Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T22:34:06.877Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part I - Theoretical Perspectives on the Imagination

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 May 2020

Anna Abraham
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bibliography

Abraham, A. (2016). The Imaginative Mind. Human Brain Mapping, 37(11), 41974211. doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ackermann, R. R., Mackay, A., and Arnold, M. L. (2015). The Hybrid Origin of “Modern” Humans. Evolutionary Biology, 43, 111.Google Scholar
Anton, S. C., Potts, R., and Aiello, L. C. (2014). Evolution of Early Homo: An Integrated Biological Perspective. Science, 345(6192), 45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arbib, M. A. (2011). From Mirror Neurons to Complex Imitation in the Evolution of Language and Tool Use. Annual Review of Anthropology, 40, 257–273.Google Scholar
Barnard, A. (2012). Genesis of Symbolic Thought. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bloch, M. (2016). Imagination from the Outside and from the Inside. Current Anthropology, 57(Supplement 13), S80S87.Google Scholar
Brooks, A. S., Yellen, J. E., Potts, R., et al. (2018). Long-Distance Stone Transport and Pigment Use in the Earliest Middle Stone Age. Science, 360(6384), 9094.Google Scholar
Coward, F. and Grove, M. (2011). Beyond the Tools: Social Innovation and Hominin Evolution. PaleoAnthropology, 2011, 111129. doi:10.4207/PA.2011.ART46Google Scholar
Deacon, T. (1997). The Symbolic Species: The Co-Evolution of Language and the Brain. London, UK: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Deacon, T.(2016). On Human (Symbolic) Nature: How the Word Became Flesh. In Fuchs, T. and Tewes, C. (eds.), Embodiment in Evolution and Culture. Tübingen, Germany: Mohr Siebeck, 129149.Google Scholar
Downey, G., and Lende, D. H. (2012). Evolution and the Brain. In Lende, D. H. and Downey, G. (eds.), The Encultured Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 103138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fogarty, L., Creanza, N., and Feldman, M. W. (2015). Cultural Evolutionary Perspectives on Creativity and Human Innovation. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 30(12), 736754.Google Scholar
Foley, R. A. (2016). Mosaic Evolution and the Pattern of Transitions in the Hominin Lineage. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 371 , 20150244.Google Scholar
Foote, A. D., Nagurjan, V., Ávila-Arcos, M. C., et al. (2016). Genome-Culture Coevolution Promote Rapid Divergence of Killer Whale Ecotypes. Nature Communications, 7, 11693.Google Scholar
Fuentes, A. (2014). Human Evolution, Niche Complexity, and the Emergence of a Distinctively Human Imagination. Time and Mind, 7(3), 241257.Google Scholar
Fuentes, A.(2015). Integrative Anthropology and the Human Niche: Toward a Contemporary Approach to Human Evolution. American Anthropologist, 117(2), 302315.Google Scholar
Fuentes, A.(2016). The Extended Evolutionary Synthesis, Ethnography, and the Human Niche: Toward an Integrated Anthropology. Current Anthropology, 57(Supplement 13), 1326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuentes, A.(2017a). The Creative Spark: How Imagination Made Humans Exceptional. New York, NY: Dutton/Penguin.Google Scholar
Fuentes, A.(2017b). Human Niche, Human Behaviour, Human Nature. Interface Focus, 7, 20160136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuentes, A.(2018). How Humans and Apes Are Different, and Why It Matters. Journal of Anthropological Research, 74(2), 151167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuentes, A.(2019). Why We Believe. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Grove, M., and Coward, F. (2008). From Individual Neurons to Social Brains. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 18, 387400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harmand, S., Lewis, J. E., Feibel, C. S., et al. (2015). 3.3-Million-Year-Old Stone Tools from Lomekwi 3, West Turkana, Kenya. Nature, 521, 310315.Google Scholar
Hecht, E. E., Gutman, D. A., Khreisheh, N., et al. (2014). Acquisition of Paleolithic Tool-Making Abilities Involves Structural Remodeling to Inferior Fronto-Parietal Regions. Brain Structure and Function, 220, 23152331.Google Scholar
Hiscock, P. (2014). Learning in Lithic Landscapes: A Reconsideration of the Hominid “Toolmaking” Niche. Biological Theory, 9(1), 2741.Google Scholar
Hodder, I. (1998). Creative Thought: A Long-Term Process. In Mithen, S. (ed.), Creativity in Human Evolution and Prehistory. London, UK: Routledge, 6177.Google Scholar
Hutchinson, G. E. (1957). Concluding Remarks. Cold Spring Harbor Symposium on Quantitative Biology, 22, 415427.Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (2000). The Perception of the Environment: Essays on Livelihood, Dwelling and Skill. Abingdon, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Ingold, T.(2004). Beyond Biology and Culture: The Meaning of Evolution in a Relational World. Social Anthropology, 12(2), 209221.Google Scholar
Ingold, T., and Paalson, G. (eds.) (2013). Biosocial Becomings: Integrating Social and Biological Anthropology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kissel, M. and Fuentes, A. (2017a). Semiosis in the Pleistocene. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 27(3), 116.Google Scholar
Kissel, M. and Fuentes, A.(2017b). A Database of Archaeological Evidence for Representational Behavior. Evolutionary Anthropology, 26(4), 1490150.Google Scholar
Kissel, M. and Fuentes, A.(2018). “Behavioral Modernity” as a Process, not an Event, in the Human Niche. Time and Mind, 11(2), 163183.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. (2017). Darwin’s Unfinished Symphony: How Culture Made the Human Mind. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Martinez, I., Rosa, M., Arsuaga, J. L., et al. (2004). Auditory Capacities in Middle Pleistocene Humans from the Sierra de Atapuerca in Spain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101(27), 9976–9981.Google Scholar
Montagu, A. (1965). The Human Revolution. New York, NY: Bantam Books.Google Scholar
Peirce, C. (1998). The Essential Peirce. Volume 2. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Pope, M. I., and Roberts, M. B. (2005). Observations on the Relationship Between Individuals and Artefact Scatters at the Middle Palaeolithic Site of Boxgrove, West Sussex. In Gamble, C. and Porr, M. (eds.), The Hominid Individual in Context: Archaeological Investigations of Lower and Middle Palaeolithic Landscapes. London, UK: Routledge, 8197.Google Scholar
Ramsey, G. (2013). Culture in Humans and Other Animals. Biology and Philosophy, 28, 457479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reader, S. M., and Laland, K. N. (2003). Animal Innovation. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sanz, C. M., Call, J., and Boesch, C. (2014). Tool Use in Animals: Cognition and Ecology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Scerri, E., Thomas, M. G., Manica, A., et al. (2018). Did Our Species Evolve in Subdivided Populations across Africa, and Why Does It Matter? Trends in Ecology & Evolution. doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2018.05.005.Google Scholar
Sherwood, C. C., and Gomez-Robles, A. (2017). Brain Plasticity and Human Evolution. Annual Review of Anthropology, 2017(46), 399419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spikins, P. (2015). How Compassion Made us Human. Barnsley, UK: Pen and Sword Press.Google Scholar
Staes, N., Sherwood, C. C., Wright, K., et al. (2017). FOXP2 Variation in Great Ape Populations Offers Insight into the Evolution of Communication Skills. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 16866.Google Scholar
Steffen, W., Grinevald, J., Crutzen, P., and McNeil, J. (2011). The Anthropocene: Conceptual and Historical Perspectives. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society A, 369, 842867.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2012). The Evolved Apprentice: How Evolution Made Humans Unique. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2017). Artifacts, Symbols, Thoughts. Biological Theory, 12:236247.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K., and Hiscock, P. (2014). Symbols, Signals, and the Archaeological Record. Biological Theory, 9(1), 13.Google Scholar
Stevenson, L. (2003). Twelve Conceptions of Imagination. The British Journal of Aesthetics, 43(3), 238259.Google Scholar
Stout, D., and Chaminade, T. (2012). Stone Tools, Language and the Brain in Human Evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367(1585), 7587.Google Scholar
Stout, D., Hecht, E., Khreisheh, N., et al. (2015). Cognitive Demands of Lower Paleolithic Toolmaking. PLoS ONE, 10(4), e0121804.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2014). The Natural History of Human Thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Vale, G. L., Dean, L., and Whiten, A. (2016). Culture in Nonhuman Animals. In Callen, H. (ed.), Wiley International Encyclopedia of Anthropology. New York, NY: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wadley, L. (2013). Recognizing Complex Cognition through Innovative Technology in Stone Age and Palaeolithic Sites. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 23(2), 163183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wake, D. B., Hadley, E. A., and Ackerly, D. (2009). Biogeography, Changing Climates, and Niche Evolution. PNAS, 106(2), 1963119636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., and Erdal, D. (2012). The Human Socio-Cognitive Niche and its Evolutionary Origins. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367, 21192129.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Ayala, F. J., Feldman, M. W., and Laland, K. N. (2017). The Extension of Biology through Culture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(30), 77757781.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Hinde, R. A., Stringer, C. B., and Laland, K. N. (2012). Culture Evolves. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wood, B. (2010). Reconstructing Human Evolution: Achievements, Challenges, and Opportunities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(S2), 89028909.Google Scholar
Zeder, M. A. (2017). Domestication as a Model System for the Extended Evolutionary Synthesis. Interface Focus, 7, 20160133.Google Scholar

References

Abraham, A. (2016). The Imaginative Mind. Human Brain Mapping, 37(11), 41974211. doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, F., and Aizawa, K. (2010). The Value of Cognitivism in Thinking about Extended Cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), 579603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Appadurai, A. (1996). Modernity at Large: Cultural Dimensions of Globalization. Volume 1. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Bachelard, G. (1983). Water and Dreams: An Essay on the Imagination of Matter. Dallas, TX: Pegasus Foundation.Google Scholar
Bachelard, G.(2014). The Poetics of Space. London, UK: Penguin Classics.Google Scholar
Benedict, A. (1983). Imagined Communities: Reflections on the Origin and Spread of Nationalism. London, UK: Verso.Google Scholar
Blake, W. (1965). The Complete Poetry and Prose of William Blake. Edited by Erdman, David V.. Commentary by Harold Bloom. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Bloch, M. (2012). Anthropology and the Cognitive Challenge. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Castoriadis, C. (1997). The Imaginary Institution of Society. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Crapanzano, V. (2004). Imaginative Horizons: An Essay in Literary-Philosophical Anthropology. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Draaisma, D. (2000). Metaphors of Memory: A History of Ideas About the Mind. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Eco, U. (2007). Foucault’s Pendulum. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Elliott, D., and Culhane, D. (eds.) (2016). A Different Kind of Ethnography: Imaginative Practices and Creative Methodologies. Toronto, Canada: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1951). Kinship and Marriage Among the Nuer. Oxford, UK:Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. (1983). The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gallagher, S. (2017). Enactivist Interventions: Rethinking the Mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ganis, G., and Schendan, H. E. (2011). Visual Imagery. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2(3), 239252.Google Scholar
Gaonkar, D. P. (2002). Toward New Imaginaries: An Introduction. Public Culture, 14(1), 119.Google Scholar
Gosden, C., and Malafouris, L. (2015). Process Archaeology (P-Arch). World Archaeology, 47(5), 117.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (2010a). Cognitive Ecology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, 705715.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E.(2010b). Enaction, Imagination, and Insight. In Stewart, J, Gapenne, O, and Di Paolo, E. A. (eds.), Enaction: Toward a New Paradigm for Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 425450.Google Scholar
Hutto, D. D. (2017). Evolving Enactivism: Basic Minds Meet Content. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hutto, D., and Myin, E. (2013). Radicalizing Enactivism: Basic Minds without Content. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (2012). Toward an Ecology of Materials. Annual Review of Anthropology, 41(1), 427442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingold, T.(2013). Making: Anthropology, Archaeology, Art and Architecture. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Ingold, T.(2018). Back to the Future with the Theory of Affordances. HAU: Journal of Ethnographic Theory, 8(1–2), 3944.Google Scholar
Kohn, E. (2013). How Forests Think: Toward an Anthropology Beyond the Human. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., and Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural Foundations of Imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(9), 635.Google Scholar
Lacan, J. (2001). Ecrits: A Selection. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Malafouris, L. (2004). The Cognitive Basis of Material Engagement: Where Brain, Body and Culture Conflate. In DeMarrais, E, Gosden, C, and Renfrew, C (eds.), Rethinking Materiality: The Engagement of Mind with the Material World. Cambridge, UK: The McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, 5362.Google Scholar
Malafouris, L.(2008). At the Potter’s Wheel: An Argument for Material Agency. In Knappett, C and Malafouris, L (eds.), Material Agency: Towards a Non-Anthropocentric Perspective. New York, NY: Springer, 1936.Google Scholar
Malafouris, L.(2013). How Things Shape the Mind: A Theory of Material Engagement. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Malafouris, L. (2014). Creative Thinging: The Feeling of and for Clay. Pragmatics and Cognition, 22(1), 140158.Google Scholar
Malafouris, L. (2016). Hylonoetics: On the Priority of Material Engagement. In Grigoriadis, K (ed.), Mixed Matters: A Multi-Material Design Compendium. Berlin, Germany: Jovis Verlag, 140146.Google Scholar
Malafouris, L. (2018). Bringing Things to Mind: 4Es and Material Engagement. In Newen, A, de Bruin, L, and Shaun, G (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 755771.Google Scholar
Malafouris, L.(2019). Mind and Material Engagement. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 18(1), 117.Google Scholar
Malafouris, L., and Koukouti, M. D. (2017). More than a Body. In Meyer, C, Streeck, J, and Jordan, J. S. (eds.), Intercorporeality: Emerging Socialities in Interaction. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 289303.Google Scholar
Malafouris, L., and Koukouti, M. D.(2018). How the Body Remembers its Skills: Memory and Material Engagement. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 25(7–8), 158180.Google Scholar
March, P. L. (2019). Playing with Clay and the Uncertainty of Agency: A Material Engagement Theory Perspective. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 18(1), 133151.Google Scholar
Newen, A., de Bruin, L., and Gallagher, S. (eds.) (2018). The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Poulsgaard, K. S. (2019). Enactive Individuation: Technics, Temporality and Affect in Digital Design and Fabrication. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 18(1), 281298.Google Scholar
Poulsgaard, K. S., and Malafouris, L. (2017). Models, Mathematics and Materials in Digital Architecture. In Cowley, S and Vallée-Tourangeau, F (eds.), Cognition Beyond the Brain: Computation, Interactivity, and Human Artifice. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing, 283304.Google Scholar
Renfrew, C. (2004). Towards a Theory of Material Engagement. In DeMarrais, E, Gosden, C, and Renfrew, C (eds.), Rethinking Materiality: The Engagement of Mind with the Material World. Cambridge, UK: The McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, 2331.Google Scholar
Sartre, J. P. (2017). L’imaginaire. Psychologie phénoménologique de l’imagination. Paris, France: Éditions Gallimard.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., Hassabis, D., et al. (2012). The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain. Neuron, 76, 677694.Google Scholar
Simondon, G. (2005). L’Individuation à la lumière des notions de forme et d’information. Grenoble, France: Millon.Google Scholar
Sneath, D., Holbraad, M., and Pedersen, M. A. (2009). Technologies of the Imagination: An Introduction. Ethnos, 74(1), 530.Google Scholar
Strathern, A., Stewart, P. J., and Whitehead, N. L. (2006). Terror and Violence: Imagination and the Unimaginable. London, UK: Pluto Press, 1250.Google Scholar
Suchman, L. A. (1987). Plans and Situated Actions: The Problem of Human-Machine Communication. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Suchman, L. A(2006). Human-Machine Reconfigurations: Plans and Situated Actions. 2nd edition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, C. (2004). Modern Social Imaginaries. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
van Dijk, L., and Rietveld, E. (2018). Situated Anticipation. Synthese, 123.Google Scholar
Walls, M., and Malafouris, L. (2016). Creativity as a Developmental Ecology. In Glaveanu, V. P. (ed.), The Palgrave Handbook of Creativity and Culture Research. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan, 553566.Google Scholar
Willerslev, R. (2006). “To Have the World at a Distance”: Reconsidering the Significance of Vision for Social Anthropology. In Grasseni, C (ed.), Between Apprenticeship and Standards. Oxford, UK: Berghahn.Google Scholar
Willerslev, R.(2007). Soul Hunters: Hunting, Animism, and Personhood Among the Siberian Yukaghirs. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Andreassen, E., Bjerck, H. B., and Olsen, B. (2010). Persistent Memories: Pyramiden – A Soviet Mining Town in the High Arctic. Trondheim, Norway: Tapir Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bailey, D. (2018). Breaking the Surface: An Art/Archaeology of Prehistoric Architecture. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barker, S. (1996). Excavations and Their Objects: Freud’s Collection of Antiquity. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Beck, U. (1992). Risk Society: Towards a New Modernity. London, UK: Sage.Google Scholar
Buchli, V., Lucas, G., and Cox, M. (2001). Archaeologies of the Contemporary Past. London, UK; New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Díaz-Andreu García, M. (2007). A World History of Nineteenth-Century Archaeology: Nationalism, Colonialism, and the Past. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dixon, D. D., and Dixon, J. T. (1903). Upper Coquetdale: Its History, Traditions, Folklore and Scenery. Newcastle-upon-Tyne, UK: Robert Redpath.Google Scholar
Finn, C. (2001). Artifacts: An Archaeologist’s Year in Silicon Valley. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Finn, C.(2004). Past Poetic: Archaeology in the Poetry of W. B. Yeats and Seamus Heaney. London, UK: Duckworth.Google Scholar
Giddens, A. (1991). Modernity and Self-Identity: Self and Society in the Late Modern Age. Cambridge, UK: Blackwell Polity.Google Scholar
González-Ruibal, A. (ed.) (2013). Reclaiming Archaeology: Beyond the Tropes of Modernity. Abingdon, UK; New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
González-Ruibal, A.(2018). An Archaeology of the Contemporary Era. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Graves-Brown, P., Harrison, R., and Piccini, A. (eds.). (2013). The Oxford Handbook of the Archaeology of the Contemporary World. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Harrison, R. (2013). Heritage: Critical Approaches. Milton Park, Abingdon; New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Harrison, R.(2018). On Heritage Ontologies: Rethinking the Material Worlds of Heritage. Anthropological Quarterly, 91, 13651383.Google Scholar
Harrison, R., and Schofield, A. J. (2010). After Modernity: Archaeological Approaches to the Contemporary Past. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hauser, K. (2007). Shadow Sites: Photography, Archaeology, and the British Landscape, 1927–1955. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hobsbawm, E., and Ranger, T. (eds.) (1983). The Invention of Tradition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hodder, I. (1999). The Archaeological Process: An Introduction. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hodder, I.(2012). Entangled: An Archaeology of the Relationships Between Humans and Things. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Holtorf, C. (2005). From Stonehenge to Las Vegas: Archaeology as Popular Culture. Walnut Creek, CA: Altamira Press.Google Scholar
Holtorf, C.(2007). Archaeology Is a Brand: The Meaning of Archaeology in Contemporary Popular Culture. Walnut Creek, CA: Left Coast Press.Google Scholar
Holtorf, C., and Piccini, A. (eds.) (2009). Contemporary Archaeologies: Excavating Now. Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Knappett, C., and Malafouris, L. (2008). Material Agency: Towards a Non-Anthropocentric Approach. Berlin, Germany: Springer.Google Scholar
Kohl, P. L., and Fawcett, C. (1995). Nationalism, Politics and the Practice of Archaeology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lowenthal, D. (2013). The Past Is a Foreign Country – Revisited. Revised and updated edition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lucas, G. (2001). Critical Approaches to Fieldwork: Contemporary and Historical Archaeological Practice. London, UK; New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Malafouris, L. (2013). How Things Shape the Mind: A Theory of Material Engagement. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Moshenska, G. (2006). The Archaeological Uncanny. Public Archaeology, 5(2), 9196.Google Scholar
Moshenska, G.(2012). M. R. James and the Archaeological Uncanny. Antiquity, 86(334), 11921201.Google Scholar
Neville, B., and Villeneuve, J. (eds.) (2002). Waste-Site Stories: The Recycling of Memory. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Olivier, L. (2012). The Dark Abyss of Time: Archaeology and Memory. Lanham, MD: AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
Olsen, B. (2010). In Defense of Things: Archaeology and the Ontology of Objects. Lanham, MD: AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
Olsen, B., and Petusdottir, Þ., (eds.) (2014). Ruin Memories: Materiality, Aesthetics and the Archaeology of the Recent Past. Abingdon, UK; New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Olsen, B., Shanks, M., Webmoor, T., and Witmore, C. (2012). Archaeology: The Discipline of Things. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Parikka, J. (2012). What is Media Archaeology? Cambridge, UK: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Pearson, M., and Shanks, M. (2001). Theatre/Archaeology. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Preucel, R. W., and Mrozowski, S. A. (2010). Contemporary Archaeology in Theory: The New Pragmatism. 2nd edition. Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Rathje, W., Shanks, M., and Witmore, C. (eds.) (2012). Archaeology in the Making: Conversations Through a Discipline. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Rathje, W. L., and Murphy, C. (1992). Rubbish!: The Archaeology of Garbage. New York, NY: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Renfrew, C., and Bahn, P. (2016). Archaeology: Theories, Methods, and Practice. 7th edition. London, UK: Thames and Hudson.Google Scholar
Russell, I., and Cochrane, A. (2013). Art and Archaeology: Collaborations, Conversations, Criticisms. New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Schnapp, A. (1996). The Discovery of the Past: The Origins of Archaeology. Translated by I. Kinnes and G. Varndell. London, UK: British Museum Press.Google Scholar
Schnapp, A. (ed.) (1997). Une archéologie du passé récent. Paris, France: Maison des Sciences de l’Homme.Google Scholar
Schnapp, J., Shanks, M., and Tiews, M. (2004). Archaeologies of the Modern. Modernism/Modernity, 11(1), 116.Google Scholar
Shanks, M. (1992). Experiencing the Past: On the Character of Archaeology. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Shanks, M.(1998). The Life of an Artifact. Fennoscandia Archaeologica, 15, 1542.Google Scholar
Shanks, M.(2012). The Archaeological Imagination. Walnut Creek, CA: Left Coast Press.Google Scholar
Shanks, M.(2013). Let Me Tell You About Hadrian’s Wall: Heritage, Performance, and Design. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Reinwardt Academie.Google Scholar
Shanks, M., and McGuire, R. (1996). The Craft of Archaeology. American Antiquity, 61, 7588.Google Scholar
Shanks, M., and Svabo, C. (2013). Archaeology and Photography: A Pragmatology. In González-Ruibal, A (ed.), Reclaiming Archaeology: Beyond the Tropes of Modernity. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Shanks, M., and Tilley, C. (1987). Reconstructing Archaeology: Theory and Practice. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Shanks, M., and Witmore, C. (2010). Memory Practices and the Archaeological Imagination in Risk Society: Design and Long-Term Community. In Koerner, S and Russell, I (eds.), The Unquiet Past: Theoretical Perspectives on Archaeology and Cultural Heritage. Farnham, UK: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (2006). Uses of Heritage. London, UK; New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Sweet, R. (2004). Antiquaries: The Discovery of the Past in Eighteenth-Century Britain. London, UK: Hambledon.Google Scholar
Thomas, J. (2004). Archaeology and Modernity. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Trigger, B. (1984). Alternative Archaeologies: Nationalist, Colonialist, Imperialist. Man, 19, 355370.Google Scholar
Tringham, R. (2019). Giving Voices (Without Words) to Prehistoric People: Glimpses into an Archaeologist’s Imagination. European Journal of Archaeology, 22(3), 338353.Google Scholar
Wallace, J. (2004). Digging the Dirt: The Archaeological Imagination. London, UK: Duckworth.Google Scholar
Witmore, C. (2007). Symmetrical Archaeology: Excerpts of a Manifesto. World Archaeology, 39(4), 546562.Google Scholar
Zielinski, S. (2006). Deep Time of the Media: Toward an Archaeology of Hearing and Seeing by Technical Means. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

References

Balcerak Jackson, M. (2016). On the Epistemic Value of Imagining, Supposing, and Conceiving. In Kind, A. and Kung, P., (eds.), Knowledge Through Imagination. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 4160.Google Scholar
Brann, E. (1991). The World of Imagination. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. M. J. (2005). The Rational Imagination: How People Create Alternatives to Reality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Church, J. (2010). Seeing Reasons. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 80(3), 638670.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. S. (1998). The Hornswoggle Problem. In Shear, J. (ed.), Explaining Consciousness: The Hard Problem. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 3744.Google Scholar
Currie, G. (1990). The Nature of Fiction. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Currie, G., and Jureidini, J. (2004). Narrative and Coherence. Mind & Language, 19, 409427.Google Scholar
Currie, G., and Ravenscroft, I. (2002). Recreative Minds. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. (1991). Consciousness Explained. Boston, MA: Little, Brown and Company.Google Scholar
Descartes, R. (1641/1986). Meditations on First Philosophy with Selections from the Objections and Replies. Translated by John Cottingham. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Doggett, T., and Egan, A. (2007). Wanting Things You Don’t Want: The Case for an Imaginative Analogue of Desire. Philosophers’ Imprint, 7(9), 117.Google Scholar
Egan, A. (2008). Imagination, Delusion, and Self-Deception. In Bayne, T. and Fernandez, J. (eds.), Delusion and Self-Deception: Affective Influences on Belief-Formation. New York, NY: Psychology Press, 263280.Google Scholar
Gendler, T., and Hawthorne, J. 2002. Conceivability and Possibility. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gertler, B. (2007). In Defense of Mind-Body Dualism. In Feinberg, J. and Shafer-Landau, R. (eds.), Reason and Responsibility. Boston, MA: Wadsworth, 312324.Google Scholar
Gettier, E. (1963). Is Justified True Belief Knowledge? Analysis, 23(6), 121123.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. I. (2006). Simulating Minds: The Philosophy, Psychology, and Neuroscience of Mindreading. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. (1979). Gödel, Escher, Bach: An Eternal Golden Braid. New York, NY: Random House Books.Google Scholar
Humberstone, I. L. (1992). Direction of Fit. Mind, 101(401), 5983.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hume, D. (1739/1985). A Treatise of Human Nature. Edited by Nidditch, P. H.. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jackson, F. (1982). Epiphenomenal Qualia. Philosophical Quarterly, 32, 127136.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1787/1998). Critique of Pure Reason. Translated by P. Guyer and A. Wood. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kind, A. (2001). Putting the Image Back in Imagination. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 62(1), 85109.Google Scholar
Kind, A.(2013). The Heterogeneity of the Imagination. Erkenntnis, 78(1), 141159.Google Scholar
Kind, A.(2016). Desire-Like Imagination. In Kind, A. (ed.), The Routledge Handbook of Philosophy of Imagination. London, UK; New York, NY: Routledge, 163176.Google Scholar
Kind, A.(2017). Imaginative Vividness. Journal of the American Philosophical Association, 3(1), 3250.Google Scholar
Kind, A.(Forthcoming). Imaginative Experience. In Kriegel, U. (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of the Philosophy of Consciousness. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kind, A., and Kung, P. (eds.) (2016). Knowledge Through Imagination. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kriegel, U. (2015). The Varieties of Consciousness. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kung, P. (2010). Imagining as a Guide to Possibility. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 81, 620663.Google Scholar
McGinn, C. (2004). Mindsight. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to Be a Bat? Philosophical Review, 83(October), 435450.Google Scholar
Nanay, B. (2010). Perception and Imagination: Amodal Perception as Mental Imagery. Philosophical Studies, 150, 239254.Google Scholar
Nichols, S. (2004). Imagining and Believing: The Promise of a Single Code. The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, 62(2), 129139.Google Scholar
Parfit, D. (1984). Reasons and Persons. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pavese, C. (2016). Skill in Epistemology I. Philosophy Compass, 11, 642649.Google Scholar
Peacocke, C. (1985). Imagination, Experience and Possibility: A Berkeleian View Defended. In Foster, J. and Robinson, H. (eds.), Essays on Berkeley. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press, 1935.Google Scholar
Perky, C. W. (1910). An Experimental Study of Imagination. American Journal of Psychology, 21, 422452.Google Scholar
Polyani, M. (1966/2009). Creative Imagination. Reprinted in Bardsley, K., Dutton, D., and Krausz, M. (eds.), The Idea of Creativity. Leiden, Netherlands: Brill, 147163.Google Scholar
Rawls, J. (1971). A Theory of Justice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sartre, J. (1936/1962). Imagination. Translated by Forrest Williams. Ann Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Sartre, J.(1940/2010). The Imaginary. Translated by Jonathan Webber. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Schellenberg, S. (2013). Belief and Desire in Imagination and Immersion. Journal of Philosophy, 110(9), 497517.Google Scholar
Shah, N., and Velleman, J. D. (2005). Doxastic Deliberation. Philosophical Review, 114(4), 497534.Google Scholar
Stanley, J., and Williamson, T. (2017). Skill. Noûs, 51(4), 713726.Google Scholar
Stock, K. (2017). Only Imagine. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Strawson, P. F. (1970). Imagination and Perception. In Foster, L. and Swanson, J. W. (eds.), Experience and Theory. Amherst, MA: University of Massachusetts Press, 3154.Google Scholar
Thomson, J. J. (1971). A Defense of Abortion. Philosophy and Public Affairs, 1(1), 4766.Google Scholar
Tidman, P. (1994). Conceivability as a Test for Possibility. American Philosophical Quarterly, 31(4), 297309.Google Scholar
Toon, A. (2012). Models as Make-Believe: Imagination, Fiction, and Scientific Representation. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
van Leeuwen, N. (2013). The Meanings of “Imagine” Part I: Constructive Imagination. Philosophy Compass, 8(3), 220230.Google Scholar
Walton, K. (1990). Mimesis as Make Believe. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Weinberg, J. M., and Meskin, A. (2006). Imagine That! In Kieran, M. (ed.), Contemporary Debates in Aesthetics and the Philosophy of Art. Oxford, UK: Blackwell, 222235.Google Scholar
White, A. R. (1990). The Language of Imagination. Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar

Bibliography

Christie, E. (1979). Indian Philosophers on Poetic Imagination (pratibhā). Journal of Indian Philosophy, 7, 153207.Google Scholar
Eck, D. L. (1998). Darśan: Seeing the Divine Image in India. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Gnoli, R. (1968). The Aesthetic Experience According to Abhinavagupta. Varanasi, India: Chowkhamba Sanskrit Series.Google Scholar
Heesterman, J. C. (1985). Brahmin, Ritual and Renouncer. In The Inner Conflict of Tradition: Essays in Indian Ritual, Kinship, and Society. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Ingalls, D. H. H, Masson, J. M., and Patwardhan, M. V. (1990). The Dhvanyāloka of Ānandavardhana with Abhinava’s Locana. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, W. (1980). Poetry and Speculation of the Ṛg Veda. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Kaviraj, G. N. (1924/1966). The Doctrine of Pratibhā in Indian Philosophy. In Aspects of Indian Thought. Calcutta, India: University of Burdwan, 144.Google Scholar
Kuanpoolpol, P. (1991). Pratibhā: The Concept of Intuition in the Philosophy of Abhinavagupta. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Department of Sanskrit and Indian Studies, PhD Thesis.Google Scholar
Mahoney, W. K. (1997). The Artful Universe. An Introduction to the Vedic Religious Imagination. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Mammaṭa, (1967). Kāvyaprakasha. Edited and translated by Ganganatha Jha. Revised edition, reprint. Varanasi, India: Bharatiya Vidya Prakashan.Google Scholar
Matilal, B. K. (1986). Perception: An Essay on Classical Indian Theories of Knowledge. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Ollett, A. (2013). What is Bhāvanā? Journal of Indian Philosophy, 41, 221262.Google Scholar
Parashar, S. (2000). Rājaśekara’s Kāvyamīmāṃsā. Text and Translation with Explanatory Notes. New Delhi, India: D. K. Printworld.Google Scholar
Pollock, S. (2010). What was Baṭṭa Nāyaka Saying? The Hermeneutical Transformation of Indian Aesthetics. In Pollock, S (ed.), Epics and Argument in Sanskrit Literary History: Essays in Honor of Robert P. Goldman. Delhi, India: Manohar.Google Scholar
Ram-Prasad, C. (2018). Human Being, Bodily Being: Phenomenology from Classical India. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shulman, D. D. (2012). More Than Real: A History of the Imagination in South India. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, B. K. (1998). Reflections on Resemblance, Ritual, and Religion. Delhi, India: Motilal Banarsidass.Google Scholar
Sreekantaiya, T. N. (1980). Imagination in Indian Poetics and Other Literary Studies. Mysore, India: Geetha Book House.Google Scholar
Timalsina, S. (2013). Imagining Reality: Image and Visualization in Classical Hinduism. Southeast Review of Asian Studies 35, 5069.Google Scholar
Nyāya-Pañcāna, Viśvanātha (1940). The Bhāṣāpariccheda with Siddhānta-Muktāvalī. Translated by Swami Madhavananda. Calcutta, India: Advaita Ashrama.Google Scholar
Walimbe, Y. S. (1980). Abhinavagupta on Indian Aesthetics. Delhi, India: Ajanta Publications.Google Scholar
Walimbe, Y. S. Yogavāsiṣṭha (1984). Translated by Swami Venkatesananda. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar

References

Balcerak Jackson, M. (2016). On Imagining, Supposing and Conceiving. In Kind, A. and Kung, P. (eds.), Knowledge Through Imagination. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 4260.Google Scholar
Balcerak Jackson, M.(2018). Justification by Imagination. In Macpherson, F. and Dorsch, F. (eds.), Perceptual Imagination and Perceptual Memory. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 209226.Google Scholar
Bayes, T. (1763). LII. An Essay Towards Solving a Problem in the Doctrine of Chances. By the Late Rev. Mr. Bayes, FRS Communicated by Mr. Price, in a Letter to John Canton, AMFR S. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 53, 370418.Google Scholar
Burr, C., and Jones, M. (2016). The Body as Laboratory: Prediction-Error Minimization, Embodiment, and Representation. Philosophical Psychology, 29(4), 586600.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2006). Language, Embodiment, and the Cognitive Niche. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(8), 370374.Google Scholar
Clark, A.(2013). Whatever Next? Predictive Brains, Situated Agents, and the Future of Cognitive Science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181204.Google Scholar
Clark, A.(2015). Perception as Prediction. In Stokes, D., Matthen, M., and Biggs, S. (eds.), Perception and its Modalities. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 2343.Google Scholar
Clark, A.(2016). Surfing Uncertainty: Prediction, Action and the Embodied Mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Corlett, P. R., Taylor, J. R., Wang, X. J., Fletcher, P. C., and Krystal, J. H. (2010). Toward a Neurobiology of Delusions. Progress in Neurobiology, 92(3), 345369.Google Scholar
De Brigard, F. (2014). Is Memory for Remembering? Recollection as a Form of Episodic Hypothetical Thinking. Synthese, 191(2), 155185.Google Scholar
De Brigard, F.(2017). Memory and Imagination. In Kind, A. (ed.), The Routledge Handbook of Philosophy of Imagination. London, UK; New York, NY: Routledge, 127–140.Google Scholar
Feldman, H., and Friston, K. (2010). Attention, Uncertainty, and Free-Energy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 215.Google Scholar
Friston, K., Adams, R., Perrinet, L., and Breakspear, M. (2012). Perceptions as Hypotheses: Saccades as Experiments. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 151.Google Scholar
Hobson, J. A., and Friston, K. J. (2012). Waking and Dreaming Consciousness: Neurobiological and Functional Considerations. Progress in Neurobiology, 98(1), 8298.Google Scholar
Hohwy, J. (2013). The Predictive Mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Keats, J. (1958). The Letters of John Keats, 1814–1821. Edited by H. E. Rollins. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kind, A. (2001). Putting the Image Back in Imagination. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 62(1), 85109.Google Scholar
Kind, A.(2016). Imagining Under Constraints. In Kind, A. and Kung, P. (eds.), Knowledge Through Imagination. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 145159.Google Scholar
Kirchhoff, M. D. (2018). Predictive Processing, Perceiving and Imagining: Is to Perceive to Imagine, or Something Close to it? Philosophical Studies, 175(3), 751767.Google Scholar
Langland‐Hassan, P. (2015). Imaginative Attitudes. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 90(3), 664686.Google Scholar
Langland‐Hassan, P.(2016). On Choosing What to Imagine. In A. Kind, and P. Kung, (eds.), Knowledge Through Imagination. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 6184.Google Scholar
Lupyan, G. (2012). Language Augmented Prediction. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 422.Google Scholar
Lupyan, G., and Clark, A. (2015). Words and the World: Predictive Coding and the Language-Perception-Cognition Interface. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(4), 279284.Google Scholar
Lupyan, G., and Ward, E. J. (2013). Language Can Boost Otherwise Unseen Objects into Visual Awareness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(35), 1419614201.Google Scholar
Metzinger, T. (2017). The Problem of Mental Action: Predictive Control Without Sensory Sheets. In Metzinger, T. and Wiese, W. (eds.), Philosophy and Predictive Processing. Mainz, Germany: Johannes Gutenberg-Universität.Google Scholar
Metzinger, T.(2018). Why is Mind-Wandering Interesting for Philosophers? In Christoff, K. and Fox, K. C. R. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Spontaneous Thought: Mind-Wandering, Creativity, and Dreaming. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 97111.Google Scholar
O’Regan, J. K., and Noë, A. (2001). A Sensorimotor Account of Vision and Visual Consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 939973.Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G. (2011). Grounding Procedural and Declarative Knowledge in Sensorimotor Anticipation. Mind & Language, 26(1), 78114.Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G.(2012). An Active Inference View of Cognitive Control. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 478.Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G.(2014). Why Do You Fear the Bogeyman? An Embodied Predictive Coding Model of Perceptual Inference. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14(3), 902911.Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G.(2017). Tracing the Roots of Cognition in Predictive Processing. In Metzinger, T. and Wiese, W. (eds.), Philosophy and Predictive Processing. Mainz, Germany: Johannes Gutenberg-Universität.Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G., and Castelfranchi, C. (2009). Thinking as the Control of Imagination: A Conceptual Framework for Goal-Directed Systems. Psychological Research PRPF, 73(4), 559577.Google Scholar
Seth, A. K. (2014). A Predictive Processing Theory of Sensorimotor Contingencies: Explaining the Puzzle of Perceptual Presence and its Absence in Synesthesia. Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(2), 97118.Google Scholar
van Leeuwen, N. (2013). The Meanings of “Imagine” Part I: Constructive Imagination. Philosophy Compass, 8(3), 220230.Google Scholar
Weisberg, D. S., and Goodstein, J. (2009). What Belongs in a Fictional World? Journal of Cognition and Culture, 9(1–2), 6978.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, S. (2014). Accounting for the Phenomenology and Varieties of Auditory Verbal Hallucination Within a Predictive Processing Framework. Consciousness and Cognition, 30, 142155.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, S., and Fernyhough, C. (2017). Auditory Verbal Hallucinations and Inner Speech: A Predictive Processing Perspective. In Radman, Z. (ed.), Before Consciousness: In Search of the Fundamentals of the Mind. Exeter, UK: Imprint Academic, Limited, 372397.Google Scholar
Williamson, T. (2016). Knowing by Imagining. In Kind, A. and Kung, P. (eds.), Knowledge Through Imagination. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 113123.Google Scholar
Yablo, S. (1993). Is Conceivability a Guide to Possibility? Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 53(1), 142.Google Scholar

References

Abraham, A., Schubotz, R. I., and von Cramon, D. Y. (2008). Thinking About the Future versus the Past in Personal and Non-Personal Contexts. Brain Research, 1233, 106119.Google Scholar
Abram, M., Picard, L., Navarro, B., and Piolino, P. (2014). Mechanisms of Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future: New Data from Autobiographical Memory Tasks in a Lifespan Approach. Consciousness and Cognition, 29, 7680.Google Scholar
Addis, D. R. (2018). Are Episodic Memories Special? On the Sameness of Remembered and Imagined Event Simulation. Journal of the Royal Society of New Zealand, 48(23), 125.Google Scholar
Addis, D. R., Cheng, T., Roberts, R. P., and Schacter, D. L. (2011). Hippocampal Contributions to the Episodic Simulation of Specific and General Future Events. Hippocampus, 21, 10451052.Google Scholar
Addis, D. R., Musicaro, R., Pan, L., and Schacter, D. L. (2010). Episodic Simulation of Past and Future Events in Older Adults: Evidence from an Experimental Recombination Task. Psychology and Aging, 25, 369376.Google Scholar
Addis, D. R., Pan, L., Vu, M. A., Laiser, N., and Schacter, D. L. (2009). Constructive Episodic Simulation of the Future and the Past: Distinct Subsystems of a Core Brain Network Mediate Imagining and Remembering. Neuropsychologia, 47, 22222238.Google Scholar
Addis, D. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). Constructive Episodic Simulation: Temporal Distance and Detail of Past and Future Events Modulate Hippocampal Engagement. Hippocampus, 18, 227237.Google Scholar
Addis, D. R., and Schacter, D. L.(2012). The Hippocampus and Imagining the Future: Where Do We Stand? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 173.Google Scholar
Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., and Schacter, D. L. (2007). Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future: Common and Distinct Neural Substrates During Event Construction and Elaboration. Neuropsychologia, 45, 13631377.Google Scholar
Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., and Schacter, D. L.(2008). Age-Related Changes in the Episodic Simulation of Future Events. Psychological Science, 19, 3341.Google Scholar
Anderson, R. J., Dewhurst, S. A., and Nash, R. A. (2012). Shared Cognitive Processes Underlying Past and Future Thinking: The Impact of Imagery and Concurrent Task Demands on Event Specificity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 356365.Google Scholar
Axelrod, V., Rees, G., and Bar, M. (2017). The Default Network and the Combination of Cognitive Processes that Mediate Self-Generated Thought. Nature Human Behaviour, 1, 896910. doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0244-9.Google Scholar
Bar, M., and Aminoff, E. (2003). Cortical Analysis of Visual Context. Neuron, 38, 347358.Google Scholar
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., and Schacter, D. L. (2016). Creative Cognition and Brain Network Dynamics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 8795.Google Scholar
Benoit, R. G., and Schacter, D. L. (2015). Specifying the Core Network Supporting Episodic Simulation and Episodic Memory by Activation Likelihood Estimation. Neuropsychologia, 75, 450457.Google Scholar
Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., and Conant, L. L. (2009). Where is the Semantic System? A Critical Review and Meta-Analysis of 120 Functional Neuroimaging Studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 27672796.Google Scholar
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The Brain’s Default Network: Anatomy, Function, and Relevance to Disease. The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 138.Google Scholar
Buckner, R. L., and Carroll, D. C. (2007). Self-Projection and the Brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 4957.Google Scholar
Cabeza, R., Ciaramelli, E., and Moscovitch, M. (2012). Cognitive Contributions of the Ventral Parietal Cortex: An Integrative Theoretical Account. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 338352.Google Scholar
Campbell, K. L., Madore, K. P., Benoit, R. G., Thakral, P. P., and Schacter, D. L. (2018). Increased Hippocampus to Ventromedial Prefrontal Connectivity during the Construction of Episodic Future Events. Hippocampus, 28, 3945.Google Scholar
Cao, X., Madore, K. P., Wang, D., and Schacter, D. L. (2018). Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future: Attachment Effects on Production of Episodic Details in Close Relationships. Memory, 26, 20402050.Google Scholar
Carpenter, A. C., and Schacter, D. L. (2017). Flexible Retrieval: When True Inferences Produce False Memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43, 335349.Google Scholar
Carpenter, A. C., and Schacter, D. L.(2018). False Memories, False Preferences: Flexible Retrieval Mechanisms Supporting Successful Inference Bias Novel Decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(7), 9881004. dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000391.Google Scholar
Cavanna, A. E., and Trimble, M. R. (2006). The Precuneus: A Review of Its Functional Anatomy and Behavioural Correlates. Brain, 129, 564583.Google Scholar
Cohen, N. J., and Eichenbaum, H. (1993). Memory, Amnesia, and the Hippocampal System. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A., and van der Linden, M. (2004). Phenomenal Characteristics Associated with Projecting Oneself Back into the Past and Forward into the Future: Influence of Valence and Temporal Distance. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 844858.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A., and van der Linden, M.(2006). Individual Differences in the Phenomenology of Mental Time Travel. Consciousness and Cognition, 15, 342350.Google Scholar
De Brigard, F. (2014). Is Memory for Remembering? Recollection as a Form of Episodic Hypothetical Thinking. Synthese, 191, 131.Google Scholar
De Brigard, F., Addis, D. R., Ford, J. H., Schacter, D. L., and Giovanello, K. S. (2013). Remembering What Could Have Happened: Neural Correlates of Episodic Counterfactual Thinking. Neuropsychologia, 51, 24012414.Google Scholar
Devitt, A. L., and Addis, D. R. (2016). Bidirectional Interactions Between Memory and Imagination. In Michaelian, K, Klein, S. B., and Szpunar, K. K. (eds.), Seeing the Future: Theoretical Perspectives on Future-Oriented Mental Time Travel. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Devitt, A. L., Monk-Fromont, E., Schacter, D. L., and Addis, D. R. (2016). Factors that Influence the Generation of Autobiographical Memory Conjunction Errors. Memory, 24, 204222.Google Scholar
Devitt, A. L., and Schacter, D. L. (2018). An Optimistic Outlook Creates a Rosy Past: The Impact of Episodic Simulation on Subsequent Memory. Psychological Science, 29, 936946. dx.doi.org/10.1177%2F0956797617753936.Google Scholar
Dewhurst, S. A., Anderson, R. J., Grace, L., and Howe, D. (2018). Simulation, False Memories, and the Planning of Future Events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 45(1), 2636. doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000575.Google Scholar
Dewhurst, S. A., Anderson, R. J., Grace, L., and van Esch, L. (2016). Adaptive False Memories: Imagining Future Scenarios Increases False Memories in the DRM Paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 44, 10761084.Google Scholar
Dudai, Y., and Carruthers, M. (2005). The Janus Face of Mnemosyne. Nature, 434, 823824.Google Scholar
Fisher, R. P., and Geiselman, R. E. (1992). Memory-Enhancing Techniques for Investigative Interviewing: The Cognitive Interview. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas Books.Google Scholar
Gaesser, B., Sacchetti, D. C., Addis, D. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2011). Characterizing Age-Related Changes in Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future. Psychology and Aging, 26, 8084.Google Scholar
Gaesser, B., Spreng, R. N., McLelland, V. C., Addis, D. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2013). Imagining the Future: Evidence for a Hippocampal Contribution to Constructive Processing. Hippocampus, 23, 11501161.Google Scholar
Gerlach, K. D., Dornblaser, D. W., and Schacter, D. L. (2014). Adaptive Constructive Processes and Memory Accuracy: Consequences of Counterfactual Simulations in Young and Older Adults. Memory, 22, 145162.Google Scholar
Gilmore, A. W., Nelson, S. M., and McDermott, K. B. (2016). The Contextual Association Network Activates More for Remembered than Imagined Events. Cerebral Cortex, 26, 611617.Google Scholar
Hallford, D. J., Austin, D. W., Takano, K., and Raes, F. (2018). Psychopathology and Episodic Future Thinking: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Specificity and Episodic Detail. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 102, 4251.Google Scholar
Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D., and Maguire, E. A. (2007a). Using Imagination to Understand the Neural Basis of Episodic Memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 1436514374.Google Scholar
Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D., Vann, D. S., and Maguire, E. A. (2007b). Patients with Hippocampal Amnesia Cannot Imagine New Experiences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 104, 17261731.Google Scholar
Irish, M., Addis, D. R., Hodges, J. R., and Piguet, O. (2012). Considering the Role of Semantic Memory in Episodic Future Thinking: Evidence from Semantic Dementia. Brain, 135, 21782191.Google Scholar
Jing, H. G., Madore, K. P., and Schacter, D. L. (2016). Worrying About the Future: An Episodic Specificity Induction Affects Problem Solving, Reappraisal, and Well-Being. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 402418.Google Scholar
Jing, H. G., Madore, K. P., and Schacter, D. L.(2017). Preparing for What Might Happen: An Episodic Specificity Induction Impacts the Generation of Alternative Future Events. Cognition, 169, 118128.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. K., and Raye, C. L. (1981). Realty Monitoring. Psychological Review, 88, 6785.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B. (2013). The Complex Act of Projecting Oneself into the Future. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews – Cognitive Science, 4, 6379.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., and Kihlstrom, J. F. (2002) Memory and Temporal Experience: The Effects of Episodic Memory Loss on an Amnesic Patient’s Ability to Remember the Past and Imagine the Future. Social Cognition, 20, 353379.Google Scholar
Levine, B., Svoboda, E., Hay, J. F., Winocur, G., and Moscovitch, M. (2002). Aging and Autobiographical Memory: Dissociating Episodic from Semantic Retrieval. Psychology and Aging, 17, 677689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madore, K. P., Addis, D. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2015). Creativity and Memory: Effects of an Episodic-Specificity Induction on Divergent Thinking. Psychological Science, 26, 14611468.Google Scholar
Madore, K. P., Gaesser, B., and Schacter, D. L. (2014). Constructive Episodic Simulation: Dissociable Effects of a Specificity Induction on Remembering, Imagining, and Describing in Young and Older Adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 609622.Google Scholar
Madore, K. P., Jing, H. G., and Schacter, D. L. (2016). Divergent Creative Thinking in Young and Older Adults: Extending the Effects of an Episodic Specificity Induction. Memory & Cognition, 44, 974988.Google Scholar
Madore, K. P., and Schacter, D. L. (2014). An Episodic Specificity Induction Enhances Means-End Problem-Solving in Young and Older Adults. Psychology and Aging, 29, 913924.Google Scholar
Madore, K. P., and Schacter, D. L.(2016). Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future: Selective Effects of an Episodic Specificity Induction on Detail Generation. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 285298.Google Scholar
Madore, K. P., Szpunar, K. K., Addis, D. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2016). Episodic Specificity Induction Impacts Activity in a Core Brain Network During Construction of Imagined Future Experiences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 113, 1069610701.Google Scholar
Madore, K. P., Thakral, P. P., Beaty, R. E., Addis, D. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2019). Neural Mechanisms of Episodic Retrieval Support Divergent Creative Thinking. Cerebral Cortex, 29(1), 150166. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhx312.Google Scholar
Mahr, J., and Csibra, G. (2018). Why Do We Remember? The Communicative Function of Episodic Memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 41, e1.Google Scholar
Martin, V. C., Schacter, D. L., Corballis, M. C., and Addis, D. R. (2011). A Role for the Hippocampus in Encoding Simulations of Future Events. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 1385813863.Google Scholar
McDermott, K. B., Woolridge, C. L., Rice, H. J., Berg, J. J., and Szpunar, K. K. (2016). Visual Perspective in Remembering and Episodic Future Thought. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 243253.Google Scholar
McFarland, C. P., Primosch, M., Maxson, C. M., and Stewart, B. T. (2017). Enhancing Memory and Imagination Improves Problem-Solving Among Individuals with Depression. Memory & Cognition, 45, 18.Google Scholar
Michaelian, K. (2016). Mental Time Travel: Episodic Memory and our Knowledge of the Personal Past. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Michaelian, K., Klein, S. B., and Szpunar, K. K. (eds.) (2016). Seeing the Future: Theoretical Perspective on Future-Oriented Mental Time Travel. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mullally, S. L., and Maguire, E. A. (2014). Memory, Imagination, and Predicting the Future: A Common Brain Mechanism? Neuroscientist, 20, 220234.Google Scholar
Okuda, J., Fujii, T., Ohtake, H., et al. (2003). Thinking of the Future and the Past: The Roles of the Frontal Pole and the Medial Temporal Lobes. NeuroImage, 19, 13691380.Google Scholar
Palombo, D. J., Hayes, S. M., Peterson, K. M., Keane, M. M., and Verfaellie, M. (2018). Medial Temporal Lobe Contributions to Episodic Future Thinking: Scene Construction or Future Projection? Cerebral Cortex, 28, 447458.Google Scholar
Palombo, D. J., Williams, L. J., Abdi, H., and Levine, B. (2013). The Survey of Autobiographical Memory (SAM): A Novel Measure of Trait Mnemonics in Everyday Life. Cortex, 49, 15261540.Google Scholar
Platt, J., and Spivack, G. (1975). Manual for the Means-End Problem-Solving Test (MEPS): A Measure of Interpersonal Problem-Solving Skill. Philadelphia, PA: Hahnemann Medical College and Hospital.Google Scholar
Raes, F., Hermans, D., Williams, J. M. G., et al. (2005). Reduced Specificity of Autobiographical Memory: A Mediator Between Rumination and Ineffective Social Problem-Solving in Major Depression? Journal of Affective Disorders, 87, 331335.Google Scholar
Raichle, M. E. (2015). The Brain’s Default Network. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 38, 433447.Google Scholar
Roberts, R. P., and Addis, D. R. (2018). A Common Mode of Processing Governing Divergent Thinking and Future Imagination. In Jung, R and Vartanian, O (eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of the Neuroscience of Creativity (Cambridge Handbooks in Psychology). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 211230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, R. P., Schacter, D. L., and Addis, D. R. (2018). Scene Construction and Relational Processing: Separable Constructs? Cerebral Cortex, 28, 17291732.Google Scholar
Roberts, R. P., Wiebels, K., Sumner, R. L., et al. (2017). An fMRI Investigation of the Relationship Between Future Imagination and Cognitive Flexibility. Neuropsychologia, 95, 156172.Google Scholar
Robin, J., Wynn, J., and Moscovitch, M. (2016). The Spatial Scaffold: The Effects of Spatial Context on Memory for Events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42, 308315.Google Scholar
Roediger, H. L., and McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating False Memories: Remembering Words not Presented in Lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803814.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L. (2012). Adaptive Constructive Processes and the Future of Memory. American Psychologist, 67, 603613.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., and Addis, D. R. (2007a). Constructive Memory: The Ghosts of Past and Future. Nature, 445, 27.Google Scholar
Schacter, D.L., and Addis, D.R.(2007b). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Constructive Memory: Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 362, 773786.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., and Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: The Prospective Brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(9), 657661.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., and Buckner, R. L.(2008). Episodic Simulation of Future Events: Concepts, Data, and Applications. The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 3960.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., Hassabis, D., et al. (2012). The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain. Neuron, 76, 677694.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., and Szpunar, K. K. (2017). Escaping the Past: Contributions of the Hippocampus to Future Thinking and Imagination. In Hannula, D. E. and Duff, M. C. (eds.), The Hippocampus from Cells to Systems: Structure, Connectivity, and Functional Contributions to Memory and Flexible Cognition. New York, NY: Springer, 439465.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Benoit, R. G., and Szpunar, K. K. (2017). Episodic Future Thinking: Mechanisms and Functions. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 17, 4150.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Carpenter, A. C., Devitt, A., Roberts, R. P., and Addis, D. R. (2018). Constructive Episodic Simulation, Flexible Recombination, and Memory Errors. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 41, e32.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Gaesser, B., and Addis, D. R. (2013). Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future in the Elderly. Gerontology, 59, 143151.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., and Madore, K. P. (2016). Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future: Identifying and Enhancing the Contribution of Episodic Memory. Memory Studies, 9, 245255.Google Scholar
Sheldon, S., and Levine, B. (2016). The Role of the Hippocampus in Memory and Mental Construction. The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1369, 7692.Google Scholar
Sheldon, S., McAndrews, M. P., and Moscovitch, M. (2011). Episodic Memory Processes Mediated by the Medial Temporal Lobes Contribute to Open-Ended Problem-Solving. Neuropsychologia, 49, 24392447.Google Scholar
Simons, J. S., Garrison, J. R., and Johnson, M. K. (2017). Brain Mechanisms of Reality Monitoring. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21, 462473.Google Scholar
St. Jacques, P. L., Carpenter, A. C., Szpunar, K. K., and Schacter, D. L. (2018). Remembering and Imagining Alternative Versions of the Personal Past. Neuropsychologia, 110, 170179.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T., and Corballis, M. C. (2007). The Evolution of Foresight: What is Mental Time Travel and is it Unique to Humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299313.Google Scholar
Szpunar, K. K., and McDermott, K. B. (2008). Episodic Future Thought and its Relation to Remembering: Evidence from Ratings of Subjective Experience. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 330334.Google Scholar
Szpunar, K. K., Spreng, R. N., and Schacter, D. L. (2014). A Taxonomy of Prospection: Introducing an Organizational Framework for Future-Oriented Cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 111, 1841418421.Google Scholar
Szpunar, K. K., Watson, J. M., and McDermott, K. B. (2007). Neural Substrates of Envisioning the Future. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 642647.Google Scholar
Thakral, P., Benoit, R. G., and Schacter, D. L. (2017a). Characterizing the Role of the Hippocampus During Episodic Simulation and Encoding. Hippocampus, 27, 12751284.Google Scholar
Thakral, P., Benoit, R. G., and Schacter, D. L.(2017b). Imagining the Future: The Core Episodic Simulation Network Dissociates as a Function of Timecourse and the Amount of Simulated Information. Cortex, 90, 1230.Google Scholar
Thakral, P., Madore, K. P., and Schacter, D. L. (2017). A Role for the Left Angular Gyrus in Episodic Simulation and Memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 37, 81428149.Google Scholar
Thakral, P., Madore, K. P., and Schacter, D. L.(2019). Content-Specific Phenomenological Similarity Between Episodic Memory and Simulation. Memory, 27(3), 417422.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and Consciousness. Canadian Psychologist, 26, 112.Google Scholar
Tulving, E.(2002). Episodic Memory: From Mind to Brain. Annual Review of Psychology 53, 125.Google Scholar
van Mulukom, V., Schacter, D. L., Corballis, M. C., and Addis, D. R. (2013). Re-Imagining the Future: Repetition Decreases Hippocampal Involvement in Future Simulation. PLoS One, 8, e69596.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viard, A., Desgranges, B., Eustache, F., and Piolino, P. (2012). Factors Affecting Medial Temporal Lobe Engagement for Past and Future Episodic Events: An ALE Meta-Analysis of Neuroimaging Studies. Brain and Cognition, 80, 111.125.Google Scholar
Wang, Q., Hou, Y., Tang, H., and Wiprovnick, A. (2011). Travelling Backwards and Forwards in Time: Culture and Gender in the Episodic Specificity of Past and Future Events. Memory, 19, 103109.Google Scholar
Ward, A. M. (2016). A Critical Evaluation of Episodic Future Thinking. Neuropsychology, 30, 887905.Google Scholar
Williams, J. M. G., Ellis, N. C., Tyers, C., et al. (1996). The Specificity of Autobiographical Memory and Imaginability of the Future. Memory and Cognition, 24, 116125.Google Scholar
Zeithamova, D., and Preston, A. R. (2010). Flexible Memories: Differential Roles for Medial Temporal Lobe and Prefrontal Cortex in Cross-Episode Binding. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 1467614684.Google Scholar

References

Abraham, A. (2016). The Imaginative Mind. Human Brain Mapping, 37, 41974211.Google Scholar
Bar, M. (2007). The Proactive Brain: Using Analogies and Associations to Generate Predictions. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11, 280289.Google Scholar
Bar, M.(2009). The Proactive Brain: Memory for Prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 12351243.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (2009). Simulation, Situated Conceptualization, and Prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 12811289.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind Variation and Selective Retention in Creative Thought as in Other Knowledge Processes. Psychological Review, 67, 380400.Google Scholar
Chatterjee, A., and Vartanian, O. (2014). Neuroaesthetics. Trends in Cognitive Science, 18, 370375.Google Scholar
Christoff, K. (2012). Undirected Thought: Neural Determinants and Correlates. Brain Research, 1428, 5159.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1976). The Selfish Gene. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1995). Darwin’s Dangerous Idea. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Dietrich, A. (2004). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Creativity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 10111026.Google Scholar
Dietrich, A.(2015). How Creativity Happens in the Brain. London, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Dietrich, A.(2019). Types of Creativity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(1), 112.Google Scholar
Dietrich, A., and Haider, H. (2017). A Neurocognitive Framework for Human Creative Thought. Frontiers in Psychology: Cognitive Science, 7, 20782085.Google Scholar
Fisher, J. C. (2006). Does Simulation Theory Really Involve Simulation? Philosophical Psychology, 19, 417432.Google Scholar
Frith, C. D. (1992). The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gendler, T. (2013). Imagination. In Zalta, E. N. (ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall). plato.stanford.edu/entries/imagination/.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1979). Shades of Lamarck. Natural History, 88, 2228.Google Scholar
Grush, R. (2004). The Emulation Theory of Representation: Motor Control, Imagery, and Perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 377396.Google Scholar
Kronfeldner, M. E. (2007). Is Cultural Evolution Lamarckian? Biology and Philosophy, 22, 493512.Google Scholar
Kronfeldner, M. E.(2010). Darwinian “Blind” Hypothesis Formation Revisited. Synthese, 175, 193218.Google Scholar
Lewontin, R. C. (1970). The Units of Selection. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 1, 118.Google Scholar
Lewontin, R. C.(1991). Biology as Ideology. New York, NY: Harper.Google Scholar
Llinás, R. R. (2001). I of the Vortex: From Neurons to Self. Boston, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayr, E. (1981). The Growth of Biological Thought: Diversity, Evolution, and Inheritance. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Medawar, P. M. (1953). A Commentary on Lamarckism. Reprinted in Medawar, P (1981), The Uniqueness of the Individual. New York, NY: Dover, 6387.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J., and Boyd, R. (2005). Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Schooler, J. W., and Dougal, S. (1999). Why Creativity Is Not like the Proverbial Typing Monkey. Psychological Inquiry, 10, 351356.Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N., and Metzler, J. (1971). Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects. Science, 171, 701703.Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1999). Creativity as Blind Variation and Selective Retention: Is the Creative Process Darwinian? Psychological Inquiry, 10, 309328.Google Scholar
Singer, J. L. (1975). Navigating the Stream of Consciousness: Research in Daydreaming and Related Inner Experiences. American Psychologist, 30, 727738.Google Scholar
Smith, E. A. (2013). Agency and Adaptation: New Directions in Evolutionary Anthropology. Annual Review of Anthropology, 41, 103120.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1998). Cognitive Mechanisms in Human Creativity: Is Variation Blind or Sighted? Journal of Creative Behavior, 32, 159176.Google Scholar
Stevenson, L. (2003). Twelve Conceptions of Imagination. The British Journal of Aesthetics, 43(3), 238259.Google Scholar
Weisberg, R. W., and Hass, R. (2007). We Are All Partly Right: Comment on Simonton. Creativity Research Journal, 19, 345360.Google Scholar
Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K., and Kawato, M. (2003). A Unifying Computational Framework for Motor Control and Social Interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society Series B, 358, 593602.Google Scholar

References

Akkerman, S. (2018). Imagination in and Beyond Education. In Zittoun, T. and Glăveanu, V. P. (eds.), Handbook of Imagination and Culture. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 211221.Google Scholar
Anderson, B. (1991). Imagined Communities: Reflections on the Origin and Spread of Nationalism. London, UK: Verso Books.Google Scholar
Appadurai, A. (2013). The Future as Cultural Fact. In Essays on the Global Condition. London, UK; New York, NY: Verso Books.Google Scholar
Bakhtin, M. M. (1981). The Dialogic Imagination: Four Essays. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Bartlett, F. C. (1916). An Experimental Study of Some Problems of Perceiving and Imagining. British Journal of Psychology, 8, 222266.Google Scholar
Bartlett, F. C.(1928). Types of Imagination. Journal of Philosophical Studies, 3(9), 7885.Google Scholar
Benson, C. (1993). The Absorbed Self. Hemel Hempstead, UK: Harvester Wheatsheaf.Google Scholar
Benson, C.(2001). The Cultural Psychology of Self: Place, Morality and Art in Human Worlds. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Bloch, E. (1995). The Principle of Hope. Translated by N. Plaice. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bloch, M. (2008). Why Religion Is Nothing Special but Is Central. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 363(1499), 20552061. doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0007.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1990). Acts of Meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. M. J. (2005). The Rational Imagination: How People Create Alternatives to Reality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Castoriadis, C. (1997). Anthropology, Philosophy, Politics. Thesis Eleven, 49(1), 99116.Google Scholar
Cerchia, F. (2009). Young Children’s Use of Symbolic Resources in an Experimental Setting Testing Metaphor Comprehension. Psychology & Society, 2(2), 200211.Google Scholar
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural Psychology: A Once and Future Discipline. Cambridge, MA; London, UK: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Cornejo, C. (2017). From Fantasy to Imagination: A Cultural History and a Moral for Cultural Psychology. In Wagoner, B., Brescó de Luna, I., and Awad, S. H. (eds.), The Psychology of Imagination: History, Theory, and New Research Horizons. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing, 344. www.ccp.aau.dk/digitalAssets/98/98719_from-20fantasy-20to-20imagination.pdf.Google Scholar
Daiute, C. (2017). Imagination in Community Engagement. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. www.oxfordscholarship.com/view/10.1093/oso/9780190468712.001.0001/oso-9780190468712-chapter-13.Google Scholar
de Saint-Laurent, C. (2018). Thinking Through Time: From Collective Memories to Collective Futures. In de Saint Laurent, C., Obradović, S., and Carriere, K. R. (eds.), Imagining Collective Futures. Place of publication not identified: Palgrave MacMillan, 5981.Google Scholar
de Saint-Laurent, C., Obradović, S., and Carriere, K. R. (2018). Imagining Collective Futures: Perspectives from Social, Cultural and Political Psychology. London: Palgrave MacMillan.Google Scholar
de Saint-Laurent, C., and Zittoun, T. (2018). Memory in Life Transitions. In Wagoner, B. (ed.), Oxford Handbook of Culture and Memory. Oxford, UK; New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 209236.Google Scholar
Descartes, R. (1641). Meditationes de prima philosophia, in qua Dei existentia et animae immortalitas demonstrantur. Paris, France: Michel Soly. gallica.bnf.fr/ark:/12148/btv1b86002964.r=.langEN.Google Scholar
Diriwächter, R., and Valsiner, J. (eds.) (2008). Striving for the Whole: Creating Theoretical Syntheses. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1959). Creative Writers and Day-Dreaming. In Strachey, J. (ed.), The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud. Original German publication 1907. Volume 9. London, UK: The Hogarth Press and the Institute of Psychoanalysis, 141154.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1963). Jokes and their Relation to the Unconscious. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (2001). The Interpretation of Dreams. Original publication 1900 (Volumes 4–5). London, UK: Vintage.Google Scholar
Gillespie, A., Corti, K., and Heasman, B. (2018). Imagining the Self through Cultural Technologies. In Zittoun, T. and Glăveanu, V. P. (eds.), Handbook of Imagination and Culture. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 301318.Google Scholar
Gillespie, A., and Martin, J. (2014). Position Exchange Theory: A Socio-Material Basis for Discursive and Psychological Positioning. New Ideas in Psychology, 32, 7379. doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2013.05.001.Google Scholar
Glăveanu, V. P. (2014). Distributed Creativity: Thinking Outside the Box of the Creative Individual. New York, NY: Springer Science + Business Media.Google Scholar
Glăveanu, V. P., and de Saint-Laurent, C. (2015). Political Imagination, Otherness and the European Crisis. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 11(4), 557564. doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v11i4.1085.Google Scholar
Glăveanu, V. P., de Saint-Laurent, C., and Literat, I. (2018). Making Sense of Refugees Online: Perspective Taking, Political Imagination, and Internet Memes. American Behavioral Scientist, 62(4), 440457. doi.org/10.1177/0002764218765060.Google Scholar
Glăveanu, V. P., Karwowski, M., Jankowska, D. M., and de Saint-Laurent, C. (2018). Creative Imagination. In Zittoun, T. and Glăveanu, V. P. (eds.), Handbook of Imagination and Culture. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 6186.Google Scholar
Hargreaves, D. J., Miell, D., and MacDonald, R. A. R. (eds.). (2012). Musical Imaginations: Multidisciplinary Perspectives on Creativity, Performance, and Perception. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. (2000). The Work of the Imagination. 1st edition. Oxford, UK/Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hawlina, H., Gillespie, A., and Zittoun, T. (2017). Difficult Differences: A Socio-Cultural Analysis of how Diversity can Enable and Inhibit Creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 0/online first, 0, 112. doi.org/10.1002/jocb.182.Google Scholar
Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways With Words: Language, Life, and Work in Communities and Classrooms. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hedegaard, M., Fleer, M., Bang, J., and Hviid, P. (2008). Studying Children: A Cultural-Historical Approach. Maidenhead, UK: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Hilppö, J. A., Rajala, A., Zittoun, T., Kumpulainen, K., and Lipponen, L. (2016). Interactive Dynamics of Imagination in a Science Classroom. Frontline Learning Research, 4(4), 2029. doi.org/10.14786/flr.v4i4.213.Google Scholar
Hviid, P., and Villadsen, J. W. (2018). Playing and Being – Imagination in the Life Course. In Zittoun, T. and Glăveanu, V. P. (eds.), Handbook of Imagination and Culture. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 137166.Google Scholar
Jovchelovitch, S. (2014). The Creativity of the Social: Imagination, Development and Social Change in Rio de Janeiro’s Favelas. In Glăveanu, V. P., Gillespie, A., and Valsiner, J. (eds.), Rethinking Creativity: Contributions from Cultural Psychology. New York, NY: Routledge, 7692.Google Scholar
Jovchelovitch, S., and Hawlina, H. (2018). Utopias and World-Making. In de Saint-Laurent, C., Obradovic, S., and Carriere, K. (eds.), Imagining Collective Futures: Perspectives from Social, Cultural and Political Psychology. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan, 129151.Google Scholar
Jovchelovitch, S., and Priego-Hernández, J. (2013). Underground Sociabilities: Identity, Culture and Resistance in Rio de Janeiro’s Favelas. Brasilia: UNESCO. www.unesco.org/new/en/brasilia/.Google Scholar
Kadianaki, I. (2010). Negotiating Immigration Through Symbolic Resources: The Case of Immigrants Living in Greece. PhD Dissertation. Cambridge, UK: University of Cambridge.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1787/1999). Critique of Pure Reason. Edited by P. Guyer and A. Wood. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Klempe, H. (2018). Music and Imagination. In Zittoun, T. and Glăveanu, V. P. (eds.), Handbook of Imagination and Culture. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 243269.Google Scholar
Kuhn, A. (2013a). Cultural Experience and the Gallery Film. In Kuhn, A. (ed.), Little Madnesses: Winnicott, Transitional Phenomena and Cultural Experience. London, UK: Tauris, 159172.Google Scholar
Kuhn, A. (ed.). (2013b). Little Madnesses: Winnicott, Transitional Phenomena and Cultural Experience. London, UK: Tauris.Google Scholar
Lehmann, O. V., Chaudhary, N., Bastos, A. C., and Abbey, E. (eds.). (2017). Poetry and Imagined Worlds. London, UK: Palgrave.Google Scholar
Lennon, K. (2010). Re-Enchanting the World: The Role of Imagination in Perception. Philosophy, 85(3), 375389. doi.org/10.1017/S0031819110000239.Google Scholar
Marková, I. (2016). The Dialogical Mind: Common Sense and Ethics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McBride, K. D. (2005). Collective Dreams: Political Imagination and Community. Pennsylvania, PA: Pennsylvania State University Press. www.jstor.org/stable/10.5325/j.ctt7v537.Google Scholar
Nanay, B. (2016). Imagination and Perception. In Kind, A. (ed.), The Routledge Handbook of Philosophy of Imagination. London, UK; New York, NY: Routledge, 124134.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (2007). Young Minds in Social Worlds. Experience, Meaning, and Memory. Cambridge, MA/London, UK: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pelaprat, E., and Cole, M. (2011). “Minding the Gap”: Imagination, Creativity and Human Cognition. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 45, 397418. doi.org/10.1007/s12124-011-9176-5.Google Scholar
Pendlebury, M. J. (1996). The Role of Imagination in Perception. South African Journal of Philosophy, 15(4), 133138.Google Scholar
Petersen, M. B., and Aarøe, L. (2013). Politics in the Mind’s Eye: Imagination as a Link between Social and Political Cognition. American Political Science Review, 107(2), 275293. doi.org/10.1017/S0003055413000026.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1945/1992). La formation du symbole chez l’enfant: Imitation, jeu et rêve, image et representation. 8th edition. Neuchâtel, Switzerland; Paris, France: Delachaux et Niestlé.Google Scholar
Pile, S. (2000). Sleepwalking in the Modern City: Walter Benjamin and Sigmund Freud in the World of Dreams. In Bridge, G. and Watson, S. (eds.), A Companion to the City. Oxford, UK: Blackwell, 7586.Google Scholar
Ratner, C. (2012). Macro Cultural Psychology. A Political Philosophy of Mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ribot, T. (2007). Essai sur l’imagination créatrice (1900). Paris, France: L’Harmattan.Google Scholar
Rogoff, B. (2003). The Cultural Nature of Human Development. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rosa, A., and Valsiner, J. (eds.) (2018). The Cambridge Handbook of Sociocultural Psychology. 2nd edition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. doi.org/10.1017/9781316662229.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., and Addis, D. R. (2007). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Constructive Memory: Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 773786. doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2007.2087.Google Scholar
Stenner, P. (2018). Liminality and Experience. A Transdisciplinary Approach to the Psychosocial. London, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Tanggaard, L., and Brinkmann, S. (2018). Imagination – Methodological Implications. In Zittoun, T. and Glăveanu, V. P. (eds.), Oxford Handbook of Imagination and Culture. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 87105.Google Scholar
Thomas, N. J. (1999). Are Theories of Imagery Theories of Imagination?: An Active Perception Approach to Conscious Mental Content. Cognitive Science, 23(2), 207245. doi.org/10.1016/S0364-0213(99)00004-X.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C., and Delafield-Butt, J. (2017). Intersubjectivity in the Imagination and Feelings of the Infant: Implications for Education in the Early Years. In White, E. and Dalli, C. (eds.), Under-Three Year Olds in Policy and Practice. London, UK: Springer, 1739.Google Scholar
Triliva, S., Varvantakis, C., and Dafermos, M. (2015). YouTube, Young People, and the Socioeconomic Crises in Greece. Information, Communication & Society, 18(4), 407423. doi.org/10.1080/1369118X.2014.953564.Google Scholar
Tulving, E., and Kim, A. (2007). The Medium and the Message of Mental Time Travel. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(03), 334335. doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X07002208.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J. (1997). Culture and the Development of Children’s Action. 2nd edition. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J.(1998). The Guided Mind: A Sociogenetic Approach to Personality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J.(2012). A Guided Science: History of Psychology in the Mirror of its Making. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Valsiner, J., and van der Veer, R. (2000). The Social Mind: Construction of the Idea. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Vico, G. (1725/1993a). La Science nouvelle. Translated by C. Trivulzio. Paris, France: Gallimard.Google Scholar
Vico, G.(1710/1993b). L’Antique sagesse de l’Italie. Edited by B. Pinchard. Translated by J. Michelet. Paris, France: Flammarion.Google Scholar
Vico, G. (1728/2004). Vie de Giambattista Vico écrite par lui-même. Edited by D. Luglio. Translated by J. Michelet. Paris, France: Allia.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1971). The Psychology of Art. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). The Collected Works of L. S. Vygotsky. Edited by R. W. Rieber and A. S. Carton. Volume 1. New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1931/1994). Imagination and Creativity of the Adolescent. In van der Veer, R. and Valsiner, J. (eds.), The Vygotsky Reader. Original publication 1931. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing, 266288.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1930/2004). Imagination and Creativity in Childhood. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 42(1), 797.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S.(2011). L’imagination et son développement chez l’enfant. In Leçons de psychologie. Paris: La Dispute, 155180.Google Scholar
Wertsch, J. V. (1998). Mind as Action. New York, NY; Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zavereshneva, E., and van der Veer, R. (2018). Vygotsky’s Notebooks. A Selection. New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Zeidman, P., and Maguire, E. A. (2016). Anterior Hippocampus: The Anatomy of Perception, Imagination and Episodic Memory. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 17(3), 173182. doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2015.24.Google Scholar
Zittoun, T. (2006). Transitions. Development Through Symbolic Resources. Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Zittoun, T. (2016a). Studying “Higher Mental Functions”: The Example of Imagination. In Valsiner, J. et al. (eds.), Psychology as a Science of Human Being: The Yokohama Manifesto. Volume 13. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer, 129147.Google Scholar
Zittoun, T. (2016b). The Sound of Music. In Klempe, H. (ed.), Cultural Psychology of Musical Experience. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing, 2139.Google Scholar
Zittoun, T., and Cerchia, F. (2013). Imagination as Expansion of Experience. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 47(3), 305324. doi.org/10.1007/s12124-013-9234-2.Google Scholar
Zittoun, T., and de Saint-Laurent, C. (2015). Life-Creativity: Imagining One’s Life. In Glăveanu, V. P., Gillespie, A., and Valsiner, J. (eds.), Rethinking Creativity: Contributions from Cultural Psychology. Hove, UK; New York, NY: Routledge, 5875.Google Scholar
Zittoun, T., and Gillespie, A. (2014). Sculpture and Art Installations: Towards a Cultural Psychological Analysis. In Wagoner, B., Chaudhary, N., and Hviid, P. (eds.), Cultural Psychology and Its Future: Complementarity in a New Key. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing, 167177.Google Scholar
Zittoun, T., and Gillespie, A. (2015). Integrating Experiences: Body and Mind Moving between Contexts. In Wagoner, B., Chaudhary, N., and Hviid, P. (eds.), Integrating Experiences: Body and Mind Moving between Contexts. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing, 349.Google Scholar
Zittoun, T., and Gillespie, A. (2016). Imagination in Human and Cultural Development. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Zittoun, T., and Gillespie, A. (2018). Imagining the Collective Future: A Sociocultural Perspective. In de Saint-Laurent, C., Obradovic, S., and Carrière, K. (eds.), Imagining Collective Futures: Perspectives from Social, Cultural and Political Psychology. London, UK: Palgrave, 1537.Google Scholar
Zittoun, T., and Glăveanu, V. P. (2018). Imagination at the Frontiers of Cultural Psychology. In Zittoun, T. and Glăveanu, V. P. (eds.), Handbook of Imagination and Culture. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 115.Google Scholar
Zittoun, T., and Rosenstein, A. (2018). Theatre and Imagination to (Re)Discover Reality. In Zittoun, T. and Glăveanu, V. P. (eds.), Handbook of Imagination and Culture. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 223242.Google Scholar
Zittoun, T., and Sato, T. (2018). Imagination in Adults and the Aging Person: Possible Futures and Actual Past. In Zittoun, T. and Glăveanu, V. P. (eds.), Handbook of Imagination and Culture. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 187208.Google Scholar
Zittoun, T., and Valsiner, J. (2016). Imagining the Past and Remembering the Future: How the Unreal Defines the Real. In Sato, T., Mori, N., and Valsiner, J. (eds.), Making of the Future: The Trajectory Equifinality Approach in Cultural Psychology. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing, 319.Google Scholar

References

de la Re, A., Abad, F., Camahort, E., and Juan, M. C. (2009). Tools for Procedural Generation of Plants in Virtual Scenes. In International Conference on Computational Science. Berlin, Germany: Springer, 801810.Google Scholar
Aitken, M., Butler, G., Lemmon, D., Saindon, E., Peters, D., and Williams, G. (2004). The Lord of the Rings: The Visual Effects that Brought Middle Earth to the Screen. Proceedings of the Conference on SIGGRAPH 2004 Course Notes – GRAPH ’04, 11-es. doi.org/10.1145/1103900.1103911.Google Scholar
Bernardes, G., and Cocharro, D. (2019). Dynamic Music Generation, Audio Analysis-Synthesis Methods. In Lee, N (ed.), Encyclopedia of Computer Graphics and Games, Berlin, Germany: Springer.Google Scholar
Boden, M. A., and Edmonds, E. A. (2009). What is Generative Art? Digital Creativity, 20(1–2), 2146.Google Scholar
Bogart, B. D., Pasquier, P., and Barnes, S. J. (2013). An Integrative Theory of Visual Mentation and Spontaneous Creativity. Proceedings of the 9th ACM Conference on Creativity & Cognition – C&C ’13, 264. doi.org/10.1145/2466627.2466639.Google Scholar
Breault, V., Ouellet, S., Somers, S., and Davies, J. (2013). SOILIE: A Computational Model of 2D Imagination. In West, R and Stewart, T (eds.), Proceedings of the 12th International Conference on Cognitive Modeling, Ottawa, Canada: Carleton University, 95100.Google Scholar
Cohen, H. (1995). The Further Exploits of AARON, Painter. Stanford Humanities Review, 4(2), 141158.Google Scholar
Coyne, B., and Sproat, R. (2001). WordsEye: An Automatic Text-to-Scene Conversion System. In Proceedings of the 28th Annual Conference on Computer Graphics and Interactive Techniques. Sydney, Australia: Association for Computing Machinery, 487496.Google Scholar
Davies, J., and Francis, A. G. (2013). The Role of Artificial Intelligence Research Methods in Cognitive Science. In West, R and Stewart, T (eds.), Proceedings of the 12th International Conference on Cognitive Modeling, Ottawa, Canada: Carleton University, 439444.Google Scholar
De Mantaras, R. L., and Arcos, J. L. (2002). AI and Music: From Composition to Expressive Performance. AI Magazine, 23(3), 4343.Google Scholar
Ebert, D. S., Musgrave, F. K., Peachey, D., Perline, K., and Worey, S. (2003). Texturing & Modeling: A Procedural Approach. 3rd edition. New York, NY: Morgan Kaufmann.Google Scholar
Eslami, S. A., Rezende, D. J., Besse, F., et al. (2018). Neural Scene Representation and Rendering. Science, 360 (6394), 12041210.Google Scholar
Fisher, M., Ritchie, D., Savva, M., Funkhouser, T., and Hanrahan, P. (2012). Example-Based Synthesis of 3D Object Arrangements. ACM Transactions on Graphics – Proceedings of ACM SIGGRAPH Asia 2012. 31(6), article 135.Google Scholar
Gaisbauer, W., and Hlavacs, H. (2017). Procedural Attack! Procedural Generation for Populated Virtual Cities: A Survey. International Journal of Serious Games, 4(2).Google Scholar
Goodfellow, I., Pouget-Abadie, J., Mirza, M., et al. (2014). Generative Adversarial Nets. In Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems. 27, 26722680.Google Scholar
Hammes, J. (2001). Modeling of Ecosystems as a Data Source for Real-Time Terrain Rendering. In Digital Earth Moving. Berlin, Germany: Springer, 98111.Google Scholar
Heath, D., Dennis, A. W., and Ventura, D. (2015). Imagining Imagination: A Computational Framework Using Associative Memory Models and Vector Space Models. In Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Computational Creativity, 244251.Google Scholar
Hendrikx, M., and Meijer, S (2011). Procedural Content Generation for Games: A Survey. ACM Trans.Multimedia Comput. Commun. Appl., 1, February, 124.Google Scholar
Hiller, L. A. Jr., and Isaacson, L. M. (1958). Musical Composition with a High-Speed Digital Computer. Journal of the Audio Engineering Society, 6(3), 154160.Google Scholar
Johnstone, K. (1979). Impro: Improvisation and the Theatre. London, UK: Faber and Faber.Google Scholar
Kato, H., and Harada, T. (2014). Image Reconstruction from Bag-of-Visual-Words. In Proceedings of the IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition. Washington, DC: IEEE Computer Society, 955962.Google Scholar
Kelly, G., and McCabe, H. (2006). A Survey of Procedural Techniques for City Generation. The ITB Journal, 7(2), 5.Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M., and Shwartz, S. P. (1977). A Simulation of Visual Imagery. Cognitive Science 1, 265295.Google Scholar
Liang, X., Zhuo, B., Li, P., and He, L. (2016). CNN Based Texture Synthesize with Semantic Segment. arxiv.org/abs/1605.04731.Google Scholar
McCorduck, P. (1991). Aaron’s Code: Meta-Art, Artificial Intelligence, and the Work of Harold Cohen. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Meuller, P., Wonka, P., Haegler, S., Ulmer, A., and van Gul, L. (2006). Procedural Generation of Buildings. ACM Transactions on Graphics, 25(3), 614623.Google Scholar
Morris, C. (2011). World of Warcraft Maker Turns 20, Looks Ahead. Plugged In. games.yahoo.com/blogs/plugged-in/world-warcraft-maker-turns-20-looks-ahead-450.html.Google Scholar
Parish, Y. I. H., and Mueller, P. (2001). Procedural Modeling of Cities. In Proceedings of ACM SIGGRAPH 2001. New York, NY: ACM Press/ ACM SIGGRAPH, 301308.Google Scholar
Pascanu, R., Li, Y., Vinyals, O., et al. (2017). Learning Model-Based Planning from Scratch. arxiv.org/abs/1707.06170.Google Scholar
Silver, D., Huang, A., Maddison, C. J., et al. (2016). Mastering the Game of Go with Deep Neural Networks and Tree Search. Nature, 529(7587), 484.Google Scholar
Silver, D., Hubert, T., Schrittwieser, J., et al. (2018). A General Reinforcement Learning Algorithm that Masters Chess, Shogi, and Go Through Self-Play. Science, 362(6419), 11401144.Google Scholar
Thorn, B. (2001). BoB: An Improvisational Music Companion. PhD Dissertation, Computer Science Department. Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie Mellon University, 281.Google Scholar
Veale, T. (2017). Déjà Vu All Over Again: On the Creative Value of Familiar Elements in the Telling of Original Tales. In Proceedings of ICCC 2017, the 8th International Conference on Computational Creativity, Atlanta, Georgia.Google Scholar
Weiner, J. (2011). “Where do Dwarf-Eating Carp Come From?” The New York Times Magazine. July 21.Google Scholar
Whitelaw, M. (2004). Metacreation: Art and Artificial Life. London: MIT Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×