). Personality heterogeneity in PTSD: distinct temperament and interpersonal typologies. Psychological assessment, 26(1), 23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, N. J., Fiorillo, D., Rothbaum, B. O., Ressler, K. J., & Michopoulos, V. (2018). Coping strategies as mediators in relation to resilience and posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 225, 153159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tolin, D. F., & Foa, E. B. (2008). Sex differences in trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder: a quantitative review of 25 years of research.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
U.S. Census Bureau. (2019). Veteran status. 2019 American Community Survey 5-Year Estimates Subject tables. Retrieved from https://data.census.gov/table/ACSST5Y2019.S2101?q=s2101Google Scholar
Von Der Warth, R., Dams, J., Grochtdreis, T., & König, H.-H. (2020). Economic evaluations and cost analyses in posttraumatic stress disorder: A systematic review. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 11(1), 1753940. https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2020.1753940CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y., Chung, M. C., Wang, N., Yu, X., & Kenardy, J. (2021). Social support and posttraumatic stress disorder: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 85, 101998. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2021.101998CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zvolensky, M. J., Farris, S. G., Kotov, R., Schechter, C. B., Bromet, E., Gonzalez, A., Vujanovic, A., Pietrzak, R. H., Crane, M., Kaplan, J., Moline, J., Southwick, S. M., Feder, A., Udasin, I., Reissman, D. B., & Luft, B. J. (2015a). World Trade Center disaster and sensitization to subsequent life stress: A longitudinal study of disaster responders. Preventive Medicine, 75, 7074. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2015.03.017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zvolensky, M. J., Kotov, R., Schechter, C. B., Gonzalez, A., Vujanovic, A., Pietrzak, R. H., Crane, M., Kaplan, J., Moline, J., Southwick, S. M., Feder, A., Udasin, I., Reissman, D. B., & Luft, B. J. (2015b). Post-disaster stressful life events and WTC-related posttraumatic stress, depressive symptoms, and overall functioning among responders to the World Trade Center disaster. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 61, 97105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Multi-level structural equation model of stress-symptom dynamics. Note: Stress-symptom dynamics are modeled at the within-person level and individual differences are modeled at the between-person level. Within-person variables reflect deviations in symptoms and stressful events from a person’s average at time t for person i. Between-person latent variables reflect individual differences in the strength of within-person processes and average levels of symptoms/stressors. Single-headed arrows with circles = random slopes. Double-headed arrows = correlations.

Figure 1

Table 1. Key parameters of multi-level structural equation model of stress-symptom dynamics

Figure 2

Figure 2. Individual differences in within-person stress-symptom dynamics. Note: Each point is an individual slope estimate. Stress reactivity is the individual’s average cross-lagged stresst−1 → symptomt slope; stress generation is the individual’s average cross-lagged symptomt−1 → stresst slope; symptom persistence is the individual’s average autoregressive symptomt−1 → symptomt slope; stress persistence is the individual’s average autoregressive stresst−1 → stresst slope.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Between-person covariation of stress-symptom dynamics. Note: Each point is an individual slope estimate. The x and y-axes are standardized beta coefficients. Stress reactivity is the individual’s average cross-lagged stresst−1 → symptomt slope; stress generation is the individual’s average cross-lagged symptomt−1 → stresst slope; symptom persistence is the individual’s average autoregressive symptomt−1 → symptomt slope; stress persistence is the individual’s average autoregressive stresst−1 → stresst slope.

Figure 4

Figure 4(a). (a) Mean PCL scores for people at upper and lower deciles of stress-symptom dynamics. Note: Dotted line = threshold for clinically significant symptoms (i.e. PCL total score > 35; Terhakopian et al., 2008). The horizontal line in the box indicates mean level of PCL for that decile. (b) Mental health care expenditures for people at upper and lower deciles of stress-symptom dynamics. Note: Expenditures include cost of mental health care services and psychiatric medications.