Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-11T00:27:38.611Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 4 - Natural pedagogy of social emotions

from Part II - On human development and affective social learning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 August 2019

Daniel Dukes
Affiliation:
Université de Fribourg, Switzerland
Fabrice Clément
Affiliation:
Université de Neuchâtel, Switzerland
Get access

Summary

This chapter proposes that during human evolution emotion-based social learning systems and the capacity for ostensive communication have become integrated to serve affective social learning and cultural knowledge transmission during ontogenetic development. Human infants communicate and learn by emotions as well as about emotions through ostensive communication and natural pedagogy even before acquiring language. We will argue that the human species’ unique cognitive adaptations for ostensive communication and natural pedagogy provide specialized social learning mechanisms for the acquisition of culturally shared representations of social emotion concepts. We will explore how ostensive emotion-reflective interactions through the mechanism of social bio-feedback also play a central role in the development of introspective sensitivity in infants for detecting and representing their own categorical emotion states and establishing their emerging capacity for emotional self-attribution and affective self-control. Based on this account, we will propose an evolutionary-based view of social emotions as special types of dispositional kind concepts that belong to the ontological kind categories humans share with other social agents in their cultural community. This theory implies a view of dispositional essentialism about representing categorical emotion kinds in humans, which will be contrasted with current versions of biological essentialism exemplified by recent approaches to basic emotions such as differential emotions theory.

Type
Chapter
Information
Foundations of Affective Social Learning
Conceptualizing the Social Transmission of Value
, pp. 87 - 114
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Armstrong, D. M. (1968). A materialist theory of mind. New York, NY: Humanities Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2006). Emotions as natural kinds? Perspectives on Psychological Sciences, 1, 2858.Google Scholar
Begus, K., & Southgate, V. (2012). Infant pointing serves an interrogative function. Developmental Science, 15(5), 611617.Google Scholar
Blair, R. J. R. (2003). Facial expressions, their communicatory functions and neuro-cognitive substrates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 358, 561572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R. (2007). The amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex in morality and psychopathy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(9), 387392. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.07.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowlby, J. (1969). Altachment and loss. Vol. 1: Attachment. London, UK: Hogarth Press and the Institute of Psychoanalysis.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1995). The growth of causal understandings of natural kinds. In Sperber, D., Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multi-disciplinary debate (pp. 263291). Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1980). Vocal recognition in free-ranging vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 28, 362367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clutton-Brock, T. H. (2002) Breeding together: Kin selection, reciprocity and mutualism in cooperative animal societies, Science, 296, 6972.Google Scholar
Crivelli, C., & Fridlund, A. J. (2018). Facial displays are tools for social influence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(5), 388399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Csibra, G. (2010). Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind & Language, 25, 141168.Google Scholar
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2006). Social learning and social cognition: The case of pedagogy. In Johnson, M. H. & Munakata, Y. M. (Eds.), Processes change in brain and cognitive development: Attention and performance XXI (pp. 249274). Oxford, UK: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csibra, G., (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 148153.Google Scholar
Csibra, G., (2011). Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 366, 11491157.Google Scholar
Demos, V. (1986). Crying in early infancy: An illustration of the motivational function of affect. In Brazelton, T. B. & Yogman, M. W. (Eds.), Affective development in infancy (pp. 3973). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Dennett, D., & Haugeland, J. C. (1987). Intentionality. In Gregory, R. L. (Ed.), The Oxford companion to the mind (pp. 383386). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dicara, L. V. (1970). Learning in the autonomic nervous system. Scientific American, 222, 3039.Google Scholar
Egyed, K., Király, I., & Gergely, G. (2013). Communicating shared knowledge in infancy. Psychological Science, 24, 13481353.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1992). Facial expressions of emotion: New findings, new questions. Psychological Science, 3(1), 3438.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1974). Detecting deception from body or face. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 288298.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Ellsworth, P. (1972). Emotion in the human face. New York, NY: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes between emotions. Science, 221, 12081210.Google Scholar
Field, T. (1994). The effects of mother’s physical and emotional unavailability on emotion regulation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2–3), 208227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E., & Target, M. (2002). Affect-regulation, mentalization, and the development of the self. New York, NY: Other Press.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., & Target, M. (2007). The parent–infant dyad and the construction of the subjective self. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(3–4), 288328.Google Scholar
Futó, J., Téglás, E., Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2010). Communicative function demonstration induces kind-based artifact representation in preverbal infants. Cognition, 117, 18.Google Scholar
Gelman, S. A. (2003). The essential child. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gergely, G. (2007a). Learning ‘about’ versus learning ‘from’ other minds: Human pedagogy and its implications. In Carruthers, P., Laurence, S., & Stich, S., (Eds.). The innate mind: Foundations and the future (pp. 170198). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gergely, G. (2007b). The social construction of the subjective self: The role of affect-mirroring, markedness, and ostensive communication in self-development. In Mayes, L., Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (Eds.), Developmental science and psychoanalysis (pp. 4588). London, UK: Karnac.Google Scholar
Gergely, G. (2013). Ostensive communication and cultural learning: The natural pedagogy hypothesis. In Terrace, H. S. & Metcalfe, J., (Eds.), Joint attention and agency (pp. 139151). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Király, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415, 755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2005). The social construction of the cultural mind: Imitative learning as a mechanism of human pedagogy. Interaction Studies, 6(3), 463481.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., (2006). Sylvia’s recipe: The role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of cultural knowledge. In Levenson, S. & Enfield, N. (Eds.), Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and human interaction (pp. 229255). Oxford, UK: Berg.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., (2013). Natural pedagogy. In Banaj, M. R. & Gelman, S. A. (Eds.), Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us (pp. 127132). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., Egyed, K., & Király, I. (2007). On pedagogy. Developmental Science, 10(1), 139146.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., & Jacob, P. (2012). Reasoning about instrumental and communicative agency in human infancy. Advances in Child Development and Behaviour, 43, 5994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gergely, G., & Unoka, Z. (2007). The development of the unreflective self. In Bush, F. N. (Ed.), Mentalization: Theoretical considerations, research findings, and clinical implications (pp. 57102). Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., (2008). Attachment, affect-regulation and mentalization: The developmental origins of the representational affective self. In Sharpe, C., Fonagy, P., & Goodyer, I. (Eds.), SociaI cognition and developmental psychology (pp. 305342). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., & Watson, J. S. (1996). The social bio-feedback theory of parental affect mirroring: The development of emotional self-awareness and self-control in infancy. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 77, 731.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., (1999). Early social-emotional development: Contingency perception and the social bio-feedback model. In Rochat, P. (Ed.), Early social cognition (pp. 101I37). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A., & Nazzi, T. (2003). Words, kinds and causal powers: A theory theory perspective on early naming and categorization. In Rakison, D. H. & Oakes, L. M. (Eds.), Early category and concept development: Making sense of the blooming, buzzing confusion (pp. 303329). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In Cole, P. & Morgan, J. (Eds.), Syntax and semantics. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1989). Studies in the way of words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P. (1993). Autism and the development of mind. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
James, W. [1890] (1950). The principles of psychology. New York, NY: Dover.Google Scholar
Jurist, E. (2018). Minding emotions. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kelemen, D., & Carey, S. (2007). The essence of artifacts: Developing the design stance. In Margolis, E. & Lawrence, S. (Eds.), Creations of the mind: Theories of artifacts and their representation (pp. 212230). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Király, I., Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2013). Beyond rational imitation: Learning arbitrary means actions from communicative demonstrations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 116(2), 471486.Google Scholar
Kovács, Á. M., Tauzin, T., Téglás, E., Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2014). Pointing as epistemic request: 12-month-olds point to receive new information. Infancy, 19(6), 543557.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kripke, S. (1972/1980). Naming and necessity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malatesta, C. Z., Culver, C., Tesman, R. J., & Shepard, B. (1989). The development of emotion expression during the first two years of life. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 54, Serial No. 219.Google Scholar
Malatesta, C. Z., & Izard, C. E. (1984). The ontogenesis of human social signals: From biological imperative to symbol utilization. In Fox, N. A. & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The psychobiology of affective development (pp. 161206). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Medin, D. L., & Ortony, A. (1989). Psychological essentialism. In Vosniadou, S. & Ortony, A. (Eds.), Similarity and analogical reasoning (pp. 179195). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mercier, H., & Sperber, D. (2011). Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34, 57111.Google Scholar
Mercier, H., (2017). The enigma of reason. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Miller, N. E. (1969). Learning visceral and glandular responses. Science, 163. 434445.Google Scholar
Miller, N. E. (1978). Bio-feedback and visceral learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 29, 373404.Google Scholar
Millikan, R. G. (1984). Language, thought and other biological categories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Millikan, R. G. (1993). White queen psychology and other essays for Alice. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mineka, S., & Cook, M. (1993). Mechanisms involved in the observational conditioning of fear. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 2338.Google Scholar
Neander, K. (1995). Misrepresenting and malfunctioning. Philosophical Studies, 79, 109141.Google Scholar
Nelson, N. L., & Russell, J. A. (2013) Universality revisited. Emotion Review, 5, 815.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., & Cohen, D. (1996). Culture of honor: The psychology of violence in the South. Boulder, CO: Westview.Google Scholar
Prinz, J. (2004). Embodied emotions. In Solomon, R. C. & Harlan, L. C. (Eds.), Thinking about feeling: Contemporary philosophers on emotion, Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (1975). The meaning of meaning. In Gunderson, K. (Ed.), Language, mind, and knowledge. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Quine, W. V. (1960). Word and object. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Russell, C. L., Bard, K. A., & Adamson, L. B. (1997). Social referencing by young chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111, 185193.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (2013). Agency: Its role in mental development. London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. London, UK: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Snowden, C., & Boe, C. (2003). Social communication about unpalatable foods in tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 142148.Google Scholar
Southgate, V., van Maanen, C., & Csibra, G. (2007). Infant pointing: Communication to cooperate or communication to learn? Child Development, 78, 735740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance: Communication and cognition. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sperber, D., (2002). Pragmatics, modularity and mindreading. Mind and Language, 17, 323.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Tauzin, T., & Gergely, G. (2018). Communicative mind-reading in preverbal infants. Scientific Reports, 8(9534).Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. J. (2001) Infant intersubjectivity: Research, theory, and clinical applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 348.Google Scholar
Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American Psychologist, 44, 112119.Google Scholar
Tronick, E. Z., & Cohn, J. F. (1989). Infant–mother face-to-face interaction: Age and gender differences in coordination and the occurrence of miscoordination. Child Development, 60, 8592.Google Scholar
Vouloumanos, A., & Waxman, S. R. (2014). Listen up! Speech is for thinking during infancy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 642646.Google Scholar
Watson, J. S. (1985). Contingency perception in early social development. In Field, T. M. & Fox, N. A. (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 157176). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Watson, J. S. (1994). Detection of self: The perfect algorithm. In Parker, S. T., Mitchell, R. W., & Boccia, M. L. (Eds.), Self-awareness in animals and humans: Developmental perspectives (pp. 131148). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. S. (1995). Mother–infant interaction: Dispositional properties and mutual designs. In Thompson, N. S. (Ed.), Perspectives in ethology. Vol. 11. Behavioral design (pp. 189210). New York, NY: Plenum Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×