Hostname: page-component-5b777bbd6c-pf7kn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-06-25T03:08:36.072Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Expectations and Support for Quebec Sovereignty: What Drives What?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 June 2025

Patrick Fournier*
Affiliation:
Université de Montréal, Montréal, Canada
André Blais
Affiliation:
Université de Montréal, Montréal, Canada
*
Corresponding author: Patrick Fournier; Email: patrick.fournier@umontreal.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This study uses panel data to examine the relationships among citizen attitudes related to Quebec independence. It has long been established that support for sovereignty is correlated to expectations about the economic and linguistic consequences of independence. Proponents of Quebec sovereignty tend to believe it will enhance the economy and preserve the French language, while opponents anticipate adverse effects on both fronts. We investigate whether economic and linguistic expectations drive preferences about independence, or whether preferences shape expectations. The analyses rely on Canadian Election Study panel surveys from 2004 to 2011 and employ cross-lagged models. Findings show that economic and linguistic expectations do affect support for sovereignty to some degree. However, the influence of sovereignty support on expectations is much more pronounced.

Résumé

Résumé

Cette étude utilise des données de panel pour examiner les relations entre les attitudes des citoyens à propos de l’indépendance du Québec. Il est établi depuis longtemps que l’appui à la souveraineté est corrélé aux attentes concernant les conséquences économiques et linguistiques de l’indépendance. Les partisans de la souveraineté du Québec ont tendance à croire qu’elle renforcera l’économie et préservera la langue française, tandis que les opposants anticipent des effets négatifs sur ces deux fronts. Nous cherchons à savoir si les attentes économiques et linguistiques déterminent les préférences à l’égard de l’indépendance, ou si les préférences façonnent les attentes. Les analyses s’appuient sur les sondages en panel de l’Étude Électorale Canadienne de 2004 à 2011 et emploient des modèles croisés décalés. Les résultats montrent que les attentes économiques et linguistiques affectent dans une certaine mesure l’appui à la souveraineté. Cependant, l’influence de l’appui à la souveraineté sur les attentes est beaucoup plus prononcée.

Type
Research Note/Note de recherche
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Canadian Political Science Association (l’Association canadienne de science politique) and/et la Société québécoise de science politique

Introduction

Public opinion surveys are extremely useful for capturing the distribution of a variable among a given population. They make it possible to determine how widespread an attitude or a behaviour is among a group. Surveys are also frequently used to examine relationships between variables. They help to identify the determinants of an attitude or a behaviour. However, the presence of a statistical association between the responses to two survey questions does not allow us to know whether one variable is really the cause of the other. To increase our confidence in the existence of a causal relationship between two variables, various investigative approaches are possible. On the one hand, we can turn to an experimental design—for instance, a survey experiment—where one variable is manipulated to discern whether the other variable is affected. On the other hand, panel surveys offer the opportunity to examine the passage of time in order to ascertain whether changes in one variable really lead to subsequent movements in the second variable.

This research note uses panel surveys to shed light on a question that remains unanswered about the factors that influence support for Quebec sovereignty. The desire to see the Canadian province become an independent country has long been correlated to citizens’ expectations about the consequences of independence on economic conditions and on the linguistic situation. Those in favor of Quebec sovereignty tend to think that it will improve the health of the economy and the French language, while those opposed to sovereignty generally anticipate negative consequences for the economy and the French language. The question that we tackle here is whether a person’s preference regarding Quebec sovereignty stems from their economic and linguistic expectations, or whether those expectations merely represent a projection of their opinion about the desirability of Quebec independence. We rely on the panel surveys of the Canadian Election Study conducted in 2004, 2006, 2008 and 2011. Methodologically, we employ cross-lagged models. We find that economic and linguistic expectations have some impact on support for sovereignty, however, the reverse relationships are much more important. For many citizens, it is the preference concerning Quebec sovereignty that shapes their expectations about the consequences of independence.

Determinants of Support for Quebec Sovereignty

The debate about Quebec’s political future has been ongoing for more than half a century. Some people want the Canadian province to separate and become an independent, sovereign country. Others prefer it to remain within Canada. Even though the proportion of people in each camp rises and falls over time, this issue structures political competition and affects the electoral behaviour of citizens decade after decade (Cloutier et al., Reference Cloutier, Guay and Latouche1992; Bélanger and Nadeau, Reference Bélanger and Nadeau2009; Yale and Durand, Reference Yale and Durand2011; Bélanger et al., Reference Bélanger, Daoust, Mahéo and Nadeau2022).

What factors explain support for and opposition to Quebec sovereignty? Two components are considered to be among the most important determinants in previous research: an identity and affective dimension, and a cost-benefit calculation dimension (Blais and Nadeau, Reference Blais and Nadeau1992; Martin, Reference Martin1994; Blais et al., Reference Blais, Martin and Nadeau1995; Nadeau and Fleury, Reference Nadeau and Fleury1995; Nadeau et al., Reference Nadeau, Martin and Blais1999; Mendelsohn, Reference Mendelsohn2003; Bélanger and Perrella, Reference Bélanger and Perrella2008). The first dimension refers to the relative attachment that individuals feel towards Quebec and Canada. People more attached to Quebec tend to be sovereignist, while those more attached to Canada tend to be federalist.Footnote 1 The second concerns people’s expectations about the impact of sovereignty on the economy and the French language, compared with the status quo. This is a comparative and forward-looking perspective. People who believe that sovereignty would have positive economic and linguistic consequences are more inclined to be in favour of it, while those who expect negative consequences are more likely to be against it.

How should we interpret these correlations between support for sovereignty and economic and linguistic expectations? On the one hand, researchers from the Montreal school consider that “the evaluation of anticipated costs and benefits influences the choices of a sufficient number of individuals to have a decisive effect on the final outcome” (Blais and Nadeau, Reference Blais and Nadeau1992; Martin, Reference Martin1994; Blais et al., Reference Blais, Martin and Nadeau1995: 638; Nadeau and Fleury, Reference Nadeau and Fleury1995; Nadeau et al., Reference Nadeau, Martin and Blais1999). The notions of utility maximisation (Blais et al., Reference Blais, Martin and Nadeau1995) and rationality (Martin, Reference Martin1994) are invoked. It is thus assumed that expectations are the independent variables and that support for sovereignty is the dependent variable. On the other hand, some studies question this conception and propose that causality flows in the opposite direction (Howe, Reference Howe1998; Mendelsohn, Reference Mendelsohn2003).Footnote 2 Rather than a rational choice, these correlations could represent rationalization.Footnote 3 More specifically, “rather than sunny assessments of the economic effects of sovereignty generating sovereignty support, it is enthusiasm for sovereignty that causes people to be optimistic about its economic consequences” (Howe, Reference Howe1998: 32). Here, it is support for sovereignty that influences expectations about the consequences of sovereignty.

Determining which of these two views is more empirically sound has not proved possible to date. For example, Howe admits that in the absence of more appropriate data, he offers only speculative and suggestive evidence to support his argument (1998: 32–33, 51–53). With cross-sectional surveys, as Mendelsohn points out, “Regression analysis in the end cannot perfectly sort out the dependent from the independent variables” (Reference Mendelsohn2003: 518).

Cross-Lagged Models

To examine the relationships between support for sovereignty and economic and linguistic expectations, we propose to use panel data and cross-lagged models (Campbell and Stanley, Reference Campbell and Stanley1963; Kenny, Reference Kenny1975; Finkel, Reference Finkel1995). This type of analysis consists of two stages. First, the correlation between expectations at time T-1 and support at time T is examined, while controlling for support at time T-1. We therefore test whether previous expectations predict change in support for independence. Then, we turn things around. We examine the correlation between support at time T-1 and expectations at time T, while controlling for expectations at time T-1. The aim is to see whether past support predicts changes in expectations. Finally, the results of the two stages are compared to decide whether the influence follows one direction more than the other.

This approach has notably been used by Lenz (Reference Lenz2009; Reference Lenz2013) to study the impact of issue positions on vote choice and, conversely, the impact of vote choice on issue positions. Lenz concludes that vote choice affects issue positions more than the opposite. While it is generally believed that citizens modify their electoral preference to support the political party that defends their positions, these results indicate instead that citizens modify their positions so that they are in line with their electoral preference. In other words, voters tend to adopt the positions of their favourite party. However, this dynamic is not dominant in all contexts. Analysing the 2017 French presidential election with cross-lagged models, Guntermann and Lachat (Reference Guntermann and Lachat2021) show that citizens’ issue positions explain their electoral choices when a new political party appears. Those who held centrist positions before the emergence of Emmanuel Macron were more likely to vote for this candidate on election day. To a lesser extent, there was also rationalization; some Macron supporters changed their positions and adopted those of the En Marche! movement. However, during this election, the influence was mainly from issue positions to vote choice rather than the other way around.

Some studies have used the same method to assess whether partisan identification leads to changes in issue positions, and whether issue positions lead to changes in partisan identification (Layman and Carsey, Reference Layman and Carsey2002; Carsey and Layman, Reference Carsey and Layman2006; Highton and Kam, Reference Highton and Kam2011; Engelhardt, Reference Engelhardt2021). Other research has done a similar exercise to assess the links between partisan identification and political values (Goren, Reference Goren2005; Evans and Neundorf, Reference Evans and Neundorf2020). A case can be made that opinions about Quebec sovereignty are very strong and can be conceptualized as the equivalent of partisanship in the United States. To our knowledge, no study has applied the technique of cross-lagged models to the case of support for Quebec sovereignty.

Data

The Canadian Election Study is a series of public opinion surveys conducted at each federal election since 1965. A large sample representative of the voting-age population is contacted each time (3,000–4,000 respondents). Approximately a quarter of the respondents are from the province of Quebec. The analyses here are limited to residents of Quebec. For three elections between 2006 and 2011, part of the sample was made up of respondents who had taken part in the study for the previous election. We can therefore use panels for the following pairs of elections: 2004/2006, 2006/2008 and 2008/2011.

In each of these surveys, support for Quebec sovereignty and expectations are consistently measured the same way. The support question asks: “Are you very favourable, somewhat favourable, somewhat opposed, or very opposed to Quebec sovereignty, that is Quebec is no longer part of Canada?” The question on economic expectations is as follows: “If Quebec separates from Canada, do you think your standard of living will get better, get worse, or stay about the same as now?” For linguistic consequences, the question is: “If Quebec separates from Canada, do you think the situation of the French language in Quebec will get better, get worse, or stay about the same?” All variables are recoded to range from 0 to 1 (where a high score indicates a positive opinion on sovereignty and the economic and linguistic consequences). Table 1 displays the means of these variables for each year. The central tendency of each attitude is relatively stable over time. The majority of respondents is opposed to Quebec sovereignty and anticipates that sovereignty would have negative economic consequences and positive linguistic consequences.

Table 1. Variable Averages

All variables range from 0 to 1.

Results

To illustrate the findings of previous research, Table 2 reports the results of regression analyses based on cross-sectional data (that is, measured in the same survey). In each of the four elections, expectations about the economic and linguistic consequences of sovereignty are strongly correlated with support for sovereignty. Individuals who are more optimistic about the impact of independence are more likely to be in favour of secession. The two expectation variables alone explain 40 per cent or more of the variance in support for sovereignty. It is important to note that the coefficients of expectations remain strong and significant when control variables, such as relative attachment to Quebec, are added.Footnote 4

Table 2. Determinants of Support for Quebec Sovereignty: Cross-Sectional Analyses

OLS non-standardised coefficients and standard errors. All variables range from 0 to 1.

***p < .001; **p < .01; *p < .05.

We now turn to the analysis of the panel data. The first stage of the cross-lagged models tests the existence of a genuine relationship between expectations and support for sovereignty. We examine the impact of past expectations on current support, while taking past support into account.Footnote 5 Table 3a presents the results for economic expectations, while Table 3b presents those for linguistic expectations.Footnote 6 The magnitude of the coefficients of the lagged dependent variable shows that support for sovereignty is very stable over time. In each of the three models in Table 3a, economic expectations have a significant effect on opinions towards sovereignty. Economic expectations are responsible for subsequent changes in support for independence. However, these effects are much smaller than those found in the cross-sectional data (Table 2). In the case of linguistic expectations (Table 3b), only one of the three coefficients is significant (Model 6). Linguistic expectations are therefore less relevant than economic expectations in explaining support for sovereignty.

Table 3a. Cross-Lagged Analyses: Economic Expectations and Support for Sovereignty

OLS non-standardised coefficients and standard errors. All variables range from 0 to 1.

***p < .001; **p < .01; *p < .05.

Table 3b. Cross-Lagged Analyses: Linguistic Expectations and Support for Sovereignty

OLS non-standardised coefficients and standard errors. All variables range from 0 to 1.

***p < .001; **p < .01; *p < .05.

The second stage of the cross-lagged models tests for the presence of rationalization. It asks whether prior support for sovereignty affects current expectations when controlling for prior expectations. The coefficients of the lagged dependent variables indicate that economic expectations (Table 3c) and linguistic expectations (Table 3d) are less stable over time than support for sovereignty (Tables 3a and 3b). Models 7, 8 and 9 reveal that views about independence systematically have a significant impact on Quebeckers’ economic expectations. The same dynamic is observed for linguistic expectations (Models 10, 11 and 12). Those who are more favourable towards sovereignty tend to develop more optimistic expectations about the consequences of separation on the state of the economy and the French language.

Table 3c. Cross-lagged analyses: Support for Sovereignty and Economic Expectations

OLS non-standardised coefficients and standard errors. All variables range from 0 to 1.

***p < .001; **p < .01; *p < .05.

Table 3d. Cross-Lagged Analyses: Support for Sovereignty and Linguistic Expectations

OLS non-standardised coefficients and standard errors. All variables range from 0 to 1.

***p < .001; **p < .01; *p < .05.

Finally, we need to compare the results of the two stages to determine whether influence flows more in one direction than the other. To facilitate the comparison, we have calculated the averages of the standardised coefficients for each trio of models (these coefficients hence differ from the non-standardised coefficients reported in Tables 3a3d). The average standardised impact of economic expectations on support for sovereignty is 0.12 (Models 1–3), while that of support for independence on economic expectations is 0.30 (Models 7–9). Thus, the influence of support for sovereignty on economic expectations is much greater than the influence of expectations on support.Footnote 7 Despite the fact that economic expectations do affect some respondents’ views about secession, there is a great deal of rationalization at work.

The story is the same for linguistic expectations. The average standardised effect of linguistic expectations on support for sovereignty is 0.09 (Models 4-6), while that of support on expectations is 0.26 (Models 10–12). The influence from expectations to support is much smaller than the influence in the opposite direction.Footnote 8

Conclusion

Two divergent views of the determinants of support for Quebec sovereignty coexist. One considers that people’s expectations regarding the economic and linguistic consequences of independence lead them to develop a favourable or unfavourable opinion about sovereignty. The other view considers that the attitude towards the desirability of sovereignty generates positive or negative expectations about the consequences of secession. Analyses based on cross-sectional survey data have so far been unable to ascertain what drives what.

In this research note, we rely on panel surveys and cross-lagged models to assess the accuracy of the two views. It turns out that both interpretations are partially valid. Some people use their predictions about the economic effects of sovereignty to decide whether or not they are in favour of independence. To a lesser extent (in one election out of three), this also applies to linguistic expectations. However, other individuals exhibit rationalization. Their economic and linguistic expectations simply stem from their constitutional preferences. This second dynamic is more important than the first. There is more rationalization than cost-benefit calculation in citizens’ decisions about Quebec sovereignty.

Our findings question the relevance of the rational choice perspective that puts much emphasis on cost-benefit evaluations as the primary determinant of support for or opposition to Quebec sovereignty. Yet, such evaluations do come into play, but their impact is limited. The question thus becomes: What does drive Quebeckers’ views about independence? We are not able to tackle that question in this research note. Our hunch is that attitudes about sovereignty, like many other attitudes, are shaped by feelings, in this case how strongly or weakly attached one feels about Quebec and Canada.

Footnotes

1 Sometimes attachment is captured by a question that asks respondents whether they identify primarily as Quebeckers or as Canadians (Blais et al., Reference Blais, Martin and Nadeau1995; Nadeau et al., Reference Nadeau, Martin and Blais1999). Sometimes the difference between feeling thermometer evaluations of Quebec and Canada is used (Blais and Nadeau, Reference Blais and Nadeau1992; Martin, Reference Martin1994; Nadeau and Fleury, Reference Nadeau and Fleury1995).

2 See also Clarke et al. (Reference Clarke, Kornberg and Stewart2004), who argue that it is crucial to consider party identification, feelings about party leaders and evaluations of government performance.

3 We use the term rationalization, but an equally appropriate term would be motivated reasoning (see, for instance, Lodge and Taber, Reference Lodge and Taber2013).

4 In 2006, 2008 and 2011, relative attachment is measured by the difference between the thermometer rating for Quebec and the thermometer rating for Canada. In 2004, we had to fall back on the Quebec thermometer rating because the item about Canada is missing.

5 The logic is similar to that in Granger causality tests for time-series data. The goal is to determine whether variable A at time T-1 affects subsequent changes in variable B between time T-1 and time T. It should be kept in mind that this is not a controlled experiment, and that we cannot rule out confounding variables.

6 There is a relatively strong correlation between the two expectations (mean correlation across surveys = 0.36), and this is the reason why we consider them separately. The results are similar if we include both expectations simultaneously. That is, there is evidence in both directions (expectations affect support for sovereignty and vice-versa), but the latter influence, related to rationalization, is greater.

7 The difference between coefficients is statistically significant at the .05 level for two of the three panels (the exception being 2008/2011).

8 The difference between coefficients is again significant for all panels except 2008/2011.

References

Bélanger, Éric, Daoust, Jean-François, Mahéo, Valérie-Anne and Nadeau, Richard. 2022. Le nouvel électeur québécois. Montréal: Presses de l’Université de Montréal.10.1515/9782760645929CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bélanger, Éric and Nadeau, Richard. 2009. Le comportement électoral des Québécois. Montréal: Presses de l’Université de Montréal.Google Scholar
Bélanger, Éric and Perrella, Andrea. 2008. “Facteurs d’appui à la souveraineté du Québec chez les jeunes: Une comparaison entre francophones, anglophones et allophones.” Politique et sociétés 27: 1340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blais, André, Martin, Pierre and Nadeau, Richard. 1995. “Attentes économiques et linguistiques et appui à la souveraineté du Québec: Une analyse prospective et comparative.” Canadian Journal of Political Science 28: 637–57.10.1017/S0008423900019338CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blais, André and Nadeau, Richard. 1992. “To be or not to be sovereignist: Quebeckers’ perennial dilemma.” Canadian Public Policy 18: 89103.10.2307/3551558CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, Donald T. and Stanley, Julian C.. 1963. Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for research. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Carsey, Thomas M. and Layman, Geoffrey C.. 2006. “Changing sides or changing minds? Party identification and policy preferences in the American electorate.” American Journal of Political Science 50: 464–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, Harold D., Kornberg, Allan and Stewart, Marianne C.. 2004. “Referendum voting as political choice: The case of Quebec.” British Journal of Political Science 34: 345–55.10.1017/S0007123404000092CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cloutier, Édouard, Guay, Jean H. and Latouche, Daniel. 1992. Le virage: L’évolution de l’opinion publique au Québec depuis 1960, ou comment le Québec est devenu souverainiste. Montréal: Éditions Québec/Amérique.Google Scholar
Engelhardt, Andrew M. 2021. “Racial attitudes through a partisan lens.” British Journal of Political Science 51: 1062–79.10.1017/S0007123419000437CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, Geoffrey and Neundorf, Anja. 2020. “Core political values and the long-term shaping of partisanship.” British Journal of Political Science 50: 1263–81.10.1017/S0007123418000339CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finkel, Steven E. 1995. Causal analysis with panel data, 105. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.10.4135/9781412983594CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goren, Paul. 2005. “Party identification and core political values.” American Journal of Political Science 49: 881–96.10.1111/j.1540-5907.2005.00161.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guntermann, Eric and Lachat, Romain. 2021. “Policy preferences influence vote choice when a new party emerges: Evidence from the 2017 French presidential election.” Political Studies: 00323217211046329.Google Scholar
Highton, Benjamin and Kam, Cindy D.. 2011. “The long-term dynamics of partisanship and issue orientations.” Journal of Politics 73: 202–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howe, Paul. 1998. “Rationality and sovereignty support in Quebec.” Canadian Journal of Political Science 31: 3159.10.1017/S0008423900008672CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenny, David A. 1975. “Cross-lagged panel correlation: A test for spuriousness.” Psychological Bulletin 82: 887903.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Layman, Geoffrey C. and Carsey, Thomas M.. 2002. “Party polarization and party structuring of policy attitudes: A comparison of three NES panel studies.” Political Behavior 24: 199236.10.1023/A:1021820523983CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lenz, Gabriel S. 2009. “Learning and opinion change, not priming: Reconsidering the priming hypothesis.” American Journal of Political Science 53: 821–37.10.1111/j.1540-5907.2009.00403.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lenz, Gabriel S. 2013. Follow the leader: How voters respond to politicians’ policies and performance. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lodge, Milton and Taber, Charles. 2013. The rationalizing voter. New York: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781139032490CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, Pierre. 1994. “Générations politiques, rationalité économique et appui à la souveraineté au Québec.” Canadian Journal of Political Science 27: 345–59.10.1017/S0008423900017388CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendelsohn, Matthew. 2003. “Rational choice and socio-psychological explanation for opinion on Quebec sovereignty.” Canadian Journal of Political Science 36: 511–37.10.1017/S0008423903778743CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nadeau, Richard and Fleury, Christopher J.. 1995. “Anticipated language gains and support for Quebec sovereignty.” Canadian Journal of Political Science 28: 3550.10.1017/S0008423900018369CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nadeau, Richard, Martin, Pierre and Blais, André. 1999. “Attitude towards risk-taking and individual choice in the Quebec referendum on sovereignty.” British Journal of Political Science 29: 523–39.10.1017/S0007123499000241CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yale, François and Durand, Claire. 2011. “What did Quebeckers want? Impact of question wording, constitutional proposal and context on support for sovereignty, 1976-2008.” American Review of Canadian Studies 41: 242–58.10.1080/02722011.2011.594517CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Variable Averages

Figure 1

Table 2. Determinants of Support for Quebec Sovereignty: Cross-Sectional Analyses

Figure 2

Table 3a. Cross-Lagged Analyses: Economic Expectations and Support for Sovereignty

Figure 3

Table 3b. Cross-Lagged Analyses: Linguistic Expectations and Support for Sovereignty

Figure 4

Table 3c. Cross-lagged analyses: Support for Sovereignty and Economic Expectations

Figure 5

Table 3d. Cross-Lagged Analyses: Support for Sovereignty and Linguistic Expectations