Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-11T14:39:56.624Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Environmental Effects on Beliefs

from Part III - Variation in Beliefs

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 November 2022

Julien Musolino
Affiliation:
Rutgers University, New Jersey
Joseph Sommer
Affiliation:
Rutgers University, New Jersey
Pernille Hemmer
Affiliation:
Rutgers University, New Jersey
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
The Cognitive Science of Belief
A Multidisciplinary Approach
, pp. 513 - 554
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

American Anthropological Association. (1947) Statement on human rights. American Anthropologist, 49(4), 540541.Google Scholar
Anthony, D. (2019) The world conference on human rights: still a guiding light a quarter of a century later. Australian Journal of Human Rights, 23(13), 411427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atran, S. (2002) In gods we trust. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1997) Mindblindness: an essay on autism and theory of mind. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C. (2005) Enzymatic computation and cognitive modularity. Mind and Language, 20(3), 259287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, H. C. (2020) Towards a cognitive science of the human: cross-cultural approaches and their urgency. Trends in Cognitive Science, 24(8), 620638.Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C., & Kurzban, R. (2006) Modularity in cognition: framing the debate. Psychological Review, 113(3), 628647.Google Scholar
Bendaña, J. & Mandelbaum, E. (2021) The fragmentation of belief. In Kindermann, D., Borgoni, C., & Onofri, A. (Eds.). The fragmentation of mind. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bettinger-Lopez, C. (2018) The long arc of human rights: a case for optimism. Foreign Affairs, 97 3, 186–190 (April 17, 2018). May/June 2018, University of Miami Legal Studies Research Paper No. 18-18, Available at SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3164566Google Scholar
Blackburn, S. (2005) The Oxford dictionary of philosophy. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Buskell, A. (2017) What are cultural attractors? Biology and Philosophy, 32(3), 118.Google Scholar
Carchidi, V. J. (2020) The nature of morals: how universal moral grammar provides the conceptual basis for the Universal Declaration of Human Rights. Human Rights Review, 21(1), 6592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, K. (2009) Fictions of justice: the international criminal court and the challenge of legal pluralism in Sub-Saharan Africa. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connors, M. H. & Halligan, P.W. (2015) A cognitive account of belief: a tentative road map. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(1588), 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (1992) Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In Barkow, J., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (Eds.). The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19136). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Council on Foreign Relations. (2009) U.S. opinion on human rights. www.cfr.org/backgrounder/us-opinion-human-rightsGoogle Scholar
Council on Foreign Relations. (2011) Public opinion on global issues. www.cfr.org/thinktank/iigg/pop/Google Scholar
Council on Foreign Relations (2012) The global human rights regime. www.cfr.org/report/global-human-rights-regimeGoogle Scholar
Council on Foreign Relations (2021) Annual report from the council on foreign relations. https://www.cfr.org/annual-report-2021Google Scholar
D’Andrade, R. (1995) The development of cognitive anthropology. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delaet, D. L. (2015) The global struggle for human rights: universal principles in world politics. Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
Dembour, M. B. (2010) What are human rights? Four schools of thought. Human Rights Quarterly, 32(1), 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doyal, L. (2001) The moral foundations of the clinical duties of care: Needs, duties and human rights. Bioethics, 15(5–6), 520535.Google Scholar
Dwyer, S. (2006) How good is the linguistic analogy? In Carruthers, P., Laurence, S., & Titch, S. (Eds.). The innate mind: culture and cognition (pp. 237256). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fariss, C. J. (2019) Yes, human rights practices are improving over time. American Political Science Review, 113(3), 868881.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, J. (2017) Inquiry and belief. Noûs, 53(2), 296315.Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (1985) The uses of diversity. In Borofsky, R. (Ed.), Assessing Cultural Anthropology (pp. 454465). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Gragl, P. & Fitzmaurice, M. (2019) The legal character of Article 18 of the Vienna Convention on the law of treaties. International and Comparative Law Quarterly, 68(3), 699717.Google Scholar
Gready, P. & Robins, S. (2020) Transitional justice and theories of change: towards evaluation as understanding. International Journal of Transitional Justice, 14(2), 280299.Google Scholar
Gu, X., Lohrenz, T., Salas, R., et al. (2015) Belief about nicotine selectively modulates value and reward prediction error signals in smokers. PNAS, 112(8), 25392544.Google Scholar
Holbraad, M. (2012) Truth in motion: the recursive anthropology of Cuban divination. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holbraad, M. & Pedersen, M. A. (2017) The ontological turn: an anthropological exposition. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hubálek, M. (2021) A brief (hi)story of just-so stories in evolutionary science. Philosophy of Social Sciences, 51(5), 447468.Google Scholar
Hyafil, A. & Baumard, N. (in press) Evoked and transmitted culture models: Using Bayesian methods to infer the evolution of cultural traits in history.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2011) Thinking, fast and slow. Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Kalyuga, S. (2006) Assessment of learners’ organized knowledge structures in adaptive learning environments. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20(3), 333342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kentridge, R. W. & Heywood, C. A. (2000) Metacognition and awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 9(2), 308312.Google Scholar
Klosko, G. (1993) Rawls’s “political” philosophy and American democracy. American Political Science Review, 87(2), 348359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koinova, M. & Karabegović, D. (2017) Diasporas and transitional justice: transitional activism from local to global levels of engagement. Global Networks, 17(2), 212233.Google Scholar
Leicester, J. (2008) The nature and purpose of belief. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 29(3), 217237.Google Scholar
Lewis, R. (2018) What actually is a belief? And why is it so hard to change? Psychology Today (October 7, 2018). www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/finding-purpose/201810/what-actually-is-belief-and-why-is-it-so-hard-changeGoogle Scholar
López, J. J. (2019) Human rights as political imaginary. Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Malinowski, B. (1961) Argonauts of the Western Pacific. Waveland Press (Original work published in 1922).Google Scholar
Matthias, R. (2015) Handbook of research methods and applications in environmental studies. Edward Elgar Publishing.Google Scholar
McFarland, S. (2011) The slow creation of humanity. Political Psychology, 32(1), 120.Google Scholar
McFarland, S. (2015) Culture, individual differences, and support for human rights: A general review. Peace and Conflict, 21(1), 1027.Google Scholar
McFarland, S., Hackett, J., Hamer, K. et al. (2019) Global human identification and citizenship: a review of psychological studies. Political Psychology, 40(1), 141171.Google Scholar
McKay, R. T. & Dennett, D. C. (2009) The evolution of misbelief. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(6), 493561.Google Scholar
Mercier, H. (2006) Some ideas to study the evolution of mathematics. In Gontier, N., Van Bendegem, J. P., & Aerts, D. (Eds.). Evolutionary Epistemology, Language and Culture (pp. 351377). Springer.Google Scholar
Mikhail, J. (2007) Universal moral grammar: theory, evidence and the future. TRENDS in Cognitive Science, 11(4), 143152.Google Scholar
Mikhail, J. (2011) Elements of moral cognition. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mikhail, J. (2012) Moral grammar and human rights: some reflections on cognitive science and enlightenment rationalism. In Goodman, R., Jinks, D., & Woods, A. K. (Eds.). Understanding social action, promoting human rights (pp. 160202). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Moyn, S. (2014) The Universal Declaration of Human Rights of 1948 in the history of cosmopolitanism. Critical Inquiry, 40(4), 365384.Google Scholar
Moyn, S. (2018) Not enough: human rights in an unequal world. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Nadin, P. (2016) UN Security Council reform. Routledge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Needham, R. (1972) Belief, language, and experience. Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Needham, R. (1975) Polythetic classification: convergence and consequences. Man, 10(3), 349369.Google Scholar
Newman, M. (2019) Transitional justice. Polity Press.Google Scholar
Oyowe, A. (2014) An African conception of human rights? Comments on the challenges of relativism. Human Rights Review, 15(3), 329347.Google Scholar
Petersen, M. B. (2012) Is the political animal politically ignorant? Applying evolutionary psychology to the study of political attitudes. Evolutionary Psychology, 10(5), 802817.Google Scholar
Petersen, M. B. (2015) Evolutionary political psychology: on the origin and structure of heuristics and biases in politics. Advances in Political Psychology, 36(1), 4578.Google Scholar
Porot, N. & Mandelbaum, E. (2021) The science of belief: a progress report WIREs Cognitive Science 12(2), 1–17.Google Scholar
Prati, F. & Loughnan, S. (2017) Imagined intergroup contact promotes support for human rights through increased humanization. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48(1), 5161.Google Scholar
Quilty-Dunn, J. (2020) Rationalization is irrational and self-serving, but useful. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43, e42. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X19002218.Google Scholar
Rawls, J. (1985) Justice as fairness: political not metaphysical. Philosophy & Public Affairs, 14(3), 223251.Google Scholar
Rawls, J. (1987) The idea of an overlapping consensus. Oxford Journal of Legal Studies, 7(1), 125.Google Scholar
Robbins, J. (2007) Continuity thinking and the problem of Christian culture: belief, time, and the anthropology of Christianity. Current Anthropology, 48(1), 517.Google Scholar
Robertson, G. (2012) Crimes against humanity: the struggle for global justice. The New Press.Google Scholar
Schwitzgebel, E. (2006) Belief. Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Zalta, N. (Ed.). (Fall 2019 Edition). https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/belief/#AcaGoogle Scholar
Sen, A. (1997) Human Rights and Asian values. Carnegie Council on Ethics and International Affairs.Google Scholar
Shue, H. (2020) Basic rights: subsistence, affluence, and U.S. foreign policy. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Sibeud, E. (2012) A useless colonial science? Practicing anthropology in the French Colonial Empire, circa 1880–1960. Current Anthropology, 53(S5), S83S94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sikkink, K. (2011) The justice cascade: how human rights prosecutions are changing world politics. W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Sikkink, K. & Kim, H. J. (2013) The justice cascade: the origins and effectiveness of prosecutions of rights violations. Annual Review of Law and Social Science, 9, 269285.Google Scholar
Soysal, N. (1994) Limits of citizenship: migrants and postnational membership in Europe. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1996) Explaining culture: a naturalistic approach. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (2009) Culturally transmitted misbeliefs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(6), 534535.Google Scholar
Stolwijk, S. & Vis, B. (2020) Politicians, the representativeness heuristic and decision-making biases. Political Behavior, 43(1), 14111432.Google Scholar
Streeter, J. (2020) Should we worry about belief? Anthropological Theory, 20(2), 133156.Google Scholar
Stremlau, N. (2019) Developing bottom-up indicators for human rights. The International Journal of Human Rights, 23(8), 13781394.Google Scholar
Téson, F. (1992) International human rights and cultural relativism. In Claude, P. & Weston, B. H. (Eds.). Human rights in the world community. University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Thagard, P. (2000) Coherence in thought and action. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (1992) Psychological foundations of culture. In Barkow, J., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (Eds.). The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19136). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Torney-Putra, J. & Barber, C. (2011) Fostering young people’s support for participatory human rights through their developmental niches. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 81(4), 473481.Google Scholar
United Nations. (2000) Principles Relating to the Status of National Institutions (The Paris Principles). www.un.org/ruleoflaw/files/PRINCI~5.PDFGoogle Scholar
Van Leeuwen, N. (2017) Two paradigms for religious representation: the physicist and the playground. Cognition, 164(July), 206211.Google Scholar
Vivieros de Castro, E. (2014) Cannibal metaphysics, Skafish, P. (Trans.). Univocal.Google Scholar
Von Hippel, W. & Trivers, R. (2011) The evolution and psychology of self-deception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34(1), 156.Google Scholar
Willard, A. K. & Norenzayan, A. (2013) Cognitive biases explain religious belief, paranormal belief, and belief in life’s purpose. Cognition 129(2), 379391.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (2009) Philosophical Investigations, 4th Ed. (Hacker, P. M., & Schulte, J., Eds.) Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar

References

Aarts, H. & Dijksterhuis, A. (2000). Habits as knowledge structures: automaticity in goal-directed behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(1), 5363. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.53Google Scholar
Abdel Sater, R., Perona, M., Huillery, E., & Chevallier, C. (2020). Behavioural insights to reduce air pollution caused by wood burning. Evidence from a Randomized Controlled Trial in France. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/5BR8YGoogle Scholar
Ajzen, I. (2005). Laws of human behavior: symmetry, compatibility, and attitude-behavior correspondence. In Multivariate Research Strategies, (pp. 3–19). Shaker Verlag.Google Scholar
Alcock, I., White, M. P., Taylor, T. et al. (2017). “Green” on the ground but not in the air: pro-environmental attitudes are related to household behaviours but not discretionary air travel. Global Environmental Change, 42, 136147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2016.11.005Google Scholar
Allcott, H. (2011). Social norms and energy conservation. Journal of Public Economics, 95(9–10), 10821095. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2011.03.003Google Scholar
Armitage, C. J. & Christian, J. (2003). From attitudes to behaviour: basic and applied research on the theory of planned behaviour. Current Psychology, 22(3), 187195.Google Scholar
Attari, S. Z., DeKay, M. L., Davidson, C. I., & Bruin, W. B. de. (2010). Public perceptions of energy consumption and savings. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(37), 1605416059. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1001509107Google Scholar
Babutsidze, Z. & Chai, A. (2018). Look at me saving the planet! The imitation of visible green behavior and its impact on the climate value–action gap. Ecological Economics, 146, 290303.Google Scholar
Baiocchi, G., Minx, J., & Hubacek, K. (2010). The impact of social factors and consumer behavior on carbon dioxide emissions in the United Kingdom: a regression based on input–output and geodemographic consumer segmentation data. Journal of Industrial Ecology, 14(1), 5072.Google Scholar
Bamberg, S. (2003). How does environmental concern influence specific environmentally related behaviors? A new answer to an old question. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23(1), 2132.Google Scholar
Bamberg, S. & Möser, G. (2007). Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: a new meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behaviour. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 27(1), 1425. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2006.12.002Google Scholar
Barclay, P. (2012). Harnessing the power of reputation: strengths and limits for promoting cooperative behaviors. Evolutionary Psychology, 10(5), 868?883. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491201000509Google Scholar
Barclay, P. & Barker, J. L. (2020). Greener than thou: people who protect the environment are more cooperative, compete to be environmental, and benefit from reputation. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 72, 114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101441Google Scholar
Barr, S. (2004). Are we all environmentalists now? Rhetoric and reality in environmental action. Geoforum, 35(2), 231249.Google Scholar
Barr, S. (2006). Environmental action in the home: investigating the “value–action” gap. Geography, 91(1), 4354.Google Scholar
Baumard, N. (2016). The origins of fairness: how evolution explains our moral nature. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bellet, C. (2017). the paradox of the Joneses superstar houses and mortgage frenzy in suburban America. CEP Discussion Paper No. 1462.Google Scholar
Bertoldo, R., & Castro, P. (2019). From legal to normative: a combined social representations and sociocognitive approach to diagnosing cultural change triggered by new environmental laws. Culture & Psychology, 25(3), 324344.Google Scholar
Blake, J. (1999). Overcoming the “value–action gap” in environmental policy: Tensions between national policy and local experience. Local Environment, 4(3), 257278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blankenberg, A.-K., & Alhusen, H. (2019). On the determinants of pro-environmental behavior: a literature review and guide for the empirical economist. Center for European, Governance, and Economic Development Research (CEGE), Number 350. http://dx.doi/10.2139/ssrn.3473702Google Scholar
Boon-Falleur, M., Grandin, A., Baumard, N., & Chevallier, C. (2022). Leveraging social cognition to promote effective climate change mitigation. Nature Climate Change, 12(4), 332338. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41558-022-01312-wGoogle Scholar
Brent, D. A., Lott, C., Taylor, M. et al. (2017). Are normative appeals moral taxes? Evidence from a field experiment on water conservation. Louisiana State Department of Economics Working Papers Series.Google Scholar
Büchs, M., Bahaj, A. S., Blunden, L. et al. (2018). Promoting low carbon behaviours through personalised information? Long-term evaluation of a carbon calculator interview. Energy Policy, 120, 284293.Google Scholar
Camilleri, A. R., Larrick, R. P., Hossain, S., & Patino-Echeverri, D. (2019). Consumers underestimate the emissions associated with food but are aided by labels. Nature Climate Change, 9(1), 5358. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41558–018-0354-zGoogle Scholar
Chai, A., Bradley, G., Lo, A., & Reser, J. (2015). What time to adapt? The role of discretionary time in sustaining the climate change value–action gap. Ecological Economics, 116, 95107.Google Scholar
Chen, M.-F. (2015). An examination of the value-belief-norm theory model in predicting pro-environmental behaviour in Taiwan. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 18(2), 145151. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajsp.12096Google Scholar
Chin, Y. S. J., Pretto, L. D., Thuppil, V., & Ashfold, M. J. (2019). Public awareness and support for environmental protection – A focus on air pollution in peninsular Malaysia. PLoS ONE, 14(3), 121. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0212206Google Scholar
Cohen, S. A., & Higham, J. E. (2011). Eyes wide shut? UK consumer perceptions on aviation climate impacts and travel decisions to New Zealand. Current Issues in Tourism, 14(4), 323335.Google Scholar
Conner, M., McEachan, R., Jackson, C., McMillan, B., Woolridge, M., & Lawton, R. (2013). Moderating effect of socioeconomic status on the relationship between health cognitions and behaviors. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 46(1), 1930. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160–013-9481-yGoogle Scholar
Courtenay-Hall, P., & Rogers, L. (2002). Gaps in mind: problems in environmental knowledge-behaviour modelling research. Environmental Education Research, 8(3), 283297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, A. R., & Jaccard, J. J. (1979). Variables that moderate the attitude–behavior relation: results of a longitudinal survey. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(8), 13641376.Google Scholar
Debove, S., André, J.-B., & Baumard, N. (2015). Partner choice creates fairness in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282(1808), 17. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.0392Google Scholar
de Gavelle, E., Davidenko, O., Fouillet, H. et al. (2019). Self-declared attitudes and beliefs regarding protein sources are a good prediction of the degree of transition to a low-meat diet in France. Appetite, 142, 19.Google Scholar
Delgado, M. S., Harriger, J. L., & Khanna, N. (2015). The value of environmental status signaling. Ecological Economics, 111, 111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolecon.2014.12.021Google Scholar
Diekmann, A. & Preisendörfer, P. (2003). Green and greenback: the behavioral effects of environmental attitudes in low-cost and high-cost situations. Rationality and Society, 15(4), 441472.Google Scholar
Dietz, T., Stern, P. C., & Guagnano, G. A. (1998). Social structural and social psychological bases of environmental concern. Environment and Behavior, 30(4), 450471.Google Scholar
Douenne, T., & Fabre, A. (2019). Can we reconcile French people with the carbon tax? Disentangling beliefs from preferences. Working Papers 2019.10, FAERE – French Association of Environmental and Resource Economists.Google Scholar
Eom, K., Kim, H. S., & Sherman, D. K. (2018). Social class, control, and action: socioeconomic status differences in antecedents of support for pro-environmental action. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 77, 6075. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2018.03.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farjam, M., Nikolaychuk, O., & Bravo, G. (2019). Experimental evidence of an environmental attitude–behavior gap in high-cost situations. Ecological Economics, 166, 16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fishbein, M. & Ajzen, I. (1977). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: an introduction to theory and research. Philosophy and Rhetoric, 10(2), 130132.Google Scholar
Gallup. (2008). Awareness, opinions about global warming vary worldwide. https://news.gallup.com/poll/117772/Awareness-Opinions-Global-Warming-Vary-Worldwide.aspxGoogle Scholar
Gelman, A. (2015). What’s the most important thing in statistics that’s not in the textbooks? Statistical Modeling, Causal Inference, and Social Science. https://statmodeling.stat.columbia.edu/2015/04/28/whats-important-thing-statistics-thats-not-textbooks/Google Scholar
Gifford, R. (2011). The dragons of inaction: psychological barriers that limit climate change mitigation and adaptation. American Psychologist, 66(4), 290.Google Scholar
Gifford, R., & Nilsson, A. (2014). Personal and social factors that influence pro-environmental concern and behaviour: a review. International Journal of Psychology, 49(3), 141157. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12034Google Scholar
Goldstein, N. J., Cialdini, R. B., & Griskevicius, V. (2008). A room with a viewpoint: using social norms to motivate environmental conservation in hotels. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(3), 472482. https://doi.org/10.1086/586910Google Scholar
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., & Van den Bergh, B. (2010). Going green to be seen: status, reputation, and conspicuous conservation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(3), 392404. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017346CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gu, D., Jiang, J., Zhang, Y., Sun, Y., Jiang, W., & Du, X. (2020). Concern for the future and saving the earth: when does ecological resource scarcity promote pro-environmental behavior? Journal of Environmental Psychology, 72, 112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101501Google Scholar
Haferkamp, A., Fetchenhauer, D., Belschak, F., & Enste, D. (2009). Efficiency versus fairness: the evaluation of labor market policies by economists and laypeople. Journal of Economic Psychology, 30(4), 527539. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2009.03.010Google Scholar
Hammami, M. B. A., Mohammed, E. Q., Hashem, A. M. et al. (2017). Survey on awareness and attitudes of secondary school students regarding plastic pollution: implications for environmental education and public health in Sharjah city, UAE. Environmental Science and Pollution Research, 24(25), 2062620633.Google Scholar
Hansmann, R., Baur, I., & Binder, C. R. (2020). Increasing organic food consumption: an integrating model of drivers and barriers. Journal of Cleaner Production, 275, 118.Google Scholar
Hennighausen, C., Hudders, L., Lange, B. P., & Fink, H. (2016). What If the rival drives a Porsche? Luxury car spending as a costly signal in male intrasexual competition. Evolutionary Psychology, 14(4), 113. https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704916678217Google Scholar
Hertwig, R., & Engel, C. (2016). Homo ignorans: deliberately choosing not to know. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(3), 359372.Google Scholar
Hines, J. M., Hungerford, H. R., & Tomera, A. N. (1987). Analysis and Synthesis of Research on Responsible Environmental Behavior: a Meta-Analysis. The Journal of Environmental Education, 18(2), 18. https://doi.org/10.1080/00958964.1987.9943482Google Scholar
Hope, A. L., Jones, C. R., Webb, T. L., Watson, M. T., & Kaklamanou, D. (2018). The role of compensatory beliefs in rationalizing environmentally detrimental behaviors. Environment and Behavior, 50(4), 401425.Google Scholar
Huber, R. A., Wicki, M. L., & Bernauer, T. (2020). Public support for environmental policy depends on beliefs concerning effectiveness, intrusiveness, and fairness. Environmental Politics, 29(4), 649673. https://doi.org/10.1080/09644016.2019.1629171Google Scholar
Hurth, V. (2010). Creating sustainable identities: the significance of the financially affluent self. Sustainable Development, 18(3), 123134.Google Scholar
Johnstone, M.-L. & Tan, L. P. (2015). Exploring the gap between consumers’ green rhetoric and purchasing behaviour. Journal of Business Ethics, 132(2), 311328.Google Scholar
Kennedy, E. H., Beckley, T. M., McFarlane, B. L., & Nadeau, S. (2009). Why we don’t “walk the talk”: understanding the environmental values/behaviour gap in Canada. Human Ecology Review, 16(2), 151160.Google Scholar
Kennedy, E. H., & Givens, J. E. (2019). Eco-habitus or eco-powerlessness? examining environmental concern across social class. Sociological Perspectives, 62(5), 646667. https://doi.org/10.1177/0731121419836966CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klöckner, C. A. (2013). A comprehensive model of the psychology of environmental behaviour – a meta-analysis. Global Environmental Change, 23(5), 10281038. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2013.05.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kollmuss, A. & Agyeman, J. (2002). Mind the gap: why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environmental Education Research, 8(3), 239260. https://doi.org/10.1080/13504620220145401Google Scholar
Kormos, C., Gifford, R., & Brown, E. (2014). The influence of descriptive social norm information on sustainable transportation behavior: a field experiment. Environment and Behavior, 47(5), 479501.Google Scholar
Kraus, S. J. (1995). Attitudes and the prediction of behavior: a meta-analysis of the empirical literature. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(1), 5875. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167295211007Google Scholar
Krosnick, J. A., Holbrook, A. L., Lowe, L., & Visser, P. S. (2006). The origins and consequences of democratic citizens’ policy agendas: a study of popular concern about global warming. Climatic Change, 77(1), 743. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584–006-9068-8Google Scholar
LaPiere, R. T. (1934). Attitudes vs. actions. Social Forces, 13(2), 230237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lea, E. & Worsley, T. (2005). Australians’ organic food beliefs, demographics and values. British Food Journal, 107(11), 855869.Google Scholar
Longhi, S. (2013). Individual pro-environmental behaviour in the household context. ISER Working Paper Series.Google Scholar
Lynn, P. (2014). Distinguishing dimensions of pro-environmental behaviour. ISER Working Paper Series.Google Scholar
Maione, M., Mocca, E., Eisfeld, K., Kazepov, Y., & Fuzzi, S. (2020). Public perception of air pollution sources across Europe. Ambio, 50(6), 11501158.Google Scholar
Maiteny, P. T. (2002). Mind in the Gap: summary of research exploring “inner” influences on pro-sustainability learning and behaviour. Environmental Education Research, 8(3), 299306.Google Scholar
McCright, A. M., Dunlap, R. E., & Xiao, C. (2013). Perceived scientific agreement and support for government action on climate change in the USA. Climatic Change, 119(2), 511518. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584–013-0704-9Google Scholar
Milfont, T. L. (2009). The effects of social desirability on self-reported environmental attitudes and ecological behaviour. The Environmentalist, 29(3), 263269.Google Scholar
Milfont, T. L. & Duckitt, J. (2010). The environmental attitudes inventory: a valid and reliable measure to assess the structure of environmental attitudes. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 30(1), 8094. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.09.001Google Scholar
Nolan, J. M., Schultz, P. W., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N. J., & Griskevicius, V. (2008). Normative social influence is underdetected. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(7), 913923.Google Scholar
Palminteri, S. & Chevallier, C. (2018). Can we infer inter-individual differences in risk-taking from behavioral tasks? Frontiers in Psychology, 9(2307), 15. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02307Google Scholar
Pew Research Center. (2019). A look at how people around the world view climate change. Pew Research Center. www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2019/04/18/a-look-at-how-people-around-the-world-view-climate-change/Google Scholar
Rabinovich, A., Morton, T., & Postmes, T. (2009). Time perspective and attitude-behaviour consistency in future-oriented behaviours. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 49(Pt 1), 6989.Google Scholar
Ramakrishnan, A., Kalkuhl, M., Ahmad, S., & Creutzig, F. (2020). Keeping up with the Patels: conspicuous consumption drives the adoption of cars and appliances in India. Energy Research & Social Science, 70, 112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erss.2020.101742Google Scholar
Redondo, I. & Puelles, M. (2017). The connection between environmental attitude–behavior gap and other individual inconsistencies: a call for strengthening self-control. International Research in Geographical and Environmental Education, 26(2), 107120. https://doi.org/10.1080/10382046.2016.1235361Google Scholar
Schultz, P. W. (1999). Changing behavior with normative feedback interventions: a field experiment on curbside recycling. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21(1), 2536. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324834basp2101_3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultz, P. W., & Oskamp, S. (1996). Effort as a moderator of the attitude-behavior relationship: general environmental concern and recycling. Social Psychology Quarterly, 59(4), 375383.Google Scholar
Shepherd, S. & Kay, A. C. (2012). On the perpetuation of ignorance: system dependence, system justification, and the motivated avoidance of sociopolitical information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(2), 264280. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026272Google Scholar
Sommer, S., Mattauch, L., & Pahle, M. (2020). Supporting carbon taxes: the role of fairness. Ruhr Economic Papers.Google Scholar
Stern, P. C. (2000). New environmental theories: toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior. Journal of Social Issues, 56(3), 407424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tam, K.-P. & Chan, H.-W. (2017). Environmental concern has a weaker association with pro-environmental behavior in some societies than others: a cross-cultural psychology perspective. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 53, 213223.Google Scholar
Tam, K.-P. & Chan, H.-W. (2018). Generalized trust narrows the gap between environmental concern and pro-environmental behavior: multilevel evidence. Global Environmental Change, 48, 182194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2017.12.001Google Scholar
Tarrant, M. A. & Cordell, H. K. (1997). The effect of respondent characteristics on general environmental attitude–behavior correspondence. Environment and Behavior, 29(5), 618637.Google Scholar
Tomás, M., López, L. A., & Monsalve, F. (2020). Carbon footprint, municipality size and rurality in Spain: inequality and carbon taxation. Journal of Cleaner Production, 266, 111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclepro.2020.121798Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1979). Values, attitudes, and interpersonal behavior. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 27, 195259.Google Scholar
van der Linden, S. L., Leiserowitz, A. A., Feinberg, G. D., & Maibach, E. W. (2015). The scientific consensus on climate change as a gateway belief: experimental evidence. PLoS ONE, 10(2), 18.Google Scholar
Vesely, S. & Klöckner, C. A. (2020). Social desirability in environmental psychology research: three meta-analyses. Frontiers in Psychology, 11(1395), 19. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01395Google Scholar
Wicker, A. W. (1969). Attitudes versus actions: the relationship of verbal and overt behavioral responses to attitude objects. Journal of Social Issues, 25(4), 4178.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×