Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T10:34:00.228Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 8 - Effects of the Postvocalic Nasal on the Perception of American English Vowels by Native Speakers of American English and Japanese

from Part II - Segmental Acquisition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 January 2021

Ratree Wayland
Affiliation:
University of Florida
Get access

Summary

Vowels are said to be less distinctive in prenasal context. The “pin/pen” merger in the Southern United States is a good example. This study attempts to investigate the effects of the postvocalic nasal on the identification and discrimination of American English vowels by native speakers of American English (NE) and Japanese (NJ). These two groups of participants identified six American English (AE) monophthongs /i, ɪ, ɛ, æ, ɑ, ʌ/ and discriminated six vowel pairs /i/-/ɪ/, /ɛ/-/ɪ/, /æ/-/ɛ/, /æ/-/ɑ/, /æ/-/ʌ /, and /ɑ/-/ʌ / in prenasal context. NJ also identified these American English vowels in terms of Japanese vowel categories. The results revealed that, overall, NE outperformed NJ in both identification and discrimination. In addition, how AE vowels were perceptually mapped to Japanese vowels predicted NJ’s discrimination. However, both groups’ performances were found to be poorer in the prenasal context when compared to their previous performances in the preplosive context (Nozawa & Wayland 2012), and NJ were more adversely (but differently) affected by nasalization than NE.

Type
Chapter
Information
Second Language Speech Learning
Theoretical and Empirical Progress
, pp. 228 - 246
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Beddor, P. S. (1993). The perception of nasal vowels. In Huffman, M. K. & Krakow, R. A. (Eds.), Nasals, nasalization, and the velum (Phonetics and Phonology Vol. 5, pp. 171196). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Beddor, P. S., & Krakow, R. A. (1999). Perception of coarticulatory nasalization by speakers of English and Thai: Evidence for partial compensation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106, 28682887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beddor, P. S., Krakow, R. A., & Goldstein, L. M. (1986). Perceptual constraints and phonological change: A study of nasal vowel height. Phonology Yearbook, 3, 197217.Google Scholar
Best, C. T. (1994). The emergence of language-specific phonemic influences in infant speech perception. In Goodman, J. G. & Nusbaum, H. C. (Eds.), The development of speech perception (pp. 167224), Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Best, C. T. (1995). A direct realist view of cross-language speech perception. In Strange, W (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience (pp. 171204). Baltimore: York Press.Google Scholar
Best, C. T., McRoberts, G. W., & Sithole, N. M. (1988). Examination of perceptual reorganization for nonnative speech contrasts: Zulu click discrimination by English-speaking adults and infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(3), 345360.Google Scholar
Bohn, O.-S., & Flege, J. (1992). The production of new and similar vowels by adult German learners of English. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 14, 131158.Google Scholar
Boomershine, A. (2013). The perception of English vowels by monolingual, bilingual, and heritage speakers of Spanish and English. In Selected proceedings of the 15th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium (pp. 103118). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Bundgaard-Nielsen, R., Best, C. T., & Tyler, M. D. (2011). Vocabulary size matters: The assimilation of second-language Australian English vowels to first-language Japanese vowel categories. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 5167.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E. (1995). Second language speech learning theory, findings, and problems. In Strange, W (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience (pp. 223277). Baltimore: York Press.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., & MacKay, I. (2004). Perceiving vowels in a second language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 134.Google Scholar
Fox, A. R., Flege, J. E., & Munro, M. J. (1995). The perception of English and Spanish vowels by native English and Spanish listeners: A multidimensional scaling analysis. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97, 25402551.Google Scholar
Frieda, E., & Nozawa, T. (2007). You are what you eat phonetically: The effect of linguistic experience on the perception of foreign vowels. In Bohn, O.-S. & Munro, M. J. (Eds.), Language experience in second language speech learning: In honor of James Emil Flege (pp. 7996). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Harnsberger, J. D. (2001). On the relationship between identification and discrimination of non-native nasal consonants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 110, 489503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hillenbrand, J. M., Clark, M. J., & Nearey, T. M. (2001). Effects of consonant environment on vowel formant patterns. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 109, 748763.Google Scholar
Ingram, J. C. L., & Park, S.-G. (1997). Cross-language vowel perception and production by Japanese and Korean learners of English. Journal of Phonetics, 25, 343370.Google Scholar
Johnson, K. (1997). Acoustic and auditory phonetics. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kasahara, K., Machida, N., Osada, E., Takahashi, T., & Yoshizawa, S. (2012). Vocabulary knowledge of 5th and 6th graders at elementary school: Connection between sound, meaning and spelling. JES Journal, 12, 90101.Google Scholar
Krakow, R. A., Beddor, P. S., Goldstein, L. M., & Fowler, C. A. (1988). Coarticulatory influence on the perceived height of nasal vowels. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 83, 11461158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Labov, W. (2010). Principles of linguistic change: Cognitive and cultural factors. West Sussex, England: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Labov, W., Ash, S., & Boberg, C. (2005). Atlas of North American English: Phonetics, phonology and sound change. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladefoged, P. (2003). Phonetic data analysis: An introduction to fieldwork and instrumental techniques. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Ladefoged, P. (2005). A course in phonetics (5th ed.). Boston: Thomson.Google Scholar
Lengeris, A. (2009). Perceptual assimilation and L2 learning: Evidence from the perception of southern British English vowels by native speakers of Greek and Japanese. Phonetica, 66, 169187.Google Scholar
Levy, E. S. (2009). Language experience and consonantal context effects on perceptual assimilation of French vowels by American-English learners of French. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125, 11381152.Google Scholar
Morrison, G. S. (2002). Japanese listeners’ use of duration cues in the identification of English high front vowels. In Proceedings of the 28th annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 189200).Google Scholar
Nozawa, T. (2018). How native speakers of Japanese and American English label vowels of each other’s L1 in terms of their L1 vowel categories. Research on Phonetic Language, 12, 3954.Google Scholar
Nozawa, T. (2019). Effects of the manner of articulation of the syllable-final consonant on the perception of American English vowels by native Japanese speakers: Divergence between Japanese Speakers’ image of English vowels and what English vowels really sound like to them. PhD dissertation, Osaka University.Google Scholar
Nozawa, T., & Cheon, S. Y. (2017). Identification of vowels of two different varieties of English by native speakers of Japanese and Korean. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 141, 3519 (abstract).Google Scholar
Nozawa, T., & Frieda, E. M. (2007). Perceptual similarity of American English and Japanese vowels for native speakers of American English and Japanese. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 122, 3529 (abstract).Google Scholar
Nozawa, T., & Wayland, R. (2012). Effects of consonantal contexts on the discrimination and identification of American English vowels by native speakers of Japanese. Journal of the Japan Society of Speech Sciences, 13, 1939.Google Scholar
Olive, J. P., Greenwood, A., & Coleman, J. (1993). Acoustics of American English speech. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Polka, L. (1992). Characterizing the influence of native language experience on adult speech perception. Perception and Psychophysics, 1, 3752.Google Scholar
Polka, L. (1995). Linguistic influence in adult perception of non-native vowel contrasts. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97, 12861296.Google Scholar
Quackenbush, E. (1974). How Japanese English words. Linguistics – An Interdisciplinary Journal of the Language Sciences, 131, 5985.Google Scholar
Roeder, R. (2010). Effects of consonantal context on the pronunciation of /æ/ in the English of speakers of Mexican heritage from South Central Michigan. In Preston, D. R. & Niedzielski, N (Eds.), A reader in sociophonetics (pp. 7189). New York: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Strange, W., Akahane-Yamada, R., Kubo, R., Trent, S. A., & Nishi, K. (2001). Effects of consonantal context on perceptual assimilation of American English vowels by Japanese listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 109, 16911704.Google Scholar
Strange, W., Akahane-Yamada, R., Kubo, R., Trent, S. A., Nishi, K., & Jenkins, J. J. (1998). Perceptual assimilation of American English vowels by Japanese listeners. Journal of Phonetics, 26, 311344.Google Scholar
Tanowitz, J., & Beddor, P. S. (1997). Temporal characteristics of coarticulatory vowel nasalization in English. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 101, 3194 (abstract).Google Scholar
Tyler, M. D., Best, C. T., Faber, A., & Levitt, A. G. (2014). Perceptual assimilation and discrimination of non-native vowel contrasts. Phonetica, 71, 421.Google Scholar
Wright, J. T. (1986). The behavior of nasalized vowels in the perceptual vowel space. In Ohara, J. J. & Jaeger, J. J. (Eds.), Experimental phonology (pp. 4567). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×