Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T08:57:34.692Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 17 - Investigating the Role of Cognitive Abilities in Phonetic Learning of Foreign Consonants and Lexical Tones

from Part V - Cognitive and Psychological Variables

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 January 2021

Ratree Wayland
Affiliation:
University of Florida
Get access

Summary

The goal of this study was to examine the role of cognitive abilities in implicit learning of two types of contrasts — lexical tone contrasts and coronal consonant contrasts. Twenty-four adult native English speakers naïve to Vietnamese were asked to learn the names of twelve funny monsters, that is monosyllabic words differing in tones and initial consonants. After a word-identification training with feedback, learning was assessed using a 12-word alternative test and an auditory discrimination test. In addition, participants completed six tests of cognitive abilities involving episodic memory, working memory, executive function and attention, processing speed, and vocabulary (NIH & Northwestern University, 2006-2012). For both identification and discrimination, the proportion of tone errors was significantly larger than the proportion of consonant errors, but cognitive measures did not predict the occurrence of tone versus consonant errors. Processing speed and executive function and attention scores were significantly correlated with response accuracies in both tests. These results suggest that for beginning learners, individual differences in some cognitive abilities related to the fluid cognition complex correlate with implicit learning of phonetic categories of different types. However, working memory, arguably the most studied factor, appears not to matter in this learning scenario.

Type
Chapter
Information
Second Language Speech Learning
Theoretical and Empirical Progress
, pp. 418 - 438
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andringa, S., Olsthoorn, N., van Beuningen, C., Schoonen, R., & Hulstijn, J. (2012). Determinants of success in native and non-native listening comprehension: An individual differences approach. Language Learning, 62(2), 4978.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 129.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A., Allen, R. J., & Hitch, G. J. (2010). Investigating the episodic buffer. Phychologica Belgica, 50(3&4), 223243.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. A. (Ed.), Recent advances in learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 4790). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Brooks, P. J., & Kempe, V. (2013). Individual differences in adult foreign language learning: The mediating effect of metalinguistic awareness. Memory and Cognition, 41(2), 281296.Google Scholar
Burnham, D., & Mattock, K. (2007). The perception of tones and phones. In Bohn, O.-S. & Munro, M. J. (Eds.), Language experience in second language speech learning: In honor of James Emil Flege (pp. 259280). Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B., & Sapon, S. S. (1959). Modern language aptitude test. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Cedrus Corporation. (2008). SuperLab 4.0 [Computer software]. Phoenix, AZ: Author. Retrieved from www.superlab.com/Google Scholar
Cooper, A., & Wang, Y. (2012). The influence of linguistic and musical experience on Cantonese word learning. The Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 131(6), 47564769.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (2012). Interactions between individual differences, treatments, and structures in SLA. Language Learning, 62(2), 189200.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. (2005). The psychology of the language learner: Individual differences in second language acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ellis, R. (2008). Individual differences and second language learning. In The study of second language acquisition (2nd ed., pp. 643724). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working memory and language. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goldinger, S. D. (1996). Words and voices: Episodic traces in spoken word identification and recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(5), 11661183.Google Scholar
Golestani, N., & Zatorre, R. J. (2009). Individual differences in the acquisition of second language phonology. Brain and Language, 109(2–3), 5567.Google Scholar
Goss, S. J., & Tamaoka, K. (2019). Lexical accent perception in highly-proficient L2 Japanese learners: The roles of language-specific experience and domain-general resources. Second Language Research, 35(3), 351376.Google Scholar
Guion, S. G., & Pederson, E. (2007). Investigating the role of attention in phonetic learning. In Bohn, O.-S. & Munro, M (Eds.), Language experience in second language speech learning (pp. 5777). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hanulíková, A., Dediu, D., Fang, Z., Bašnaková, J., & Huettig, F. (2012). Individual differences in the acquisition of a complex L2 phonology: A training study. Language Learning, 62(s2), 79109.Google Scholar
Hao, Y.-C. (2018). Second language perception of Mandarin vowels and tones. Language and Speech, 61(1), 135152.Google Scholar
Heaton, R. K., Akshoomoff, N., Tulsky, D., Mungas, D., Weintraub, S., Dikmen, S., … Gershon, R. (2014). Reliability and validity of Composite Sores from the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery in adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20(6), 588598.Google Scholar
Hyman, L. M. (2011). Tone: Is it different? In Goldsmith, J, Riggle, J, & Yu, A (Eds.), The handbook of phonological theory (2nd ed., pp. 197239). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
IBM Corp. (2017). IBM SPSS Statistics for Macintosh (Version 25.0). Armonk, NY: Author.Google Scholar
Kidd, E., Donnelly, S., & Christiansen, M. H. (2017). Individual differences in language acquisition and processing. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 22(2), 154169.Google Scholar
Kirby, J. P. (2011). Illustration of the IPA: Vietnamese (Hanoi Vietnamese). Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 41(3), 381392.Google Scholar
Kyllonen, P., & Christal, R. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working memory capacity?! Intelligence, 14, 389433.Google Scholar
Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (2005). Detection theory: A user’s guide (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Maddox, W. T., & Chandrasekaran, B. (2014). Tests of a dual-system model of speech category learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(4), 709728.Google Scholar
Martin, K. I., & Ellis, N. C. (2012). The roles of phonological short-term memory and working memory in L2 grammar and vocabulary learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34(3), 379413.Google Scholar
Morgan-Short, K., Faretta-Stutenberg, M., Brill-Schuetz, K. A., Carpenter, H., & Wong, P. C. M. (2014). Declarative and procedural memory as individual differences in second language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(1), 5672.Google Scholar
Necka, E., Machera, M., & Miklas, E. (1992). Incidental learning, intelligence, and verbal ability. Learning and Instruction, 2(2), 141153.Google Scholar
National Institute of Health & Northwestern University. (2006–2012). NIH Toolbox: Cognition. Retrieved from www.assessmentcenter.net/Google Scholar
Pajak, B., Creel, S. C., & Levy, R. (2016). Difficulty in learning similar-sounding words: A developmental stage or a general property of learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 49(2), 13771399.Google Scholar
Pallier, C., Bosch, L., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (1997). A limit on behavioral plasticity in speech perception. Cognition, 64(3), B9–B17. Perrachione, T. K., Lee, J., Ha, L. Y. Y, & Wong, P. C. M. (2011). Learning a novel phonological contrast depends on interactions between individual differences and training paradigm design. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130(1), 461472.Google Scholar
Roberts, L. (2012). Individual differences in second language sentence processing. Language Learning, 62(2), 172188.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (1997). Individual differences and the fundamental similarity of implicit and explicit adult second language learning. Language Learning, 47(1), 4599.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2001). Individual differences, cognitive abilities, aptitude complexes and learning conditions in second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 17(4), 368392.Google Scholar
Serafini, E. J., & Sanz, C. (2016). Evidence for the decreasing impact of cognitive ability on second language development as proficiency increases. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38(4), 607646.Google Scholar
Shport, I. A. (2008). Acquisition of Japanese pitch accent by American learners. In Heinrich, P & Sugita, Y (Eds.), Japanese as foreign language in the age of globalization (Vol. 43, pp. 165187). Munich, Germany: Iudicium.Google Scholar
Shport, I. A. (2015). Perception of acoustic cues to Tokyo Japanese pitch-accent contrasts in native Japanese and naïve English listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 138(1), 307318.Google Scholar
Shport, I. (2016). Perceptual assimilation and discrimination of falling, level, and rising lexical tones by native English speakers. In Barnes, J, Brugos, A, Shattuck-Hufnagel, S, & Veilleux, N (Eds.), Speech Prosody 2016 (pp. 533537). Boston: Boston University.Google Scholar
Shport, I. A. (2019). Perception of Vietnamese back vowels contrasting in rounding by English listeners. Journal of Phonetics, 73, 823.Google Scholar
Silbert, N., Smith, B. K., Jackson, S. R., Campbell, S. G., Hughes, M. M., & Tare, M. (2015). Non-native phonemic discrimination, phonological short working memory, and word learning. Journal of Phonetics, 50, 99119.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (1991). Individual differences in second language learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 13(2), 275298.Google Scholar
Slotkin, J., et al. (2012). NIH Toolbox: Scoring and interpretation manual. National Institute of Health & Northwestern University. Retrieved from www.healthmeasures.net/images/nihtoolbox/Training-Admin-Scoring_Manuals/NIH_Toolbox_Scoring_and_Interpretation_Manual_9-27-12.pdfGoogle Scholar
So, C. K., & Best, C. T. (2010). Cross-language perception of non-native tonal contrasts: Effects of native phonological and phonetic influences. Language and Speech, 53(2), 273293.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In Tulving, E & Donaldson, W (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381403). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T. (2001). The declarative/procedural model of lexicon and grammar. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(1), 3769.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Weintraub, S., et al. (2013). Cognition assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology, 80, S54S64.Google Scholar
Wong, P. C. M., & Perrachione, T. K. (2007). Learning pitch patterns in lexical identification by native English-speaking adults. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(4), 565585.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×