Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T05:32:32.360Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - Understanding of human motion, form and levels of meaning

evidence from the perception of human point-light displays by infants and people with autism

from Part II - The bodies of others

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 October 2011

Virginia Slaughter
Affiliation:
University of Queensland
Celia A. Brownell
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
Get access

Summary

The study of the development of infant responses to the visual representation of the human form in photographs and drawings is clearly important, but equally so is consideration of the role that human motion plays in delivering meanings about the bodies of other people. Much is still to be learned about infants’ abilities to represent the whole human form, and to establish which of the material properties of humans are used and encoded by infants. Moreover, we need to consider the role that specific patterns of motion might play in the formation of these representations. Evidence on these issues has emerged from the study of responses to human point-light displays (PLDs). In this chapter I review evidence on the emergence of abilities to perceive different ‘levels of meaning’ conveyed by human PLDs and then outline how consideration of these levels of meaning can help in understanding potential difficulties that people with autism may have in perceiving and making sense of human bodily motion.

Levels of human bodily motion

Before we proceed we need to clarify the distinction between the terms human motion, biomechanical motion and biological motion. These terms are sometimes used without sufficient consideration. The term biological motion can be considered to cover all forms of motion shown by animals, with the most basic level of motion being that of self-starting irregular, partially contingent motion. Biomechanical motion is a more specific aspect of biological motion that is associated with having articulated moving limbs. This is movement that humans and many, but not all, animals show. While human motion is biological and contains biomechanical movement that overlaps with that of many animals, human motion also has specific properties that are constrained by the human form, with the arms and legs being vertically aligned, and that also convey important conspecific social and psychological meanings. Thus there are levels of subtlety to the patterning of human motion that we may be particularly attuned to. The point is that we should not consider that human motion is simply equivalent to or synonymous with biological or biomechanical motion, and when considering the literature on the development of these abilities we must keep in mind these distinctions.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allison, T.Puce, A.McCarthy, G. 2000 Social perception from visual cues: Role of the STS regionTrends in Cognitive Science 4 267Google Scholar
Annaz, D.Remington, A.Milne, E.Campbell, R.Coleman, M.Swettenham, J. 2010 Development of motion processing in children with autismDevelopmental Science 13 826Google Scholar
Arterberry, M. E.Bornstein, M. H. 2001 Three-month-old categorisation of animals and vehicles based on static and dynamic attributesJournal of Experimental Child Psychology 80 222Google Scholar
Arterberry, M. E.Bornstein, M. H. 2002 Infant perceptual and conceptual categorisation: The roles of static and dynamic stimulus attributesCognition 86 1Google Scholar
Atkinson, A. P. 2009 Impaired recognition of emotions from body movements is associated with elevated motion coherence thresholds in autism spectrum disordersNeuropsychologia 47 3,023Google Scholar
Atkinson, A. P.Dittrich, W. H.Gemmell, A. J.Young, A. W. 2004 Emotion perception from dynamic and static body expressions in point-light and full-light displaysPerception 33 717Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. 2000 The Developing Visual BrainNew YorkOxford University Press
Bertenthal, B. I. 1993 Infants’ perception of biomechanical motions: Intrinsic image and knowledge-based constraintsGranrud, C. E.Visual Perception and Cognition in InfancyMahwah, NJLawrence Erlbaum Associates
Bertenthal, B. I.Pinto, J. 1994 Global processing of biological motionsPsychological Science 5 221Google Scholar
Bertenthal, B. I.Proffitt, D. R.Cutting, J. E. 1984 Infant sensitivity to figural coherence in biomechanical motionsJournal of Experimental Child Psychology 37 213Google Scholar
Bertenthal, B. I.Proffitt, D. R.Kramer, S. J. 1987 Perception of biomechanical motions by infants – implementation of various processing constraintsJournal of Experimental Psychology – Human Perception and Performance 13 577Google Scholar
Bertenthal, B. I.Proffitt, D.Kramer, S.Spetner, N. 1987 Infants’ encoding of kinetic displays varying in relative coherenceDevelopmental Psychology 23 171Google Scholar
Bertenthal, B. I.Proffitt, D. R.Spetner, N. B.Thomas, M. A. 1985 The development of infant sensitivity to biomechanical motionsChild Development 56 531Google Scholar
Blake, R.Shiffrar, M. 2007 Perception of human motionAnnual Review of Psychology 58 47Google Scholar
Blake, R.Turner, L. M.Smoski, M. J.Pozdol, S. L.Stone, W. L. 2003 Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autismPsychological Science 14 151Google Scholar
Bonda, E.Petrides, M.Ostry, D.Evans, A. 1996 Specific involvement of human parietal systems and the amygdala in the perception of biological motionJournal of Neuroscience 16 3,737Google Scholar
Booth, A. E.Pinto, J.Bertenthal, B. I. 2002 Perception of the symmetrical patterning of human gait by infantsDevelopmental Psychology 38 554Google Scholar
Christie, T.Slaughter, V. 2010 Movement facilitates infants’ recognition of the whole human formCognition 114 329Google Scholar
Clarke, T. J.Bradshaw, M. F.Field, D. T.Hampson, S. E.Rose, D. 2005 The perception of emotion from body movement in point-light displays of interpersonal dialoguePerception 34 1,171Google Scholar
Cutting, J. E.Kozlowski, L. T. 1977 Recognising friends by their walk: Gait perception without familiarity cuesBulletin of the Psychonomic Society 9 353Google Scholar
Dakin, S.Frith, U. 2005 Vagaries of visual perception in autismNeuron 48 497Google Scholar
Del Viva, M. M.Igliozzi, R.Tancredi, R.Brizzolara, D. 2006 Spatial and motion integration in children with autismVision Research 46 1,242Google Scholar
Dittrich, W. H.Troscianko, T.Lea, S. E. G.Morgan, D. 1996 Perception of emotion from dynamic point-light displays represented in dancePerception 25 727Google Scholar
Downing, P.Jiang, Y.Shuman, M.Kanwisher, N. 2001 A cortical area selective for visual processing of the human bodyScience 293 23Google Scholar
Durand, K.Lecuyer, R. 2002 Object permanence observed in 4-month-old infants with a 2D displayInfant Behaviour and Development 25 269Google Scholar
Fox, R.McDaniel, C. 1982 The perception of biological motion by human infantsScience 218 486Google Scholar
Freitag, C. M.Konrad, C.Haeberlen, M.Kleser, C.von Gontard, A.Reith, W. 2008 Perception of biological motion in autism spectrum disordersNeuropsychologia 46 1,480Google Scholar
Frykholm, G. 1983 Perceived identity I: Recognition of others by their kinematic patternsUppsala Psychological Reports 351Google Scholar
Frykholm, G. 2005 How the Body Shapes the MindOxford, UKOxford University Press/Clarendon Press
Gergley, G.Bekkering, H.Kiraly, I. 2002 Rational imitation in preverbal infantsNature 413 755Google Scholar
Giese, M. A.Poggio, T. 2003 Neural mechanisms for the recognition of biological movementsNature Reviews Neuroscience 4 179Google Scholar
Grezes, J.Fonlupt, P.Bertenthal, B.Delon-Martin, C.Segebarth, C.Decety, J. 2001 Does perception of biological motion rely on specific brain regions?Neuroimage 13 775Google Scholar
Grossman, E. D.Battelli, L.Pascual-Leone, A. 2005 Repetitive TMS over posterior STS disrupts perception of biological motionVision Research 45 2,847Google Scholar
Hadjikhani, N.Joseph, R. M.Manoach, D. S.Naik, P.Snyder, J.Dominick, K. 2009 Body expressions of emotion do not trigger fear contagion in autism spectrum disorderSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 4 70Google Scholar
Hefter, R. L.Manoach, D. S.Barton, J. J. 2005 Perception of facial expression and facial identity in subjects with social developmental disordersNeurology 65 1,620Google Scholar
Hirai, M.Hiraki, K. 2005 An event-related potentials study of biological motion perception in human infantsCognitive Brain Research 22 301Google Scholar
Hirai, M.Watanabe, S.Honda, Y.Kakigi, R. 2009 Developmental changes in point-light walker processing during childhood and adolescence: An event-related potential studyNeuroscience 161 311Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P. 1991 Methodological issues for experiments on autistic individuals’ perception and understanding of emotionJournal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 32 1,135Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P. 1993 Autism and the Development of MindHove, UKLEA
Hobson, R. P.Ouston, J.Lee, A. 1988 Emotion recognition in autism: Coordinating faces and voicesPsychological Medicine 18 911Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P.Ouston, J.Lee, A. 1989 Recognition of emotion by mentally retarded adolescents and young adultsAmerican Journal of Mental Retardation 93 434Google Scholar
Hubert, B.Wicker, B.Moore, D. G.Monfardini, E.Duverger, H.Da Fonseca, D. 2007 Brief report: Recognition of emotional and non-emotional biological motion in individuals with autistic spectrum disordersJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 37 1,386Google Scholar
Johansson, G. 1973 Visual perception or biological motion and a model for its analysisPerception and Psychophysics 14 201Google Scholar
Johansson, G. 1976 Spatio-temporal differentiation and integration in visual motion perception: An experimental and theoretical analysis of calculus-like functions in visual data processingPsychological Research 38 379Google Scholar
Johnson, S. P.Bremner, J. G.Slater, A. M.Mason, U. C.Foster, K. 2002 Young infants’ perception of unity and form in occlusion displaysJournal of Experimental Child Psychology 81 358Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H.Mareschal, D.Csibra, G. 2001 The functional development and integration of the dorsal and ventral visual pathways: A neurocomputational approachNelson, C. A.Luciana, M.Handbook of Developmental Cognitive NeuroscienceCambridge, MAMIT press
Kozlowski, L. T.Cutting, J. E. 1977 Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light displayPerception and Psychophysics 21 575Google Scholar
Kuhlmeier, V. A.Bloom, P.Wynn, K. 2004 Do 5-month-old infants see humans as material objects?Cognition 94 95Google Scholar
Kuhlmeier, V. A.Troje, N. F.Lee, V. 2010 Young infants detect the direction of biological motion in point-light displaysInfancy 15 83Google Scholar
Maas, J. B.Johansson, G.Jansson, G. 1971 Motion Perception, Parts 1 and 2 (films)BostonHoughton Mifflin
Marshall, P. J.Shipley, T. F. 2009 Event-related potentials to point-light displays of human actions in 5-month-old infantsDevelopmental Neuropsychology 34 368Google Scholar
Milne, E.Swettenham, J.Campbell, R. 2005 Motion perception and autistic spectrum disorder: A reviewCahiers De Psychologie Cognitive (Current Psychology of Cognition) 23 3Google Scholar
Moore, D. G.Goodwin, J. E.George, R.Axelsson, E. L.Braddick, F. M. B. 2007 Infants perceive human point-light displays as solid formsCognition 104 377Google Scholar
Moore, D. G.Hobson, R. P.Anderson, M. 1995 Person perception: Evidence for IQ-independent perceptual processing?Intelligence 20 65Google Scholar
Moore, D. G.Hobson, R. P.Lee, A. 1997 Components of person perception: An investigation with autistic, non-autistic retarded and typically developing children and adolescentsBritish Journal of Developmental Psychology 15 401Google Scholar
Mottron, K. L.Burack, J. A.Iarocci, G.Belleville, S.Enns, J. T. 2003 Locally oriented perception with an intact global processing among adolescents with high-functioning autism: evidence from multiple paradigmsJournal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 44 904Google Scholar
Murphy, P.Brady, N.Fitzgerald, M.Troje, N. F. 2009 No evidence for impaired perception of biological motion in adults with autistic spectrum disordersNeuropsychologia 47 3,225Google Scholar
Opfer, J. 2002 Identifying living and sentient kinds from dynamic information: The case of goal-directed versus aimless autonomous movement in conceptual changeCognition 86 97Google Scholar
Parron, C.Da Fonseca, D.Santos, A.Moore, D. G.Monfardini, E.Deruelle, C. 2008 Recognition of biological motion in children with autistic spectrum disordersAutism 12 261Google Scholar
Pauen, S. 2000 Early differentiation within the animate domain: Are humans something special?Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 75 134Google Scholar
Pavlova, M.Sokolov, A. 2000 Orientation specificity in biological motion perceptionPerception and Psychophysics 62 889Google Scholar
Pavlova, M.Sokolov, A. 2003 Prior knowledge about display inversion in biological motion perceptionPerception 32 937Google Scholar
Pavlova, M.Krageloh-Mann, I.Sokolov, A.Birbaumer, N. 2001 Recognition of point-light biological motion displays by young childrenPerception 30 925Google Scholar
Pavlova, M.Sokolov, A.Birbaumer, N.Krageloh-Mann, I. 2006 Biological motion processing in adolescents with early periventricular brain damageNeuropsychologia 44 586Google Scholar
Pelphrey, K.Adolphs, R.Morris, J. P. 2004 Neuroanatomical substrates of social cognition dysfunction in autismMental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 101 259Google Scholar
Perrett, D. I.Harries, M. H.Benson, P. J.Chitty, A. J.Mistlin, A. J. 1990 Retrieval of structure from rigid biological motion: An analysis of the visual responses of neurones in the Macaque temporal cortexBlake, A.Troscianko, T.AI and the EyeChichester, UKNew Wiley
Philip, R. C. M.Whalley, H. C.Stanfield, A. C.Sprengelmeyer, R.Santos, I. M.Young, A. W. 2010 Deficits in facial, body movement and vocal emotional processing in autism spectrum disordersPsychological Medicine 40 1,919Google Scholar
Pinto, J.Shiffrar, M. 2009 The visual perception of human and animal motion in point-light displaysSocial Neuroscience 4 332Google Scholar
Rakison, D. H.Poulin-Dubois, D. 2001 The developmental origin of the animate–inanimate distinctionPsychological Bulletin 127 209Google Scholar
Reid, V. M.Hoehl, S.Striano, T. 2006 The perception of biological motion by infants: An event-related potential studyNeuroscience Letters 395 211Google Scholar
Reid, V. M.Hoehl, S.Landt, J.Striano, T. 2008 Human infants dissociate structural and dynamic information in biological motion: Evidence from neural systemsSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 3 161Google Scholar
Robel, L.Ennouri, K.Piana, H.Vaivre-Douret, L.Perier, A.Flament, M. F. 2004 Discrimination of face identities and expressions in children with autism: Same or different?European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 13 227Google Scholar
Rondan, C.Deruelle, C. 2004 Face processing in high functioning autistic adults: A look into spatial frequencies and the inversion effectJournal of Cognitive and Behavioral Psychotherapies 4 149Google Scholar
Rondan, C.Deruelle, C. 2005 Developmental trends in visuo-spatial abilities in the autistic pathologyCurrent Psychology of Cognition 23 198Google Scholar
Runeson, S.Frykholm, G. 1983 Kinematic specification of dynamics as an informational basis for person-and-action perception: Expectation, gender recognition, and deceptive intentionJournal of Experimental Psychology General 112 585Google Scholar
Runeson, S.Frykholm, G. 1986 Kinematic specification of gender and gender expressionMcCabe, V.Balzano, G. J.Event Cognition: An Ecological PerspectiveHillsdale, NJ and LondonLawrence Erlbaum Associates
Saxe, R.Tzelnic, T.Carey, S. 2006 Five-month-old infants know humans are solid, like inanimate objectsCognition 101 1Google Scholar
Simion, F.Regolin, L.Bulf, H. 2008 A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn babyProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105 809Google Scholar
Slaughter, V.Heron, M.Sim, S. 2002 Development of preferences for the human body shape in infancyCognition 85 71Google Scholar
Thirkettle, M.Benton, C. P.Scott-Samuel, N. E. 2009 Contributions of form, motion and task to biological motion perceptionJournal of Vision 9 1Google Scholar
Troje, N. F.Westhoff, C. 2006 The inversion effect in biological motion perception: Evidence for a ‘life detector’?Current Biology 16 821Google Scholar
Ungerleider, L. G.Haxby, J. V. 1994 What’ and ‘where’ in the human brainCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 4 157Google Scholar
Vaina, L. M.Lemay, M.Bienfang, D. C.Choi, A. Y.Nakayama, K. 1990 Intact biological motion and structure from motion perception in a patient with impaired motion mechanisms – a case-studyVisual Neuroscience 5 353Google Scholar
Virji-Babul, N.Kerns, K.Zhou, E.Kapur, A.Shiffrar, M. 2006 Perceptual-motor deficits in children with Down Syndrome: Implications for interventionDown Syndrome Research and Practice 10 74Google Scholar
Yoon, J. M. D.Johnson, S. C. 2009 Biological motion displays elicit social behavior in 12-month-oldsChild Development 80 1,069Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×