Hostname: page-component-5b777bbd6c-v47t2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-06-19T05:02:26.694Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Between and within-person relations between psychological wellbeing and distress in adolescence: A random intercept cross-lagged panel examination

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 May 2025

Hena Thakur*
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Social Sciences, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, IL, USA Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, USA
Jae Wan Choi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, USA
Jeff R. Temple
Affiliation:
School of Behavioral Health Sciences, UTHealth, Houston, TX, USA
Joseph R. Cohen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, USA
*
Corresponding author: Hena Thakur; Email: hena.thakur@northwestern.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Holistic frameworks of mental health outline that a focus on psychopathology does not represent an optimal approach to defining, measuring and treating mental health. Rather, theoretical, empirical, and applied psychological efforts should incorporate psychological well-being (PWB). Studies of PWB have overwhelmingly focused on adult populations, rendering a translation down to adolescence difficult. The current study explores the between-person, as well as within-person short-term, prospective relations between psychopathology and wellbeing within a community sample of adolescents (i.e., 553 youth aged 12 – 18, mean age: 14.97 years, 51.2% Male, 40.7% of participants identified as Hispanic (225 individuals), 38.5% identified as White (213 individuals), and 35.6% identified as Black (197 individuals), 3-wave, 1-year survey). Results demonstrated significant, negative between-person relations between psychopathology and PWB (bPHQ = −0.25, SE = 0.11, p = 0.021, bVDS = −0.39, SE = 0.15, p = 0.011). At the within-person level, consistent positive prospective relations were identified for violent-delinquent behaviors and PWB, such that increases in individual levels of violent-delinquent behaviors tended to forecast higher levels of PWB at the next follow-up (bPWBW2 = 0.21, SEPWBW2 = 0.076, p < 0.01; bPWBW3 = 0.14, SEPWBW3 = 0.051, p < 0.01). At the within-person level, prospective relations between depressive and PWB were not identified. Gender and racial/ethnic identities did not moderate findings.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Holistic frameworks of mental health theorize that positive states of being do not occur in the absence of psychopathology, but rather simultaneously (e.g., Keyes et al., Reference Keyes, Dhingra and Simoes2010). Translationally, these frameworks posit that decreasing or ameliorating distress symptoms is a suboptimal threshold within the field of mental health, and instead, efforts should focus on conceptualizing and promoting psychological well-being as well (e.g., Howell et al., Reference Howell, Keyes, Passmore, Proctor and Linley2013). To date, however, a holistic framework has not been comprehensively empirically explored, which inhibits its translation into mental health practice. Given increasing interest in clinical initiatives that utilize holistic mental health treatment within adolescence (e.g., Ruini et al., Reference Ruini, Ottolini, Tomba, Belaise, Albieri, Visani, Offidani, Caffo and Fava2009), our study focused on the prospective relation between psychological well-being (PWB) and subtypes of psychopathology. Specifically, given the personal and societal costs associated with adolescent depression (combination of some or all of the following symptoms: sadness, anhedonia, changes in appetite, fatigue, psychomotor agitation or retardation, sleep difficulties, worthlessness or guilt, concentration difficulties, persistent thoughts of death; Petito et al., Reference Petito, Pop, Namazova-Baranova, Mestrovic, Nigri, Vural, Sacco, Giardino, Ferrara and Pettoello-Mantovani2020) and violent-delinquent behaviors (Cohen & Piquero, Reference Cohen and Piquero2009; e.g., tendency to use physical force in interpersonal interactions, nonphysical aggression; Broidy et al., Reference Broidy, Nagin, Tremblay, Bates, Brame, Dodge, Fergusson, Horwood, Loeber, Laird, Lynam, Moffitt, Pettit and Vitaro2003), we examined how PWB may influence, or be influenced, by these two outcomes.

Psychological wellbeing (PWB) reflects personal fulfillment and the realization of one’s potential (Keyes, Reference Keyes2007), which, in turn, fosters goals and meaning in life (Ryan & Deci, Reference Ryan and Deci2001). Ryff’s measure of PWB (1989) is an important index of eudaimonic wellbeing and conceptualization of this construct consists of several facets, including self-acceptance (holding a positive view of oneself even with awareness of one’s limitations), positive relations with others (developing and maintaining close and trusting relationships with others), environmental mastery (shaping one’s environment to meet needs), autonomy (evaluating oneself by their own standards/resistance to societal pressures to behave or think a certain way), purpose in life (meaning making in challenges, having goals and a sense of direction in life), and personal growth (capitalizing on personal strengths). As such, PWB represents tasks that individuals navigate when realizing their potential, capacity or strengths (Ryff et al., Reference Ryff, Boylan, Kirsch, Lee, Kubzansky and VanderWeele2021). Previous literature examining the psychometric properties of PWB (e.g., Gao & McLellan, Reference Gao and McLellan2018; Thakur et al., Reference Thakur, Choi, Andrews, Temple and Cohen2024) provides support for the reliable and valid measurement of this construct within adolescence. Despite growing empirical interest in this index, few studies examine the bidirectional relation between PWB and psychopathology, precluding an understanding of its role in mental health. At the cross-sectional level, correlational studies demonstrate small to medium negative associations between psychopathology and PWB (depressive symptoms: Ford et al., Reference Ford, Hurd, Jagers and Sellers2013; depressive symptoms and externalizing behaviors; Inguglia et al., Reference Inguglia, Ingoglia, Liga, Lo Coco and Lo Cricchio2015), suggesting the two relate to each other. Among adults, there is a significant bidirectional relation between distress and positive well-being (which combines PWB and hedonic well-being); however, changes in psychopathology better account for variance in future positive mental health outcomes than vice versa (Lamers et al., Reference Lamers, Westerhof, Glas and Bohlmeijer2015). To date, it is unclear though how PWB may relate to distress in adolescents. Recent research demonstrates that PWB may reflect a single factor in adolescents comprised of Ryff’s original domains (Thakur et al., Reference Thakur, Choi, Andrews, Temple and Cohen2024) as opposed to the multi-faceted construct in adulthood (Ryff & Keyes, Reference Ryff and Keyes1995). This suggests that PWB may present differently in adolescents and have distinct relations to other forms of well-being. Thus, given these developmental differences, separating PWB from hedonic well-being and examining its prospective relation to psychopathology within adolescence, is important for developing holistic models of mental health that are developmentally appropriate.

A secondary aim of the present study sought to parse between and within-person relations between positive and negative forms of mental health. Separating between and within-person effects is not only important for statistical conceptualizations of psychological phenomenon (Hamaker et al., Reference Hamaker, Kuiper and Grasman2015) but also has significant translational implications. Specifically, between-person effects can help inform population mental health efforts (e.g., screening), while the effects at the individual level can provide insight into the cascading effects of intervening with a given psychological construct. To date, Kraiss and colleagues (Reference Kraiss, Kohlhoff and Ten Klooster2022) represents one of the few empirical investigations exploring between- and within-person associations between internalizing distress (i.e., anxiety and depressive symptoms) and wellbeing (i.e., PWB and hedonic wellbeing). In a sample of 25 university students, the authors conducted an intensive longitudinal design study and, using a multilevel modeling framework, found a negative relation between psychological distress and wellbeing at the between and within-levels. In addition, Joshanloo & Blasco-Belled (Reference Joshanloo and Blasco-Belled2023) used a random-intercept cross lagged panel approach to understand the relations between depressive symptoms, eudaimonic wellbeing and life satisfaction in a sample of 17,056 participants with a mean age of 58.8 years at the first timepoint. The authors found that all cross-lagged effects were significant, with life satisfaction and eudaimonic wellbeing demonstrating positive effects and depressive symptoms demonstrating negative effects with life satisfaction and eudaimonic wellbeing. These findings, when combined with Lamers and colleagues (Reference Lamers, Westerhof, Glas and Bohlmeijer2015), suggest that, if replicated in a larger, more representative sample of adolescents across a longer duration of time, reducing psychological distress across the lifespan may be beneficial for increasing PWB.

Importantly, the above literature limited their studies to examining linear combinations of positive and negative mental health states, despite potential for the relations to be non-linear. For instance, although emotional clarity is positively and linearly associated with PWB (Augusto-Landa et al., Reference Augusto-Landa, Pulido-Martos and Lopez-Zafra2011), others have demonstrated that higher and lower levels of emotional clarity are associated with increased internalizing distress (Park & Naragon-Gainey, Reference Park and Naragon-Gainey2019). This suggests that there may be processes that influence the relation between internalizing distress and PWB, such that there are comparable, nonlinear presentations of distress across the spectrum of PWB profiles. Considering curvilinear relations may also be important for externalizing (e.g., violent or rule-breaking behaviors) forms of psychopathology. Within adolescence, risk taking can be adaptive and socially reinforced until a certain limit, as it is associated with a variety of functional outcomes that may pave the way toward higher PWB (e.g., pursuit of goals; Sijtsema et al., Reference Sijtsema, Lindenberg, Ojanen and Salmivalli2020). Further, Panayiotou and Humphrey (Reference Panayiotou and Humphrey2018) found that externalizing problems in adolescence contribute to reduced emotional distress in the future. The authors hypothesized this may be due to social support from delinquent peers leading to higher self-esteem at this developmental stage, as well as increased attention from adults, which can serve as a protective factor in the aftermath of future stressors. Thus, in line with broader literature examining mental health as a non-linear phenomenon (e.g., Seery et al., Reference Seery, Holman and Silver2010), it may be that externalizing symptoms demonstrate a quadratic existence with wellbeing, such that lower and higher levels of externalizing symptoms correspond to lower states of PWB. As such, another aim of the current study was to examine the linearity of the relations between PWB and psychopathology.

Finally, given the established role of adolescent identity in distress outcomes (e.g., Nolen-Hoeksema & Girgus, Reference Nolen-Hoeksema and Girgus1994), it is important to consider how the relation between psychopathology and PWB varies across subpopulations. For example, females are more likely to ruminate, which may sometimes lead towards positive outcomes (e.g., reflection), but also may engage in repetitive thought processes that lead to emotional distress (e.g., brooding; Burwell & Shirk, Reference Burwell and Shirk2007). Therefore, it is possible that a closer relation between internalizing distress and PWB exists for girls relative to boys. For externalizing symptoms findings are mixed, with some showing that overt forms of aggression are associated with adaptive functioning among adolescent boys (e.g., Warren et al., Reference Warren, Wray-Lake, Rote and Shubert2016), other studies showing that externalizing behaviors are equivalently positive across genders (e.g., popularity; Prinstein & Cillessen, Reference Prinstein and Cillessen2003), and other research suggesting more negative consequences for rule-breaking in boys relative to girls (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Thompson, Watson, Montena and Warren2023). Thus, explicit research on how PWB and externalizing behaviors may vary across genders is necessary. Meanwhile, the relation between PWB and psychopathology may also vary as a function of racial/ethnic identity due to cultural differences. Specifically, investigators caution against viewing PWB as a desirable or beneficial mental health outcome within individuals belonging to a more collectivistic identity (e.g., Christopher, Reference Christopher1999), as the principles underlying PWB appear antithetical to those identities (mastery of one’s environment versus achieving harmony with one’s environment; e.g., Harding et al., Reference Harding, Hughes and Way2017; Joshanloo, Reference Joshanloo2013). As individuals belonging to certain racial/ethnic identities in the United States (e.g., those identifying as Black or Latinx) may have a more collectivistic orientation (Markus & Kitayama, Reference Markus and Kitayama1991), PWB and psychopathology may be more closely related in White adolescents, for whom individualistic qualities are reinforced. Thus, the current study additionally aims to examine differences in relations between PWB and psychopathology as they relate to adolescent identity.

The present investigation

The current investigation aims to understand the between- and within-person relations between two important constructs of mental health, PWB and psychopathology (depressive symptoms and externalizing behaviors), within adolescence. Data from a short-term (1 year), three-wave study is examined within a longitudinal, random intercept cross-lagged panel analytic approach to determine: if the cross-sectional and longitudinal relations between psychopathology and wellbeing are linear or curvilinear (Aim 1), the magnitudes of the cross-sectional relations between different forms of psychopathology and wellbeing (Aim 2), the directionalities and magnitudes of the longitudinal relations between psychopathology and wellbeing (i.e., cross-lagged paths) (Aim 3), and the impact of gender and racial/ethnic identity on the paths between psychopathology and wellbeing (Aim 4). It was hypothesized that, in line with previous literature highlighting discrepancies in the relation between PWB and psychopathology (i.e., Ansary & Luthar, Reference Ansary and Luthar2009; Salmivalli et al., Reference Salmivalli, Kaukiainen and Lagerspetz2000), a quadratic approach would be incrementally valid to a linear conceptualization (Aim 1), such that moderate levels of internalizing distress and externalizing behaviors related to the highest levels of PWB. With regard to cross-sectional relations (Aim 2), it was hypothesized that both internalizing distress and externalizing behaviors would have a small-moderate negative effect size similar to what has been previously found in the adolescent literature for internalizing distress and PWB (e.g., Ford et al., Reference Ford, Hurd, Jagers and Sellers2013). In light of prior work demonstrating that psychopathology (i.e., primarily indicators of internalizing distress) uniquely predicts PWB in adults, it was hypothesized that psychopathology would prospectively predict PWB (Aim 3) as opposed to PWB predicting psychopathology. Finally, we predicted that internalizing distress and PWB would be more closely associated in girls relative to boys, while all other potential demographic differences were probed in an exploratory fashion (Aim 4).

Methods

Participants

The baseline sample for the current study stems from an overarching study examining mental health outcomes among adversity-exposed youth. Specifically, youth lived either in a large, urban setting in southeast Texas (N = 507), or were connected with a juvenile justice system (JJS) diversion program in a small city in Illinois (N = 77). Youth were connected to the diversion program in various ways including being referred by police for a low-level infraction, referred by the school for behavioral problems (e.g., truancy), or through community events targeting families who are potentially at-risk for involvement with the JJS. Demographically, the Texas and Illinois samples were similar, with the only significant differences being across gender and race/ethnicity. Specifically, the sample from Texas more commonly identified as Hispanic (48% relative to 8%), χ2(1) = 42.05; p < 0.05, and the sample in Illinois tended to identify more as White (50% relative to 37%), χ2(2) = 6.39; p < 0.05. Finally, the only other differences were that the Illinois sample was lower on PWB at baseline, t (551) = −4.02; p < 0.01, and violent behavior at Wave 3 (t(344.06) = −3.65; p < 0.01). These differences between recruitment sources reflected demographic compositions of the sites, and did not exhibit consistent, systematic differences in the constructs being examined. As such, and in line with large public domain datasets for vulnerable populations, such as the Longitudinal Studies on Child Abuse and Neglect, which intentionally recruit and combine samples from geographically different sites to ensure findings are not contributed to specific subsamples and demonstrate variability in outcomes of interest (Runyan et al., Reference Runyan, Dubowitz, English, Kotch, Litrownik and Thompson2014), we perform analyses by including participants from both sites. Of the 584 participants in the overall study, 553 participants completed traditional mental health self-report forms (i.e., depressive and violent-delinquent behaviors) and PWB at baseline. Approximately 71% (N = 391) completed measures at Wave 2, while approximately 66% (N = 364) completed measures at Wave 3. Within the analytic sample, the mean age was 14.97 years (SD = 1.21), and 51.2% of the sample identified as male at baseline. 40.7% of participants identified as Hispanic (225 individuals), 38.5% identified as White (213 individuals), and 35.6% identified as Black (197 individuals). Percentages total over 100, as participants were able to identify with multiple categories.

Procedure

Youth in the southeast Texas sample were introduced to the study within school settings, and if interested, youth were provided with parental consent forms. Youth located in the midwestern United States sample were referred to the study by a case manager at a juvenile diversion program or were introduced to the study at a community event sponsored in part by the juvenile diversion program. At the event, if youth were alone, they were provided with consent-to-contact forms to sign so that the study team could contact caregivers. Subsequently, caregivers accompanied adolescents to the baseline session to sign the consent form. After this stage, procedures were identical across sites. Subsequent to providing assent, adolescents participated in a 45-minute baseline survey that assessed demographic characteristics, life events, and mental health. Approximately 6 months (M = 5.44) after the baseline visit (beginning in March 2020), adolescents completed a second survey (Wave 2) and a third survey (Wave 3) approximately six months later (M = 6.13) (beginning in October 2020). The survey protocol was identical across waves. The institutional review board at the last author’s institution approved this study.

Measures

Depressive symptoms (PHQ, waves 1 – 3)

Adolescent depression was assessed using the nine-item Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9; Kroenke et al., Reference Kroenke, Spitzer and Williams2001). Example items included, “Trouble falling asleep, staying asleep, or sleeping too much” and “Trouble concentrating on things like school work, reading, or watching TV.” Reflecting on the previous two weeks, adolescents indicated the frequency of symptoms across response options, including not at all (0), several days (1), more than half the days (2), and nearly every day (3). Previous empirical literature demonstrates the reliable and valid use of this measure among adolescents (Richardson et al., Reference Richardson, McCauley, Grossman, McCarty, Richards, Russo, Rockhill and Katon2010). Within the current study, the alpha reliability for this measure was acceptable at all timepoints of the study (αW1 = 0.92, αW2 = 0.90, αW3 = 0.93).

Violent-delinquent behaviors (VDS, waves 1 – 3)

To understand adolescent engagement in violent-delinquent behaviors, youth answered the eight-item Violence Delinquency Scale (VDS; Broidy et al., Reference Broidy, Nagin, Tremblay, Bates, Brame, Dodge, Fergusson, Horwood, Loeber, Laird, Lynam, Moffitt, Pettit and Vitaro2003). Reflecting on the past 6 months, youth answered items regarding activities, including “I participated in fighting with weapons” and “I participated in carrying a weapon that is not a gun”, with response options including never, once or twice, a few times, many times, and not in the past 6 months but in my lifetime. The variable was coded as such: never/not in the past 6 months but in my lifetime (1), once or twice (2), a few times (3), and many times (4). Past empirical literature has demonstrated the reliable and valid use of the VDS (Gold et al., Reference Gold, Sullivan and Lewis2011). Within the current study, the alpha reliability for this measure was acceptable at all timepoints of the study (αW1 = 0.84, αW2 = 0.84, αW3 = 0.86).

Psychological wellbeing (PWB, waves 1 – 3)

A version of the PWB Posttraumatic Change Questionnaire (PWB-PTCQ; Joseph et al., Reference Joseph, Maltby, Wood, Stockton, Hunt and Regel2012) was utilized within the current study. The PWB-PTCQ is an 18-item self-report that queries adolescents regarding facets of PWB within the past 6 months. Response options range from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much so). Unlike the original study, the present questionnaire did not tie answers to a specific event, which allowed for measurement of PWB in a manner that aligns with Ryff’s (Reference Ryff1989) original conceptualization. Example items include “I have confidence in my opinions” and “I am able to cope with what life throws at me.” Past research found this measure reliably captured a unidimensional conceptualization of PWB in a diverse sample of adolescents over time (Thakur et al., Reference Thakur, Choi, Andrews, Temple and Cohen2024). Within the current study, the alpha reliability for this measure was acceptable at all timepoints of the study (αW1 = 0.97, αW2 = 0.98, αW3 = 0.98).

Demographics (Wave 1)

Three key demographics were retained within the dataset (i.e., age, gender, and race). Age was coded as a categorical variable (12 years old, 13 years old, 14 years old, 15 years old, 16 years old, 17 years old, 18 years old). Gender was a binary variable (Male, Female), while race was dichotomized into three non-mutually exclusive categories to allow for those identifying as multi-racial to be captured in all possible racial categories (Hispanic or not Hispanic, White or not White, Black or not Black).

Data analytic plan

Missing data was examined via the Little’s Missing Completely at Random (MCAR) test. If significant, indicating data were not MCAR, supplemental analyses tested whether the data were missing at random (MAR). Specifically, chi-square and one-way ANOVA tests examined whether those who had missed a follow-up differed from those who had complete data on all study variables. Significant differences would signify data is not missing at random. Across analyses, expectation maximization methods were used for imputation.

Random intercept cross-lagged panel models (RI-CLPMs) were used to explore the aims of the current study. RI-CLPMs allow for modeling of trait-like stability in a time invariant manner through random intercepts (i.e., between person differences), as well as individual fluctuations around one’s expected score (i.e., within person differences) (Hamaker et al., Reference Hamaker, Kuiper and Grasman2015). Other modeling of longitudinal data (e.g., traditional cross-lagged panel models) does not parse between within-person and between-person effects, running the risk of conflating these two sources of information within the interpretation of results (Hamaker et al., Reference Hamaker, Kuiper and Grasman2015). Within the RI-CLPMs tested, mean scale scores were entered into a multivariate model to examine the acceptability of a RI-CLPM model inclusive of PWB, depression, and violent-delinquent behavior. Once acceptable fit was established with mean scale scores, a series of models were run in which depression and violent-delinquent behavior scores were entered as polynomial terms, to capture possible quadratic relations between psychopathology and PWB (Aim 1). Three sets of quadratic models were run, including one in which both psychopathology terms were quadratic, and two in which one psychopathology variable was quadratic. This allowed for an examination of the possibility that quadratic effects provided an incremental better fit for depression or violent-delinquent behavior separately. Linear effects would suggest that, across cross-lagged and covariance paths, each standard deviation unit variation in PWB, for example, is associated with a standard deviation variation in depressive symptoms that is equal to that path coefficient for all values of PWB (e.g., Kock & Gaskins, Reference Kock and Gaskins2016). However, within a quadratic approach, this consistent relation in standard deviation variation is untenable, and instead requires a visual inspection of the relation, with a negative path coefficient suggesting an inverted-U relation between the constructs (e.g. see Ahrholdt et al., Reference Ahrholdt, Gudergan and Ringle2019). Finally, after determining whether linearity or non-linearity was preferred for the RI-CLPM, paths were examined to understand the magnitude and significance of the correlations at the between-person and within-person levels (Aim 2), and of the cross-lagged paths at the within-person level (Aim 3). Next, the moderating role of gender and race/ethnicity on relations was examined by constraining parameters across groups (Aim 4). This was accomplished by examining models in which paths were freely estimated (i.e., baseline model) to one in which paths were constrained to be equal across groups (i.e., stationary model). If no substantial difference in fit was observed by constraining parameters to be equal, then moderation was not supported (i.e., change in CFI ≤ 0.01).

Given the fewer degrees of freedom associated with random-intercept cross-lagged panels, investigators have cautioned against the use of RMSEA and x2/df for model fit, as these indicators unfairly penalize models with smaller degrees of freedom (Shi et al., Reference Shi, DiStefano, Maydeu-Olivares and Lee2022). It is instead recommended that a multi-indicator approach with comparative fit index (CFI) and standardized root mean squared residual (SRMR) be utilized to determine fit, which is in line with Hu and Bentler’s (Reference Hu and Bentler1999) recommendation to use SRMR in combination with at least one other indicator. Thus, acceptable model fit was achieved if CFI ≥ .95 and SRMR < 0.08 (Schreiber et al., Reference Schreiber, Nora, Stage, Barlow and King2006). To compare models that are unnested, the Akaike Information Criteria (AIC) and Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC) were used, with a change in AIC and BIC ≥10 akin to model improvement (Burnham & Anderson, Reference Burnham and Anderson2004). As χ2 difference tests exhibit sample size and non-normality sensitivity (Kline, Reference Kline2016), a cutoff of ∆CFI ≤ .01 was used to assessinvariance and significant differences between nested models (Cheung & Rensvold, Reference Cheung and Rensvold2002). All ∆CFIs reported in the manuscript represent the absolute value of change.

Results

Preliminary analysis

Within the dataset, MCAR analyses were significant χ2 (43) = 76.422; p <0.01. In response, we explored if data was MAR by testing if those who completed all three waves differed from those with missing data across any baseline variables. Pearson chi-square tests indicated a significant association between gender and missingness at follow-up (χ(2) = 11.72, p = 0.003), such that males were more likely to miss a follow-up. No other differences were found across study variables, p > .05. Given that most of our study variables were not impacted by missing data, we used expectation maximization to impute missing data prior to conducting our analyses. Descriptive statistics before and after imputation are provided in Table 1 and bivariate correlations are displayed in Table 2.

Table 1. Means and standard deviations of sum score before and after imputation

Note. PWB = Psychological Wellbeing Posttraumatic Change Questionnaire (Joseph et al., Reference Joseph, Maltby, Wood, Stockton, Hunt and Regel2012), Patient Health Questionnaire (Kroenke et al., Reference Kroenke, Spitzer and Williams2001), Violence Delinquency Scale (Broidy et al., Reference Broidy, Nagin, Tremblay, Bates, Brame, Dodge, Fergusson, Horwood, Loeber, Laird, Lynam, Moffitt, Pettit and Vitaro2003). PHQ scored on a 0 (not at all) to 3 (nearly every day) scale. PWB scored on a 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much so) scale. VDS scored on a 1 (never/not in the past 6 months but in my lifetime) to 4 (many times) scale.

Table 2. Correlations between imputed sum score variables

Note. PHQ = Patient Health Questionnaire (Kroenke et al., Reference Kroenke, Spitzer and Williams2001), PWB = Psychological Wellbeing Posttraumatic Change Questionnaire (Joseph et al., Reference Joseph, Maltby, Wood, Stockton, Hunt and Regel2012), VDS = Violence Delinquency Scale (Broidy et al., Reference Broidy, Nagin, Tremblay, Bates, Brame, Dodge, Fergusson, Horwood, Loeber, Laird, Lynam, Moffitt, Pettit and Vitaro2003). W1=Wave 1, W2=Wave 2, W3=Wave 3. PHQ scored on a 0 (not at all) to 3 (nearly every day) scale. PWB scored on a 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much so) scale. VDS scored on a 1 (never/not in the past 6 months but in my lifetime) to 4 (many times) scale. **. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). *. Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).

As for our null models for each psychological construct, intraclass correlation coefficients examining between- and within-person variance for each outcome indicated that 62% of the variance in PWB (ICC = 0.38), 35% of the variance in depressive symptoms (ICC = 0.65), and 64% of the variance in violent-delinquent behaviors (ICC = 0.36) could be attributed to within-person differences. These findings supported the partitioning of between- and within-person variance within the random-intercept cross-lagged approach.

Model building

Following the steps outlined by Mulder and Hamaker (Reference Mulder and Hamaker2021), fit of an unconstrained RI-CLPM was examined. Across linear and quadratic models, fit was acceptable (CFIlinear = 0.96, SRMRlinear = 0.025; CFIquadratic = 0.97, SRMRquadratic = 0.021). In terms of model assumptions, across the linear and quadratic models, tenability of the constraints on means and covariances was disproven (Change in CFIlinearmeans = 0.26, Change in CFIquadraticmeans = 0.30, Change in CFIlinearcovariances = 0.038, Change in CFIquadraticcovariances = 0.046), such that the unconstrained baseline model fit significantly better. In contrast, equality of paths was upheld across linear and quadratic models (Change in CFIlinearpaths = 0.016, Change in CFIquadraticpaths = 0.027). More specifically, though fit was significantly different between the baseline and constrained models, the constrained model provided significantly better fit for the linear and quadratic frameworks. This pattern of equality constraints maintained within models in which a single psychopathology measure was quadratic. As such, the baseline model across linear and quadratic models consisted of constrained autoregressive and cross-lagged paths across time.

Model selection (aim 1)

To understand whether it was best to proceed with linear or quadratic conceptualizations of psychopathology, AIC and BIC values were compared across models. This comparison indicated that the linear formulation was preferred (AIC: 7281.16, BIC: 7462.40) to all potential quadratic formulations (AICPHQandVDSquadratic = 16,454.12, BICPHQandVDSquadratic = 16,635.37; AICPHQquadratic = 12,215.21, BICPHQquadratic = 12,396.46; AICVDSquadratic = 11,522.53, BICVDSquadratic = 11,703.78). Thus, it was concluded that the relation between PWB and psychopathology was best defined as linear and this model was retained for further analyses. The CFI for the linear constrained model was 0.98, and the SRMR was 0.041.

RI-CLPM significant paths (aims 2 and 3)

Within the linear model, several significant findings emerged. To facilitate interpretation, standardized coefficients are presented here and in Table 3. First, with regard to correlations between our random intercepts, the correlation between depression and PWB (b PHQ = −0.25, SE = 0.11, p = 0.021), as well as violent-delinquent behavior and PWB (b VDS = −0.39, SE = 0.15, p = 0.011) was negative. Thus, both depression and violent-delinquent behavior exhibited a small, negative, cross-sectional, between-person relation with PWB. With regard to within-person cross-sectional correlations, only two significant associations emerged. Depressive symptoms negatively related to PWB at baseline (b PWB = −0.34, SE = 0.061, p < 0.01) and violent-delinquent behavior was positively related to PWB at Wave 3 (b = 0.21, SE = 0.080, p < 0.01), suggesting those who exhibited heightened violent-delinquent behavior also concurrently experienced elevated PWB. The effect size of depressive symptoms and PWB at Wave 2 was similar to that at baseline (albeit, not statistically significant), while the cross-sectional relation between PWB and violent-delinquent behavior was inconsistent across waves (see Table 3).

Table 3. Summary of findings from linear random intercept cross-lagged panel model with fixed autoregressive and cross-lagged paths

Note. PHQ = Patient Health Questionnaire (Kroenke et al., Reference Kroenke, Spitzer and Williams2001), PWB = Psychological Wellbeing Posttraumatic Change Questionnaire (Joseph et al., Reference Joseph, Maltby, Wood, Stockton, Hunt and Regel2012), VDS = Violence Delinquency Scale (Broidy et al., Reference Broidy, Nagin, Tremblay, Bates, Brame, Dodge, Fergusson, Horwood, Loeber, Laird, Lynam, Moffitt, Pettit and Vitaro2003). 1=Wave 1, 2=Wave 2, 3=Wave 3. PHQ scored on a 0 (not at all) to 3 (nearly every day) scale. PWB scored on a 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much so) scale. VDS scored on a 1 (never/not in the past 6 months but in my lifetime) to 4 (many times) scale.

With regard to the prospective, cross-lagged paths (Aim 3), we did not identify any prospective relation between depressive symptoms and PWB, ps > .05. However, violent-delinquent behaviors at time t-1 significantly predicted PWB at time t at all waves (b PWBW2 = 0.21, SEPWBW2 = 0.076, p < 0.01; b PWBW3 = 0.14, SEPWBW2 = 0.051, p < 0.01), albeit in a positive direction. Similar to the cross-sectional relation at Wave 3, these findings suggested that upward fluctuations in violent-delinquent symptoms prospectively predicted upward fluctuations in PWB. We did not find any examples of PWB prospectively predicting patterns of psychopathology, p > .05.

Moderation (aim 4)

To test for moderating effects, we first examined baseline models in which autoregressive and cross-lagged paths were constrained to be equal across time and the correlations between random intercepts were constrained across groups (i.e., between violent-delinquent behaviors and depressive symptoms, between depressive symptoms and PWB, and between violent-delinquent behaviors and PWB) to a model in which correlations between random intercepts were freely estimated. Across gender and dummy-coded racial/ethnic variables, this comparison indicated the absence of moderating effects (Change in CFIgender: 0.004, Change in CFIWhite: 0.012, Change in CFIBlack: 0.01, Change in CFIHispanic: 0.001). Given the lack of moderation for the correlation between random intercepts, we subsequently tested the moderating effect of autoregressive and cross-lagged paths by comparing a model in which random intercepts were held the same across groups and autoregressive and cross-lagged paths were held the same across time to a model that additionally constrained autoregressive and cross-lagged paths to be the same across groups. This comparison also demonstrated the absence of moderating effects (Change in CFIgender: 0.011, Change in CFIWhite: 0.005, Change in CFIBlack: 0.006, Change in CFIHispanic: 0.002). Finally, we examined moderation for cross-sectional correlations between constructs. To test this, we compared a baseline model in which random intercepts were held the same across groups, cross-lagged/autoregressive paths were held the same across time and groups, and cross-sectional correlations were held the same across groups to a model in which cross-sectional correlations were freely estimated across groups. This comparison also demonstrated no moderation for gender and race/ethnicity (Change in CFIgender: 0.013, Change in CFIWhite: 0.001, Change in CFIBlack: 0.01, Change in CFIHispanic: 0.01). Overall, these findings suggest that the pathways outlined in Table 3 generalized to the entire sample.

Discussion

The current study examined the relation between adolescent PWB and two forms of psychopathology (i.e., depressive symptoms and violent-delinquent behaviors) within a random intercept cross-lagged panel approach. Between-person findings indicated that adolescents higher on psychopathology consistently exhibit lower PWB than their peers. Similarly, the within-person relations between depressive symptoms and PWB exhibited a small-medium, negative effect. These findings aligned with hypotheses, past research with adults (Keyes, Reference Keyes2005), and extant theory, which posits negative and positive mental health states act antagonistically (Fredrickson et al., Reference Fredrickson, Cohn, Coffey, Pek and Finkel2008). Alternatively, contrary to hypotheses, we found no predictive relation between depressive symptoms and PWB and, unexpectedly, found a positive, predictive relation between violent behavior and future PWB. Below, we contextualize these findings, and others, in the extant adult literature on positive and negative mental health (e.g., Kraiss et al., Reference Kraiss, Kohlhoff and Ten Klooster2022), and elaborate on how these findings inform our understanding of adolescent mental health.

Empirical studies examining the relation between PWB and distress assume a linear relation between the two constructs (Lamers et al., Reference Lamers, Westerhof, Glas and Bohlmeijer2015). Contrary to hypotheses, results from the current study support this assumption suggesting that increases or decreases in PWB are associated with a consistent change in psychopathology. From an applied perspective, this may suggest that current clinical screening and assessment programs focused on distress may be indexing holistic well-being as well. However, it may be premature to conclude that screening and assessment protocols solely rely on the use of distress indicators to capture holistic mental health. The effect size of the relations between distress and PWB suggests that though there is some overlap in construct content, they represent distinct entities (small to medium versus large effect size). In fact, previous research demonstrates meaningful statistical inferences when utilizing both indices for differentiation of mental health profiles within adolescence (Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Choi, Thakur and Temple2021). To best capture the integration of both indices within applied settings, it may be additionally necessary to establish clinically meaningful thresholds across both constructs, which would allow for interpretation of linear changes in measures and guide clinical decision-making that is patient-centered (e.g., minimally important differences; Jaeschke et al., Reference Jaeschke, Singer and Guyatt1989).

To date, few studies have disentangled between and within-person effects when examining PWB and internalizing symptoms. Kraiss and colleagues (Reference Kraiss, Kohlhoff and Ten Klooster2022) found that internalizing distress and PWB were negatively, cross-sectionally associated in a sample of emerging adults across between and within-person associations. Joshanloo and Blasco-Belled (Reference Joshanloo and Blasco-Belled2023) found significant, negative between-person correlations and cross-lagged effects between depressive symptoms and PWB in a sample of older adults. The present study’s findings are largely congruent with these studies, with both between and within-levels of depression and PWB exhibiting a negative, small-moderate association. That internalizing distress and PWB showed relatively similar relations in adults and adolescents suggests developmental continuity between positive and negative indices of mental health. Indeed, past empirical work demonstrates continuity in internalizing distress from adolescence to adulthood (Essau et al., Reference Essau, Lewinsohn, Olaya and Seeley2014; Gutman & Sameroff, Reference Gutman and Sameroff2004), with aspects of PWB (i.e., less perceived social support; Stice et al., Reference Stice, Rohde, Gau and Ochner2011; Strine et al., Reference Strine, Kroenke, Dhingra, Balluz, Gonzalez, Berry and Mokdad2009) linked to depressive symptoms during these periods as well. Continuity in these presentations has been hypothesized to be maintained via several mechanisms, including cognitive factors that span adolescence through adulthood (e.g., depression; Hankin & Abela, Reference Hankin and Abela2005). As such, it is possible that cognitive mechanisms hypothesized to contribute to the link between psychopathology and PWB are maintaining this consistent relation. As an example, positive appraisal is linked to fewer depressive symptoms (Garnefski et al., Reference Garnefski, Legerstee, Kraaij, van Den Kommer and Teerds2002), as well as PWB (Puente-Martínez et al., Reference Puente-Martínez, Páez, Ubillos-Landa and Da Costa-Dutra2018), which may make it a promising integrated factor for positive and negative mental health outcomes across adults and adolescents. Collectively, this would suggest that protocols, such as Well-Being Therapy (Fava, Reference Fava1999), that leverage the negative relation between PWB and distress, and which target shared cognitive pathways, are well positioned to facilitate holistic mental health in a developmentally appropriate manner.

It is notable, however, that within-person findings between depressive symptoms and PWB were not consistently significant in the current study. Comparisons between the current study and prior research examining within- and between-person relations exhibit important differences. For example, Kraiss and colleagues (Reference Kraiss, Kohlhoff and Ten Klooster2022) conceptualized within-person relations using daily ecological momentary assessments over the course of two weeks. The measures within the current study were administered 6 months apart, which may suggest that PWB and depression may impact each other during shorter intervals. As such, detection of significant signals may occur with more proximal follow-up points. However, it is also possible that with the accumulation of more follow-up points (momentary assessments for 14 days; Kraiss et al., Reference Kraiss, Kohlhoff and Ten Klooster2022; 8 intervals spanning two years; Joshanloo & Blasco-Belled, Reference Joshanloo and Blasco-Belled2023), in contrast to the three waves of data in the current study, consistent significant findings would be better captured. Significant findings with shorter, more frequent time intervals would suggest that though depressive and PWB tend to negatively relate to each other at the within-person level, current long-term, retrospective based methods fail to capture the real-world dynamics of these constructs.

The proposed study also extended prior work on PWB and psychopathology by finding evidence of significant between and within-person effects for externalizing behaviors. At the between-person level, we found that violent behavior was negatively associated with PWB. This was consistent with our hypothesis and consistent with research that demonstrates that externalizing tendencies may lay the foundation for decreased PWB (poor interpersonal relationships; Sharp et al., Reference Sharp, Ha and Fonagy2011). Alternatively, our within-person findings suggested prospective increases in PWB for those exhibiting externalizing behaviors. Although not hypothesized, prior research conducted among adolescents suggests a protective, even positive, role of externalizing behaviors during this developmental period (Panayiotou & Humphrey, Reference Panayiotou and Humphrey2018). These findings are in line with theories that suggest delinquency arises in response to situations in which eudaimonic wellbeing, specifically positive relations with others, autonomy and environmental mastery (Self-Determination Theory; Deci & Ryan, Reference Deci, Ryan, Van Lange, Kruglanski and Higgins2012), appears unattainable through prosocial methods (Ward & Stewart, Reference Ward and Stewart2003). As an example of this phenomenon, focus group work demonstrated that adolescents aged 13–17 indicated that delinquency is exhibited when authority figures are perceived to be ineffective, thus motivating adolescents to rely on externalizing behaviors to resolve conflict (Shetgiri et al., Reference Shetgiri, Lee, Tillitski, Wilson and Flores2015). It is also possible that there is a context specific effect observed within the current study, with data collection for follow-up periods overlapping with the COVID-19 pandemic. The violent-delinquent measure used within the current study captures physical aggression but also maps onto behaviors that imply an interpersonal context (e.g., participated in fist fighting, participated in throwing things at other people). Given that the pandemic led to unprecedented social isolation for adolescents (e.g., Cingel et al., Reference Cingel, Lauricella, Taylor, Stevens, Coyne and Wartella2022), it is possible that those reporting elevated aggressive and violent behaviors were defying social distancing policies for immediate social interaction gratification. These increases in opportunities to engage with others, although in an aggressive manner, may have led to increases in PWB. In other words, within the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, the violent, delinquent subscale may have acted as an indicator of interpersonal engagement rather than pure externalizing behaviors. In summary, our results suggest that those with elevated externalizing behaviors may currently experience lower levels of PWB relative to their peers, however, may experience, relative to themselves, higher future PWB.

The phenomenon of contrasting relations (i.e., positive vs. negative) across different levels of analysis (i.e., within vs. between) is referred to as the Simpson’s Paradox (Blyth, Reference Blyth1972) and exists for other psychological phenomena. For instance, at the between-person level, adolescents who endorse higher perceptions of privacy invasion by parents also report higher secrecy behaviors. However, at the within-person level, increased adolescent secrecy behaviors prospectively predict decreased parental privacy invasion behaviors (Dietvorst et al., Reference Dietvorst, Hiemstra, Hillegers and Keijsers2018). For our study, it may be that displaying externalizing behaviors leads to more environmental stressors (e.g., punishment) concurrently, leading to lower levels of PWB for these youth relative to their peers. However, prospectively, once the punitive consequences fade, acting aggressively may be positively reinforced during adolescence via admiration from peers or reinforcing through engagement in environmental mastery (e.g., maintaining respect among peers, perception of authority figures as ineffective; Shetgiri et al., Reference Shetgiri, Lee, Tillitski, Wilson and Flores2015). Critiques of the Simpson’s Paradox suppose that its presence signals a potential moderating or quadratic relation (Kock & Gaskins, Reference Kock and Gaskins2016). However, as the current study explicitly explored potential moderating and quadratic effects for the relation between PWB and distress, it is likely that the current findings do not reflect the methodological error and is a finding that warrants further attention.

This Simpson’s Paradox has important implications for intervening with externalizing behaviors. Predominant interventions for externalizing behaviors (i.e., Defiant Teens; Barkley & Robin, Reference Barkley and Robin2014) identify reduction in parent-child conflict as a primary therapeutic target and subscribe to social learning models (i.e., coercion theory; Patterson, Reference Patterson, Dishion and Snyder2016), which assert that externalizing behaviors are learned and reinforced through familial interactions. As such, techniques employed in intervention focus on the shaping of parental behavior, including consistent parenting practices, delivery of positive attention, and reinforcement of prosocial behavior (Barkley & Robin, Reference Barkley and Robin2014). These approaches map onto the between-person findings within the current study, which suggest that disrupting tendencies to engage in delinquent behaviors may be associated with higher wellbeing tendencies. However, it is possible that predominant intervention approaches miss other motivations for engagement in externalizing behavior, such as opportunities to connect with peers who provide meaningful sources of community (Barnert et al., Reference Barnert, Perry, Azzi, Shetgiri, Ryan, Dudovitz, Zima and Chung2015). Within this context, focusing on the parent-child relationship will miss the main motivation for the delinquent behavior. Instead, interventions should focus on providing other opportunities for youth to socially connect and on demonstrating prosocial ways to exercise one’s autonomy (Larson et al., Reference Larson, Pearce, Sullivan and Jarrett2007). Without consideration for the nomological network surrounding externalizing behaviors and positive wellbeing, clinicians run the risk of focusing too narrowly on reducing externalizing behaviors, without maximizing solutions that simultaneously manage delinquent behaviors while cultivating PWB at the individual level.

Given the importance of understanding demographic characteristics on mental health phenomena (e.g., Nolen-Hoeksema & Girgus, Reference Nolen-Hoeksema and Girgus1994), this study additionally examined considerations that the relations between PWB and psychopathology may differ by gender and racial identities. Ultimately, results indicated a lack of moderation for demographic characteristics (i.e., race, gender) for within-person and between-person relations. It is important to note, however, that identity was measured simplistically with the use of common demographic responses (i.e., Male or Female). It is possible that identity-related differences in the holistic mental health framework are elicited by focusing on experiences or processes (e.g., masculinity/femininity; Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, DeLay and Martin2017), rather than relying on identity categories. In addition, the time period and methodology (i.e., 1 year timespan) associated with data collection did not allow for investigation into changes in the relations between PWB and psychological distress across adolescent development. More specifically, examination of within- and between-person relations for individuals as they progress through adolescence (i.e., early to middle to late stages) would provide insight into normative trait fluctuations at the population level and state fluctuations at the individual level. Insight into normative development would subsequently better inform establishment of clinically meaningful thresholds for these relations and allow for better distinguishing between context-dependent and development-dependent impacts.

Although the current study possesses several methodological strengths, including the use of a racially/ethnically-diverse United States-based population, several limitations can help to inform future directions. As is common with longitudinal data, the current study experienced attrition, but uniquely overlapped with an emergent public health crisis. As such replication of this study with other longitudinal samples will help to strengthen generalizability. The present study relied on self-report for measures. Although there are benefits to utilizing adolescents as their own informants (Tein et al., Reference Tein, Roosa and Michaels1994), the use of multiple informants represents an important next direction. It is possible that differences between an adolescent’s reported relations between psychological distress and PWB, and another informant’s report, would provide insight into how psychopathology and PWB impact each other across different settings (e.g., De Los Reyes et al., Reference De Los Reyes, Thomas, Goodman and Kundey2013). Finally, it is important to consider that current methods for measuring PWB and distress may benefit from continued examination. As an example, other measures of wellbeing employed within adolescence include age and developmental specific behaviors (Self-Perception Profile for Children (Harter, Reference Harter1985) versus Self-Perception for Adolescents (Harter, Reference Harter2012)), with the intention of more accurately capturing constructs across these transition points. It is possible that PWB measurement may similarly benefit from understanding the constellation of behaviors that best characterize PWB within early, middle and late adolescence. In addition, current measures are limited in their ability to capture holistic mental health effects in non-physical contexts. As adolescents increasingly incorporate non-physical interactions into their daily lives via an online presence (e.g., Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Faverio and Gottfried2023), the translation of the holistic mental health framework to this context will be important to explore. In fact, investigators theorize that online behaviors represent a method for coping with distress (Theory of Compensatory Internet Use; Kardefelt-Winther, Reference Kardefelt-Winther2014) and reflect engagement with PWB-related behaviors (Ross & Tolan, Reference Ross and Tolan2021), which would suggest there may be opportunities to beneficially leverage the holistic mental health framework outside traditional approaches. As such, characterization of PWB and distress in non-physical spaces, with considerations for important age and developmental behavioral manifestations, may not only better capture the holistic mental health framework, but also represent the next frontier for culturally and developmentally appropriate assessment and intervention efforts during the adolescent epoch.

Data availability statement

This data can be accessed at: https://doi.org/10.3886/ICPSR38324.v1

Acknowledgements

The authors have no acknowledgements to disclose.

Funding statement

This research was supported by the National Institute of Justice awarded to Joseph R. Cohen (2018-R2-CX-0022; 15PNIJ-21-GG-02803-MUMU). Writing and editing of the manuscript was supported by a training grant for Hena Thakur (5T32MH126368).

Competing interests

The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

References

Ahrholdt, D. C., Gudergan, S. P., & Ringle, C. M. (2019). Enhancing loyalty: When improving consumer satisfaction and delight matters. Journal of Business Research, 94, 1827. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2018.08.040 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, M., Faverio, M., & Gottfried, J. (2023). Teens, social media and technology 2023. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/internet/2023/12/11/teens-social-media-and-technology-2023/ Google Scholar
Ansary, N. S., & Luthar, S. S. (2009). Distress and academic achievement among adolescents of affluence: A study of externalizing and internalizing problem behaviors and school performance. Development and Psychopathology, 21(1), 319341. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579409000182 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Augusto-Landa, J. M., Pulido-Martos, M., & Lopez-Zafra, E. (2011). Does perceived emotional intelligence and optimism/pessimism predict psychological well-being? Journal of Happiness Studies, 12(3), 463474. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-010-9209-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barkley, R. A., & Robin, A. L. (2014). Defiant teens: A clinician’s manual for assessment and family intervention (2nd ed.). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Barnert, E. S., Perry, R., Azzi, V. F., Shetgiri, R., Ryan, G., Dudovitz, R., Zima, B., & Chung, P. J. (2015). Incarcerated youths’ perspectives on protective factors and risk factors for juvenile offending: A qualitative analysis. American Journal of Public Health, 105(7), 13651371. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2014.302228 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blyth, C. R. (1972). On Simpson’s paradox and the sure-thing principle. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 67(338), 364366. https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1972.10482387 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broidy, L. M., Nagin, D. S., Tremblay, R. E., Bates, J. E., Brame, B., Dodge, K. A., Fergusson, D., Horwood, J. L., Loeber, R., Laird, R., Lynam, D. R., Moffitt, T. E., Pettit, G. S., & Vitaro, F. (2003). Developmental trajectories of childhood disruptive behaviors and adolescent delinquency: A six-site, cross-national study. Developmental Psychology, 39(2), 222245. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.2.222 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burnham, K. P., & Anderson, D. R. (2004). Multimodel inference: Understanding aic and bic in model selection. Sociological Methods & Research, 33(2), 261304. https://doi.org/10.1177/0049124104268644 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burwell, R. A., & Shirk, S. R. (2007). Subtypes of rumination in adolescence: Associations between brooding, reflection, depressive symptoms, and coping. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 36(1), 5665. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374410709336568 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 9(2), 233255. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christopher, J. C. (1999). Situating psychological well-being: Exploring the cultural roots of its theory and research. Journal of Counseling & Development, 77(2), 141152. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6676.1999.tb02434.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cingel, D. P., Lauricella, A. R., Taylor, L. B., Stevens, H. R., Coyne, S. M., & Wartella, E. (2022). U.S. adolescents’ attitudes toward school, social connection, media use, and mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic: Differences as a function of gender identity and school context. PLOS ONE, 17(10), e0276737. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0276737 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. R., Choi, J. W., Thakur, H., & Temple, J. R. (2021). Psychological distress and well-being in trauma-exposed adolescents: A residualized, person-centered approach to resilience. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 34(3), 487500. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.22646 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, M. A., & Piquero, A. R. (2009). New evidence on the monetary value of saving a high risk youth. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 25(1), 2549. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-008-9057-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Los Reyes, A., Thomas, S. A., Goodman, K. L., & Kundey, S. M. A. (2013). Principles underlying the use of multiple informants’ reports. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9(1), 123149. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185617 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2012). Self-determination theory. In Van Lange, P. A. M., Kruglanski, A. W., & Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psycology (pp. 416436). Sage Publications Ltd. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446249215.n21 Google Scholar
Dietvorst, E., Hiemstra, M., Hillegers, M. H. J., & Keijsers, L. (2018). Adolescent perceptions of parental privacy invasion and adolescent secrecy: An illustration of simpson’s paradox. Child Development, 89(6), 20812090. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Essau, C. A., Lewinsohn, P. M., Olaya, B., & Seeley, J. R. (2014). Anxiety disorders in adolescents and psychosocial outcomes at age 30. Journal of Affective Disorders, 163, 125132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.12.033 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fava, G. A. (1999). Well-being therapy: Conceptual and technical issues. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 68(4), 171179. https://doi.org/10.1159/000012329 Portico.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ford, K. R., Hurd, N. M., Jagers, R. J., & Sellers, R. M. (2013). Caregiver experiences of discrimination and African American adolescents’ psychological health over time. Child Development, 84(2), 485499. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01864.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fredrickson, B. L., Cohn, M. A., Coffey, K. A., Pek, J., & Finkel, S. M. (2008). Open hearts build lives: Positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness meditation, build consequential personal resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(5), 10451062. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013262 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gao, J., & McLellan, R. (2018). Using Ryff’s scales of psychological well-being in adolescents in mainland China. BMC Psychology, 6(1), 17. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-018-0231-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garnefski, N., Legerstee, J., Kraaij, V., van Den Kommer, T., & Teerds, J. (2002). Cognitive coping strategies and symptoms of depression and anxiety: A comparison between adolescents and adults. Journal of Adolescence, 25(6), 603611. https://doi.org/10.1006/jado.2002.0507 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gold, J., Sullivan, M. W., & Lewis, M. (2011). The relation between abuse and violent delinquency: The conversion of shame to blame in juvenile offenders. Child Abuse & Neglect, 35(7), 459467. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2011.02.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gutman, L. M., & Sameroff, A. J. (2004). Continuities in depression from adolescence to young adulthood: Contrasting ecological influences. Development and Psychopathology, 16(4), 967984. https://doi.org/10.1017/s095457940404009x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamaker, E. L., Kuiper, R. M., & Grasman, R. P. P. P. (2015). A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychological Methods, 20(1), 102116. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038889 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hankin, B., & Abela, J. R. Z. (2005). Depression from childhood through adolescence and adulthood: A developmental vulnerability and stress perspective. In Development of psychopathology: A vulnerability-stress perspective (pp. 245288). SAGE Publications, Inc, https://doi.org/10.4135/9781452231655 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harding, J. F., Hughes, D. L., & Way, N. (2017). Racial/ethnic differences in mothers’ socialization goals for their adolescents. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 23(2), 281290. https://doi.org/10.1037/cdp0000116 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harter, S. (1985). Self-perception profile for children [dataset]. In PsycTESTS dataset. American Psychological Association (APA), https://doi.org/10.1037/t05338-000 Google Scholar
Harter, S. (2012). Self-perception profile for adolescents (SPPA), APA PsycTests, https://doi.org/10.1037/t05703-000 Google Scholar
Howell, A. J., Keyes, C. L. M., & Passmore, H. A. (2013). Flourishing among children and adolescents: Structure and correlates of positive mental health, and interventions for its enhancement: In Proctor, C., & Linley, P. A. (Eds.), Research, applications, and interventions for children and adolescents (pp. 5979). Springer Netherlands, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-6398-2_5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 155. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inguglia, C., Ingoglia, S., Liga, F., Lo Coco, A., & Lo Cricchio, M. G. (2015). Autonomy and relatedness in adolescence and emerging adulthood: Relationships with parental support and psychological distress. Journal of Adult Development, 22(1), 113. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10804-014-9196-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaeschke, R., Singer, J., & Guyatt, G. H. (1989). Measurement of health status: Ascertaining the minimal clinically important difference. Controlled Clinical Trials, 10(4), 407415. https://doi.org/10.1016/0197-2456(89)90005-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joseph, S., Maltby, J., Wood, A. M., Stockton, H., Hunt, N., & Regel, S. (2012). The psychological well-being—post-traumatic changes questionnaire (PWB-PTCQ): Reliability and validity. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 4(4), 420428. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024740 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joshanloo, M. (2013). A comparison of Western and Islamic conceptions of happiness. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14(6), 18571874. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-012-9406-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joshanloo, M., & Blasco-Belled, A. (2023). Reciprocal associations between depressive symptoms, life satisfaction, and eudaimonic well-being in older adults over a 16-year period. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 20(3), 2374. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20032374 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kardefelt-Winther, D. (2014). A conceptual and methodological critique of internet addiction research: Towards a model of compensatory internet use. Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 351354. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.059 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keyes, C. L. M. (2005). Mental illness and/or mental health? Investigating axioms of the complete state model of health. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(3), 539548. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.539 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keyes, C. L. M. (2007). Promoting and protecting mental health as flourishing: A complementary strategy for improving national mental health. American Psychologist, 62(2), 95108. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.62.2.95 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keyes, C. L. M., Dhingra, S. S., & Simoes, E. J. (2010). Change in level of positive mental health as a predictor of future risk of mental illness. American Journal of Public Health, 100(12), 23662371. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2010.192245 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kline, R. B. (2016). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (4th ed.). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kock, N., & Gaskins, L. (2016). Simpson’s paradox, moderation and the emergence of quadratic relationships in path models: An information systems illustration. International Journal of Applied Nonlinear Science, 2(3), 200. https://doi.org/10.1504/IJANS.2016.077025 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kraiss, J. T., Kohlhoff, M., & Ten Klooster, P. M. (2022). Disentangling between- and within-person associations of psychological distress and mental well-being: An experience sampling study examining the dual continua model of mental health among university students. Current Psychology, 42(20), 1678916800. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-02942-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. W. (2001). The PHQ-9: Validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 16(9), 606613. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamers, S. M. A., Westerhof, G. J., Glas, C. A. W., & Bohlmeijer, E. T. (2015). The bidirectional relation between positive mental health and psychopathology in a longitudinal representative panel study. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 10(6), 553560. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2015.1015156 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larson, R. W., Pearce, N., Sullivan, P. J., & Jarrett, R. L. (2007). Participation in youth programs as a catalyst for negotiation of family autonomy with connection. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36(1), 31. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-006-9133-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, K., Thompson, R. C., Watson, J., Montena, A. L., & Warren, S. L. (2023). Developmental trajectories of internalizing and externalizing symptoms in youth and associated gender differences: A directed network perspective. Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 51(11), 16271639. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-023-01106-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224253. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295x.98.2.224 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulder, J. D., & Hamaker, E. L. (2021). Three extensions of the random intercept cross-lagged panel model. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 28(4), 638648. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2020.1784738 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Girgus, J. S. (1994). The emergence of gender differences in depression during adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 115(3), 424443. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.424 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panayiotou, M., & Humphrey, N. (2018). Mental health difficulties and academic attainment: Evidence for gender-specific developmental cascades in middle childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 30(2), 523538. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941700102X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, J., & Naragon-Gainey, K. (2019). Is more emotional clarity always better? An examination of curvilinear and moderated associations between emotional clarity and internalising symptoms. Cognition and Emotion, 34(2), 273287. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2019.1621803 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patterson, G. R. (2016). Coercion theory: The study of change: In Dishion, T. J., & Snyder, J. (Eds.), The oxford handbook of coercive relationship dynamics (pp. 722). Oxford University Press, https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199324552.013.2 Google Scholar
Petito, A., Pop, T. L., Namazova-Baranova, L., Mestrovic, J., Nigri, L., Vural, M., Sacco, M., Giardino, I., Ferrara, P., & Pettoello-Mantovani, M. (2020). The burden of depression in adolescents and the importance of early recognition. The Journal of Pediatrics, 218, 265267. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2019.12.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prinstein, M. J., & Cillessen, A. H. (2003). Forms and functions of adolescent peer aggression associated with high levels of peer status. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49(3), 310342. https://doi.org/10.1353/mpq.2003.0015 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puente-Martínez, A., Páez, D., Ubillos-Landa, S., & Da Costa-Dutra, S. (2018). Examining the structure of negative affect regulation and its association with hedonic and psychological wellbeing. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1592. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01592 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richardson, L. P., McCauley, E., Grossman, D. C., McCarty, C. A., Richards, J., Russo, J. E., Rockhill, C., & Katon, W. (2010). Evaluation of the patient health questionnaire-9 item for detecting major depression among adolescents. Pediatrics, 126(6), 11171123. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2010-0852 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, A. A., DeLay, D., & Martin, C. L. (2017). Traditional masculinity during the middle school transition: Associations with depressive symptoms and academic engagement. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 46(4), 709724. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-016-0545-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, K. M., & Tolan, P. H. (2021). Positive youth development in the digital age: Expanding PYD to include digital settings: In Handbook of positive youth development (pp. 531548). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-70262-5_35 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruini, C., Ottolini, F., Tomba, E., Belaise, C., Albieri, E., Visani, D., Offidani, E., Caffo, E., & Fava, G. A. (2009). School intervention for promoting psychological well-being in adolescence. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 40(4), 522532. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2009.07.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Runyan, D., Dubowitz, H., English, D. J., Kotch, J. B., Litrownik, A., Thompson, R., & The LONGSCAN Investigator Group (2014). Longitudinal Studies of Child Abuse and Neglect (LONGSCAN) Assessments 0-18. [Dataset]. Available from National Data Archive on Child Abuse and Neglect. http://www.ndacan.cornell.eduGoogle Scholar
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 141166. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(6), 10691081. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryff, C. D., Boylan, J. M., & Kirsch, J. A. (2021). Eudaimonic and hedonic well-being: An integrative perspective with linkages to sociodemographic factors and health. In Lee, M. T., Kubzansky, L. D., & VanderWeele, T. J. (Eds.), Measuring well-being: Interdisciplinary perspectives from the social sciences and the humanities (pp. 92135). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780197512531.003.0005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of psychological well-being revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(4), 719727. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.69.4.719 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salmivalli, C., Kaukiainen, A., & Lagerspetz, K. (2000). Aggression and sociometric status among peers: Do gender and type of aggression matter? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 41(1), 1724. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9450.00166 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schreiber, J. B., Nora, A., Stage, F. K., Barlow, E. A., & King, J. (2006). Reporting structural equation modeling and confirmatory factor analysis results: A review. The Journal of Educational Research, 99(6), 323338. https://doi.org/10.3200/JOER.99.6.323-338 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seery, M. D., Holman, E. A., & Silver, R. C. (2010). Whatever does not kill us: Cumulative lifetime adversity, vulnerability, and resilience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(6), 10251041. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021344 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharp, C., Ha, C., & Fonagy, P. (2011). Get them before they get you: Trust, trustworthiness, and social cognition in boys with and without externalizing behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 23(2), 647658. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579410000003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shetgiri, R., Lee, S. C., Tillitski, J., Wilson, C., & Flores, G. (2015). Why adolescents fight: A qualitative study of youth perspectives on fighting and its prevention. Academic Pediatrics, 15(1), 103110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acap.2014.06.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shi, D., DiStefano, C., Maydeu-Olivares, A., & Lee, T. (2022). Evaluating sem model fit with small degrees of freedom. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 57(2-3), 179207. https://doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2020.1868965 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sijtsema, J. J., Lindenberg, S. M., Ojanen, T. J., & Salmivalli, C. (2020). Direct aggression and the balance between status and affection goals in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 49(7), 14811491. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-019-01166-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stice, E., Rohde, P., Gau, J., & Ochner, C. (2011). Relation of depression to perceived social support: Results from a randomized adolescent depression prevention trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49(5), 361366. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.02.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strine, T. W., Kroenke, K., Dhingra, S., Balluz, L. S., Gonzalez, O., Berry, J. T., & Mokdad, A. H. (2009). The associations between depression, health-related quality of life, social support, life satisfaction, and disability in community-dwelling US adults. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 197(1), 6164. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181924ad8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tein, J. Y., Roosa, M. W., & Michaels, M. (1994). Agreement between parent and child reports on parental behaviors. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 56(2), 341. https://doi.org/10.2307/353104 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thakur, H., Choi, J. W., Andrews, A. R. III, Temple, J. R., & Cohen, J. R. (2024). Measurement of adolescent psychological wellbeing: A test of factor structure and measurement invariance. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 34(3), 111. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12939 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ward, T., & Stewart, C. A. (2003). The treatment of sex offenders: Risk management and good lives. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 34(4), 353360. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7028.34.4.353 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warren, M. T., Wray-Lake, L., Rote, W. M., & Shubert, J. (2016). Thriving while engaging in risk? Examining trajectories of adaptive functioning, delinquency, and substance use in a nationally representative sample of U.S. adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 52(2), 296310. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039922 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Means and standard deviations of sum score before and after imputation

Figure 1

Table 2. Correlations between imputed sum score variables

Figure 2

Table 3. Summary of findings from linear random intercept cross-lagged panel model with fixed autoregressive and cross-lagged paths