Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T06:08:15.409Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

15 - Working Memory and Natural Syntax

from Part III - Linguistic Theories and Frameworks

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 July 2022

John W. Schwieter
Affiliation:
Wilfrid Laurier University
Zhisheng (Edward) Wen
Affiliation:
Hong Kong Shue Yan University
Get access

Summary

Research on working memory and language has followed two quite divergent paths. The first line of inquiry examines questions relating to the components and organization of working memory – whether there are specialized buffers, the nature of the link to long-term memory, and so on. For the most part, studies of this type have little to say about the workings of language per se – why it has the particular types of relative clauses or patterns of verbal agreement that it does, for example. These issues fall under the purview of a different line of research, which seeks to trace various fundamental properties of language to the cognitive processes involved in the storage and manipulation of information – working memory, broadly construed. The goal of the latter research program, which I will try to advance here, is to establish that general properties of working memory, however they are ultimately integrated into a theoretical model, can contribute to a deeper understanding of the human language faculty. I will focus here on three phenomena that help illustrate this point – a restriction on the interpretation of reflexive pronouns, a curious prohibition on phonological contraction in a type of wh question, and a baffling constraint commonly known as the ‘that-trace effect.’ A careful examination of their properties reveals a previously unsuspected finding: they are shaped by the need to minimize processing cost, a key factor in our understanding of working memory as well.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alexopoulou, T., & Keller, F. 2007. Locality, cyclicity and resumption: At the interface between the grammar and the human sentence processor. Language, 83, 110160.Google Scholar
Aoshima, S., Phillips, C., & Weinberg, A. 2002. Active filler effects and reanalysis: A study of Japanese wh-scrambling constructions. University of Maryland Working Papers in Linguistics, 12, 124.Google Scholar
Archangeli, D., & Pulleyblank, D. 2015. Phonology without universal grammar. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, article 1229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bresnan, J. 1977. Variables in the theory of transformations. In Culicover, P., Wasow, T. & Akmajian, A. (Eds.), Formal syntax (pp. 157196). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bruening, B. 2006. Differences between the wh-scope-marking and wh-copy constructions in Passamaquoddy. Linguistic Inquiry, 37, 2549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caplan, D., & Waters, G. 2013. Memory mechanisms supporting syntactic computation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 243268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, P., Miake, A., & Just, M. 1994. Working memory constraints in comprehension: Evidence from individual differences, aphasia, and aging. In Gernsbacher, M. (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 10751122). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1956. Three models for the description of language. Institute of Radio Engineers Transactions on Information Theory2(3), 113124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1980. Rules and representations. Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1986. Barriers. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N., & Miller, G. 1963. Introduction to the formal analysis of natural languages. In Luce, R., Bush, R., & Galanter, E. (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 269321). Wiley.Google Scholar
Cinque, G. 2020. The syntax of relative clauses: A unified analysis. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Clifton, C., & Frazier, L. 1989. Comprehending sentences with long-distance dependencies. In Carlson, G. & Tanenhaus, M. (Eds.), Linguistic structure in language processing (pp. 273317). Kluwer.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. 2015. George Miller’s magical number of immediate memory in retrospect: Observations on the faltering progress of science. Psychological Review, 122, 536541.Google Scholar
Crain, S., Goro, T., & Thornton, R. 2006. Language acquisition is language change. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 35, 3149.Google Scholar
Culicover, P. 1993. Evidence against ECP accounts of the that-t effect. Linguistic Inquiry, 24, 557561.Google Scholar
Dyakonova, M. 2009. A phase-based approach to Russian free word order. LOT.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. 1985. The mind’s new science: A history of the cognitive revolution. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. 2008. Working memory. In Roediger, H. (Ed.), Cognitive psychology of memory (pp. 3352). Elsevier.Google Scholar
Getz, H. 2019. Acquiring wanna: Beyond universal grammar. Language Acquisition, 26, 119143.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. 1998. Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68, 176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodall, G. 2004. On the syntax and processing of wh-questions in Spanish. In Chand, V., Kelleher, A., Rodrígues, A., & Schmeiser, B. (Eds.), Proceedings of the West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (pp. 101114). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Hawkins, J. 2004. Efficiency and complexity in grammars. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, J. 2014. Cross-linguistic variation and efficiency. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hofmeister, P., & Sag, I. 2010. Cognitive constraints and island effects. Language, 86, 366415.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. 2007. A parallel architecture perspective on language processing. Brain Research, 1146, 222.Google Scholar
Jaeggli, O. 1980. Remarks on to-contraction. Linguistic Inquiry, 11, 239245.Google Scholar
Jespersen, O. 1933. Essentials of English grammar. Allen and UnwinGoogle Scholar
Kayne, R. 1994. The antisymmetry of syntax. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kluender, R. 1998. On the distinction between strong and weak islands: A processing perspective. In Culicover, P. & McNally, L. (Eds.), The limits of syntax (Syntax and Semantics 29), 241279. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kluender, R., & Kutas, M. 1993. Subjacency as a processing phenomenon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 573633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, R., Vasishth, S., & Van Dyke, J. 2006. Computational principles of working memory in sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 447454.Google Scholar
Lutken, C. J., Legendre, G., & Omaki, A. 2020. Syntactic creativity errors in children’s wh-questions. Cognitive Science, 44(7), e12849.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. 2015. Introduction. In MacWhinney, B. & O’Grady, W. (Eds.), The handbook of language emergence (pp. 131). Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
McDaniel, D. 1989. Partial and multiple wh-movement. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 7, 565604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDaniel, D., Chiu, B., & Maxfield, T. 1995. Parameters for wh-movement types: Evidence from child English. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 13, 709753.Google Scholar
Miller, G. 1956. Human memory and the storage of information. Transactions on Information Theory, 2(3), 129137.Google Scholar
Miller, G., & Chomsky, N. 1963. Finitary models of language users. In Luce, R., Bush, R., & Galanter, E. (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology (Vol. 2, 419491. Wiley.Google Scholar
O’Grady, W. 2005. Syntactic carpentry: An emergentist approach to syntax. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
O’Grady, W. 2015. Anaphora and the case for emergentism. In MacWhinney, B. & O’Grady, W. (Eds.), The handbook of language emergence, 100122. Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
O’Grady, W. 2021. Natural syntax: An emergentist primer. http://ling.hawaii.edu/william-ogrady/ and researchgate.netGoogle Scholar
Pearl, L., & Sprouse, J. 2013Syntactic islands and learning biases: Combining experimental syntax and computational modeling to investigate the language acquisition problemLanguage Acquisition20, 2368.Google Scholar
Perlmutter, D. 1968. Deep and surface constraints in syntax (Doctoral dissertation, Department of Linguistics, MIT).Google Scholar
Phillips, C. 2013. On the nature of island constraints I: Language processing and reductionist accounts. In Sprouse, J. & Hornstein, N. (Eds.), Experimental syntax and island effects (pp. 64108). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Phillips, C., Kazanina, N., & Abada, S. 2005. ERP effects of the processing of syntactic long-distance dependencies. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 407428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pickering, M. 2000. No evidence for traces in sentence comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 4748.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. 1982. Issues in Italian syntax. Foris.Google Scholar
Schwering, S. & MacDonald, M. 2020. Verbal working memory as emergent from language comprehension and production. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, Article 68. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00068Google Scholar
Sprouse, J., Wagers, M., & Phillips, C. 2012. A test of the relation between working memory capacity and syntactic island effects. Language, 88, 82123.Google Scholar
Thornton, R. 1990. Adventures in long-distance moving: The acquisition of complex wh-questions (Doctoral dissertation, University of Connecticut).Google Scholar
Traxler, M., & Pickering, M. 1996. Plausibility and the processing of unbounded dependencies: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 454475.Google Scholar
Wagers, M., & Phillips, C. 2009. Multiple dependencies and the role of the grammar in real-time comprehension. Journal of Linguistics, 45, 395433.Google Scholar
Warren, P., Speer, S., & Schafer, A. 2003. Wanna-contraction and prosodic disambiguation in US and NZ EnglishWellington Working Papers in Linguistics15, 3149.Google Scholar
Yngve, V. 1960. A model and an hypothesis for language structure. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 104, 444466.Google Scholar
Yngve, V. 1998. Clues from the Depth Hypothesis: A reply to Geoffrey Sampsons’ review. Computational Linguistics, 24, 633640.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×