Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-w79xw Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-22T14:46:01.767Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - The SOV Mystery and Language Evolution

from Part I - The Evolution of Syntax

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 February 2025

Dany Adone
Affiliation:
Universität zu Köln
Astrid Gabel
Affiliation:
Universität zu Köln
Get access

Summary

Givón’s chapter presents an evolutionary hypothesis suggesting that the earliest rigid word order in human language must have been (S)OV. The hypothesis is supported first by synchronic distributional data suggesting that the vast majority of known language families can be easily reconstructed to SOV on purely internal grounds. Unlike the vast majority of VO languages, SOV languages show no reconstructible traces of any prior VO word order. What is more, a non-contact-induced drift from VO to OV has yet to be conclusively documented. The chapter offers a cultural-communicative explanation of why the early evolved word order of human language must have been SOV, as well as why it has been drifting away from that early order ever since, first to free (pragmatically determined) word order, then to V-first (VSO, VOS), and eventually to SVO. Why some languages have never undergone this drift remains an open question, perhaps related to isolation and/or cultural conservatism.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Evolution, Acquisition and Development of Syntax
Insights from Creole Languages and Beyond
, pp. 49 - 73
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aristotle, . De partibus animalium. In Barnes, J. (ed. 1984).Google Scholar
Aristotle, . Historiae animalium. In Barnes, J. (ed. 1984).Google Scholar
Aristotle, . De generationem animalium. In Barnes, J. (ed. 1984).Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1896). A new factor in evolution. American Naturalist, 30, 441451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, J. (ed.). (1984). The Complete Works of Aristotle: The Revised Oxford Translation. Vols. I and II. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E. (1974). Language in context: Studies in the acquisition of pragmatics. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Bates, E. (1976). Language in Context: The Acquisition of Pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E. (1978). The emergence of symbols: Ontogeny and phylogeny. Ms. University of Colorado.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Camioni, L. and Volterra, V.. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merril-Palmer Quarterly, 21(3), 205226.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (1981). Roots of Language. Ann Arbor, MI: Karoma.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (2005). The origin of language in niche construction. Ms. University of Hawaii at Manoa.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1973). One Word at a Time: The Use of Single Word Utterances before Syntax. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. (2002). Cooperative hunting among Tai chimpanzees. Human Nature, 13(1), 2746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C. (2005). Joint cooperative hunting among wild chimpanzees: Taking natural observations seriously. Behavior and Brain Science, 28, 692702.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. and Boesch-Achermann, H.. (2000). The Chimpanzees of Tai Forest: Behavioral Ecology and Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1973). Early Syntactic Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Carter, A. (1974). Communication in the sensory-motor period. Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Cheney, D. and Seyfarth, R.. (1990). How Monkeys See the World. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D. and Seyfarth, R. (2007). Baboon Metaphysics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1968). Language and Mind. New York: Harcourt, Brace and World.Google Scholar
Clark, H. and Clark, E.. (1977). Psychology and Language. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Creider, C. (1975). Thematization and word-order. LSA Winter meeting, London, Ont.: University of Western Ontario (ms.).Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection, or, The preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. London: John Murray.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Waal, F. (1982). Chimpanzee Politic: Power and Sex among the Apes. London: Unwin/Counterpoint.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. and Lanting, F.. (1997). Bonobo: The Forgotten Ape. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dingwall, W. O. (1979). The evolution of human communication systems. In Whitaker, H. and Whitaker, H. A. (eds.), Studies in Neuro-Linguistics, Vol. IV. New York: Academic Press, pp. 195.Google Scholar
Dore, J. (1975). Holophrases, speech acts and language universals. Journal of Child Language, 2(1), 2140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. (1992). Co-evolution of cortex size, group size and language. Brain and Behavior Sciences, 16(4), 681694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1970). Discourse agreement: How children answer questions. In Hays, J (ed.), Cognition and the Development of Language. New York: Wiley & Sons, pp. 79107.Google Scholar
Fernald, R. D. and White, S. A.. (2000). Social control of brain: From behavior to genes. In Gazzaniga, M. (ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences, 2nd ed. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 11931208.Google Scholar
Foley, W. (1976). Comparative syntax in Austronesian. Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Gell-Mann, M. and Ruhlen, M.. (2011). The origin and evolution of word order. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(42), October 18, 1729017295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Givón, T. (1977). The drift from VSO to SVO in Biblical Hebrew: The pragmatics of tense-aspect. In Li, C. N. (ed.), Mechanisms of Syntactic Change. Austin: University of Texas Press, pp. 181254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T. (1979). On Understanding Grammar. New York: Academic Press; rev. ed. (2019). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Givón, T. (1983a). Topic continuity and word-order change in Ute. In Givón, T. (ed.), Topic Continuity in Discourse: A Quantitative Cross-Language Study, TSL No. 3. Amsterdam and New York: John Benjamins, pp. 141214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T. (1983b). Topic Continuity in Discourse: A Quantitative Cross-Language Study, TSL No. 3. Amsterdam and New York: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T. (1988). The pragmatics of word-order: Predictability, importance and attention. In Hammond, M., Moravcsik, E. and Wirth, J. (eds.), Studies in Syntactic Typology, TSL No. 17. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 243284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T. (1989). Mind Code and Context: Essays in Pragmatics. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Givón, T. (1995). Functionalism and Grammar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T. (2001). Syntax: An Introduction. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Givón, T. (2002). Bio-Linguistics: The Santa Barbara Lectures. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T. (2005). Context as Other Mind: The Pragmatics of Sociality, Cognition and Communication. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T. (2009). The Genesis of Syntactic Complexity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T. (2017). The Story of Zero. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodall, J. (1965). Chimpanzees of the Gombe Stream reserve. In DeVore, I. (ed.), Primate Behavior: Field Studies of Monkeys and Apes. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1977). Ontogeny and Phylogeny. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Haeckel, E. (1874). Die Gastraea-Theorie die phylogenetische Klassifikation der Tierreichen und der Homologie der Keimblätter, Jenaische Zeitschrift für Naturwissenschaft, 8, 155.Google Scholar
Hauser, M., Chomsky, N. and Fitch, W. T.. (2002). The faculty of language: What it is, who has it, how did it evolve? Science, 298(5598), 15691579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hrdy, S. B. (2009). Mothers and Others. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hyman, L. (1975). The change from SOV to SVO: Evidence from Niger-Congo. In Li, C. N. (ed.), Word Order and Word Order Change. Austin: University of Texas Press, pp. 113147.Google Scholar
Janda, R. (1976). The language of note-taking as a simplified register. Stanford, CA: Stanford University (ms.).Google Scholar
Jay, P. (1965). Field studies. In Schrier, M. A., Harlow, H. F. and Stolnitz, F. (eds.), Behavior of Non-Human Primates. New York: Academic Press, pp. 525591.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, R. (1989/1999). The Human Career, 2nd ed. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lamarck, J.-B. (1809). Philosophie Geologique, ou Exposition des Considerations Relative al’Histoire Naturelle des Animaux. Paris; English translation by H. (1914), The Zoological Philosophy. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Lamendella, J. (1976). Relations between the ontogeny and phylogeny of language: A neorecapitulationist view. In Harnad, S. R., Stelkis, S. D. and Lancaster, J. (eds.), The Origins and Evolution of Language and Speech. New York: New York Academy of Science, pp. 396412.Google Scholar
Lamendella, J. (ms.). The early growth of language and cognition: A neuro-psychological approach. San Jose, CA: San Jose State University.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1982). Basic syntactic processes. In Kuczaj, S. (ed.), Development, Vol. I: Syntax and Semantics. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 73135.Google Scholar
Mardirussian, G. (1978). The drift from VO to OV in Armenian. Los Angeles, CA: University of California (ms.).Google Scholar
Marlowe, F. W. (2005). Hunter-gatherers and human revolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 14, 5467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marlowe, F. W. (2010). The Hadza Hunter-Gatherers of Tanzania. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1976). Evolution and the Diversity of Life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Menn, L. (1990). Agrammatism in English. In Menn, L. and Obler, L. (eds.), Agrammatic Aphasia. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 117178.Google Scholar
Menn, L. and Obler, L.. (1990). Agrammatic Aphasia (3 vols.). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Ochs-Keenan, E. (1974a). Conversational competence in children. Journal of Child Language, 1(2), 163183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ochs-Keenan, E. (1974b). Again and again: The pragmatics of imitation in child language. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Anthropological Association, Mexico City, November 1974.Google Scholar
Ochs-Keenan, E. (1975a). Making it last: Uses of repetition in children’s discourse. Proceedings of the First Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 279294.Google Scholar
Ochs-Keenan, E. (1975b). Evolving discourse: The next step. Ms. University of Southern California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Ochs-Keenan, E. and Schieffelin, B.. (1976). Topic as a discourse notion: A study of topic in conversation of children and adults. In Li, C. N. (ed.), Subject and Topic. New York: Academic Press, pp. 335384.Google Scholar
Ochs-Keenan, E., Schieffelin, B. and Platt, M.. (1979). Propositions across utterances and speakers. In Ochs-Keenan, E. and Schieffelin, B. (eds.), Developmental Pragmatics. New York: Academic Press, pp. 251268.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1999). The Alex Studies: Cognitive and Communicative of Grey Parrots. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952). The Origins of Intelligence in Children. New York: International Universities Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Popper, K. (1934/1959). The Logic of Scientific Discovery. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Rumbaugh, D. M. and Washburn, D. W.. (2003). Intelligence of Apes and Other Rational Beings. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, S. and Lewin, R.. (1993). Kanzi: The Ape on the Brink of the Human Mind. New York: Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Murphy, S. J., Sevcik, R. A., Brakke, K. E., Williams, S. L. and Rumbaugh, D. L.. (1993). Language Comprehension in Ape and Child (Monographs of the Society of Research in Child Development, 58(34, Serial No. 233).Google Scholar
Schaller, G. B. (1961). The orang-utan in Sarawak. Zoologica, 46(6), 7382.Google Scholar
Schaller, G. B. (1963). The Mountain Gorilla: Ecology and Behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Schaller, G. B. (1965). The behavior of the mountain gorilla. In DeVore, I. (ed.), Primate Behavior: Field Studies of Monkeys and Apes. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, pp. 324367.Google Scholar
Scollon, R. (1974). One child’s language from one to two: The origins of construction. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Hawaii at Manoa.Google Scholar
Scollon, R. (1976). Conversations with a One-Year-Old Child. Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. (1977). Language change in childhood and history. In Macnamara, J. (ed.), Language Learning and Thought. New York: Academic Press, pp. 185214.Google Scholar
Stockwell, R.P. (1977). Motivations for exbraciation in Old English. In Li, C. N. (ed.), Mechanisms of Syntactic Change. Austin: University of Texas Press, pp. 291315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugiyama, Y. (1973). The social structure of wild chimpanzees: A review of field studies. In Michael, R. P. and Crook, J. H. (eds.), Comparative Ecology and Behavior of Primates. New York: Academic Press, pp. 375410.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2009). Why We Cooperate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. and Call, J.. (1997). Primate Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tucker, D. M. and Luu, P.. (2012). Cognition and Neural Development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1968). A preliminary report on expressive movements and communication in the Gombe Stream chimpanzees. In Jay, P. C. (ed.), Primate Studies in Adaptability and Variability. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, pp. 313374.Google Scholar
van Lawick-Goodall, J. and van Lawick, H. (1971). Innocent Killers. Boston: Houghton and Mifflin.Google Scholar
Vennemann, T. (1973). Topic, subject and word-order: From SXV to SVX via TVX. In Anderson, J. (ed.), Proceedings of the 1st International Congress on Historical Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 339376.Google Scholar
Waddington, C. H. (1942). Canalization of development and inheritance of acquired characters. Nature, 150, 563565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waddington, C. H. (1953). Genetic assimilation of an acquired character. Evolution, 7(2), 118126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werner, H. and Caplan, B.. (1963). Symbol Formation. New York: Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
West-Eberhard, M. J. (2004). Evolution and Developmental Plasticity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2000). Referential labeling in Diana monkeys. Animal Behavior, 59(5), 917927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuberbühler, K. (2001). A syntactic rule in forest monkey communication. Animal Behavior, 63(2), 293299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×