Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-956mj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-01T16:08:56.952Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Constructing Experience

Expectation and Attention in Perception

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 January 2025

Jason Clarke
Affiliation:
University of West London

Summary

This Element examines the influence of expectation and attention on conscious perception. It explores the debate on whether attention is necessary for conscious perception by presenting empirical evidence from studies on inattentional blindness, change blindness, and the attentional blink. While the evidence strongly suggests that attention is necessary for conscious perception, other research has shown that expectation can shape perception, sometimes leading to illusory experiences where predicted stimuli are perceived despite their absence. This phenomenon, termed 'expectation awareness', suggests that attention may not be necessary for all conscious experiences. These findings are explored within the predictive processing framework, where the brain is characterized as a prediction engine, continuously updating its internal models to minimize prediction errors. Integrating findings from psychology, neuroscience, and cognitive science, this Element provides a predictive processing model of how attention and expectation construct perceptual reality. It also discusses clinical and theoretical implications and suggests future research.
Get access
Type
Element
Information
Online ISBN: 9781009588577
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication: 30 January 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, W. J., Graf, E. W., & Ernst, M. O. (2004). Experience can change the ‘light-from-above’ prior. Nature Neuroscience, 7(10), 10571058. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aitchison, L., & Lengyel, M. (2017). With or without you: Predictive coding and Bayesian inference in the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 46, 219227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2017.08.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alink, A., Schwiedrzik, C. M., Kohler, A., Singer, W., & Muckli, L. (2010). Stimulus predictability reduces responses in primary visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(8), 29602966. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3730-10.2010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amir, Y. Z., Assaf, Y., Yovel, Y., & Mudrik, L. (2023). Experiencing without knowing? Empirical evidence for phenomenal consciousness without access. Cognition, 238, 105529. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aru, J., Bachmann, T., Singer, W., & Melloni, L. (2012). Distilling the neural correlates of consciousness. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(2), 737746. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.12.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aru, J., & Bachmann, T. (2017). Expectation creates something out of nothing: The role of attention in iconic memory reconsidered. Consciousness and Cognition, 53, 203210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2017.06.017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aru, J., Tulver, K., & Bachmann, T. (2018). It’s all in your head: Expectations create illusory perception in a dual-task setup. Consciousness and Cognition, 65, 197208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2018.09.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aru, J., Suzuki, M., Rutiku, R., Larkum, M. E., & Bachmann, T. (2019). Coupling the state and contents of consciousness. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 13, 43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Averbach, E., & Coriell, A. S. (1961). Short-term memory in vision. Bell System Technical Journal, 40, 309328. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1538-7305.1961.tb03987.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baars, B. J. (1993). A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Baars, B. J. (1997). In the theatre of consciousness: Global workspace theory, a rigorous scientific theory of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 4(4), 292309.Google Scholar
Bachmann, T., Breitmeyer, B. G., & Öğmen, H. (2011). The experimental phenomena of consciousness: A brief dictionary revised edition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bachmann, T. (2021). ‘Normal’ hallucinations and attention. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 15, 731600. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2021.731600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bachmann, T., & Aru, J. (2023). Conscious interpretation: A distinct aspect for the neural markers of the contents of consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 108, 103471. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2023.103471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bar, M. (2004). Visual objects in context. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(8), 617629. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bar, M., & Ullman, S. (1996). Spatial context in recognition. Perception, 25(3), 343352. https://doi.org/10.1068/p250343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayes, T. (1763). An essay towards solving a problem in the doctrine of chances. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 53, 370418. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstl.1763.0053.Google Scholar
Bayne, T., Hohwy, J., & Owen, A. M. (2016). Are there levels of consciousness? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(6), 405413. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.03.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Becker, M. W., Pashler, H., & Anstis, S. M. (2000). The role of iconic memory in change-detection tasks. Perception, 29(3), 273286. https://doi.org/10.1068/p3035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berkeley, G. (1948). An essay toward a new theory of vision, 1709. In Dennis, W. (Ed.), Readings in the history of psychology (pp. 6980). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. https://doi.org/10.1037/11304-009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biederman, I., Glass, A. L., & Stacy, E. W. (1973). Searching for objects in real-world scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 97(1), 2227. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0033776.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biederman, I., Rabinowitz, J. C., Glass, A. L., & Stacy, E. W. (1974). On the information extracted from a glance at a scene. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103(3), 597600. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0037158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biederman, I. (1981). On the semantics of a glance at a scene. In Kubovy, M., & Pomerantz, J. R. (Eds.), Perceptual Organization (pp. 213253). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Biederman, I., Mezzanotte, R. J., & Rabinowitz, J. C. (1982). Scene perception: Detecting and judging objects undergoing relational violations. Cognitive Psychology, 14(2), 143177. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(82)90007-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bisley, J. W., & Goldberg, M. E. (2010). Attention, intention, and priority in the parietal lobe. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 33, 121. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-152823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blackmore, S. (2005). Consciousness: A very short introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/actrade/9780192805850.001.0001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blackmore, S. J., Brelstaff, G., Nelson, K., & Trościanko, T. (1995). Is the richness of our visual world an illusion? Transsaccadic memory for complex scenes. Perception, 24(9), 10751081. https://doi.org/10.1068/p241075.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blake, R., & Logothetis, N. (2002). Visual competition. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 3(1), 1321. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Block, N. (1995). On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18(2), 227287. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00038188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Block, N. (2005). Two neural correlates of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 4652. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Block, N. (2007). Consciousness, accessibility, and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(5–6), 481499. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X07002786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Block, N. (2011). Perceptual consciousness overflows cognitive access. Trends in cognitive sciences, 15(12), 567575. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bola, M., & Doradzińska, Ł. (2021). Perceptual awareness negativity – does it reflect awareness or attention? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 15, 742513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonneh, Y., Cooperman, A., & Sagi, D. (2001). Motion-induced blindness in normal observers. Nature, 411, 798801. https://doi.org/10.1038/35081073.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brandman, T., & Peelen, M. V. (2017). Interaction between scene and object processing revealed by human fMRI and MEG decoding. The Journal of Neuroscience, 37(32), 77007710. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0582-17.2017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breitmeyer, B. G. (2007). Visual masking: Past accomplishments, present status, future developments. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 3(1–2), 9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breitmeyer, B. G., & Öğmen, H. (2006). Visual masking: Time slices through conscious and unconscious vision (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198530671.001.0001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breitmeyer, B. G. (2014). The visual (un)conscious and its (dis)contents: A microtemporal approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. Oxford: Pergamon Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronfman, Z. Z., Brezis, N., Jacobson, H., & Usher, M. (2014). We see more than we can report: ‘Cost free’ color phenomenality outside focal attention. Psychological Science, 25(7), 13941403. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614532656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bugelski, B. R., & Alampay, D. A. (1961). The role of frequency in developing perceptual sets. Canadian Journal of Psychology / Revue canadienne de psychologie, 15(4), 205211. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0083443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buschman, T. J., & Miller, E. K. (2007). Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science, 315(5820), 18601862. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1138071.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carrasco, M., Penpeci-Talgar, C., & Eckstein, M. (2000). Spatial covert attention increases contrast sensitivity across the CSF: Support for signal enhancement. Vision Research, 40(10–12), 12031215. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0042-6989(00)00024-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carrasco, M., & McElree, B. (2001). Covert attention accelerates the rate of visual information processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(9), 53635367. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.081074098.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carrasco, M., Williams, P. E., & Yeshurun, Y. (2002). Covert attention increases spatial resolution with or without masks: Support for signal enhancement. Journal of Vision, 2(6), 467479. https://doi.org/10.1167/2.6.4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carrasco, M., Ling, S., & Read, S. (2004). Attention alters appearance. Nature Neuroscience, 7(3), 308313. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chun, M. M., & Potter, M. C. (1995). A two-stage model for multiple target detection in rapid serial visual presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(1), 109127. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.21.1.109.Google ScholarPubMed
Ciupińska, K., Orłowska, W., Zębrowski, A., et al. (2024). The influence of spatial and temporal attention on visual awareness-a behavioral and ERP study. Cerebral Cortex, 34(6), bhae241. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhae241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. (2015). Surfing uncertainty: Prediction, action, and the embodied mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clarke, J., & Mack, A. (2015). Iconic memory for natural scenes: Evidence using a modified change-detection procedure. Visual Cognition, 23(7), 917938. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2015.1103826.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, J., & Porubanova, M. (2020). Scene and object violations cause subjective time dilation. Timing & Time Perception, 8(3–4), 279298. https://doi.org/10.1163/22134468-bja10012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, M. A., Alvarez, G. A., & Nakayama, K. (2011). Natural-scene perception requires attention. Psychological Science, 22(9), 11651172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, M. A., & Dennett, D. C. (2011). Consciousness cannot be separated from function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(8), 358364. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.06.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, M. A., Dennett, D. C., & Kanwisher, N. G. (2016). What is the bandwidth of perceptual experience? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 324335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, M. A., Ortego, K., Kyroudis, A., & Pitts, M. (2020). Distinguishing the neural correlates of perceptual awareness and postperceptual processing. Journal of Neuroscience, 40(25), 49254935.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coltheart, M. (1980). Iconic memory and visible persistence. Perception & Psychophysics, 27, 183228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coltheart, M., & Coltheart, V. (2010). Visual memories. In Coltheart, V. (Ed.), Tutorials in visual cognition New York: Psychology Press, Taylor and Francis.Google Scholar
Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201215. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corlett, P. R., Horga, G., Fletcher, P. C., et al. (2019). Hallucinations and strong priors. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(2), 114127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(1), 87185. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x01003922.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowan, N. (2010). The magical mystery four: How is working memory capacity limited, and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(1), 5157. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721409359277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowan, N. (2010). Multiple concurrent thoughts: The meaning and developmental neuropsychology of working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35(5), 447474. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2010.494985.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowey, A., & Stoerig, P. (2004). Stimulus cueing in blindsight. Progress in Brain Research, 144, 261277. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(03)14418-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davenport, J. L., & Potter, M. C. (2004). Scene consistency in object and background perception. Psychological Science, 15(8), 559564. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00719.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S., Kerszberg, M., & Changeux, J. P. (1998). A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(24), 1452914534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S., & Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition, 79(1–2), 137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S., Changeux, J. P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., & Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5), 204211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J. P. (2011). Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron, 70(2), 200227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S., Changeux, J. P., & Naccache, L. (2011). The global neuronal workspace model of conscious access: From neuronal architectures to clinical applications. In S. Dehaene & Y. Christen (Eds.), Characterizing consciousness: From cognition to the clinic? (pp. 55–84).Google Scholar
Dembski, C., Koch, C., & Pitts, M. (2021). Perceptual awareness negativity: A physiological correlate of sensory consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(8), 660670. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2021.05.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193222. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ding, Y., Hults, C. M., Raja, R., & Simons, D. J. (2023). Similarity of an unexpected object to the attended and ignored objects affects noticing in a sustained inattentional blindness task. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 85(7), 21502169. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-023-02794-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doradzińska, Ł. , & Bola, M. (2024). Early electrophysiological correlates of perceptual consciousness are affected by both exogenous and endogenous attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 36(7), 12971324. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_02156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drew, T., , M. L. H., & Wolfe, J. M. (2013). The invisible gorilla strikes again: Sustained inattentional blindness in expert observers. Psychological Science, 24(9), 18481853.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duyar, A., Ren, S., & Carrasco, M. (2024). When temporal attention interacts with expectation. Scientific Reports, 14(1), 4624. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-55399-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egly, R., Driver, J., & Rafal, R. D. (1994). Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: Evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123(2), 161177. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.123.2.161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egner, T., Monti, J. M., & Summerfield, C. (2010). Expectation and surprise determine neural population responses in the ventral visual stream. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(49), 1660116608. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2770-10.2010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eklund, R., & Wiens, S. (2018). Visual awareness negativity is an early neural correlate of awareness: A preregistered study with two Gabor sizes. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 18(1), 176188. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-0562-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eriksen, C. W., & St James, J. D. (1986). Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model. Perception & Psychophysics, 40(4), 225240. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03211502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erol, M., Mack, A., Clarke, J., & Bert, J. (2016). Inattentional blindness to absent stimuli: The role of expectation. Journal of Vision, 16(12), 40. https://doi.org/10.1167/16.12.40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erol, M., Mack, A., & Clarke, J. (2018). Expectation blindness: Seeing a face when there is none. Journal of Vision, 18(10), 1115. https://doi.org/10.1167/18.10.1115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, H., & Friston, K. J. (2010). Attention, uncertainty, and free-energy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 215. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2010.00215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, P. C., & Frith, C. D. (2009). Perceiving is believing: A Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(1), 4858.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Förster, J., Koivisto, M., & Revonsuo, A. (2020). ERP and MEG correlates of visual consciousness: The second decade. Consciousness and Cognition, 80, 102917.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frässle, S., Sommer, J., Jansen, A., Naber, M., & Einhäuser, W. (2014). Binocular rivalry: Frontal activity relates to introspection and action but not to perception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34(5), 17381747. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4403-13.2014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K. (2003). Learning and inference in the brain. Neural Networks, 16(9), 13251352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2003.06.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K. (2005). A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological Sciences, 360(1456), 815836. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2005.1622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature reviews: Neuroscience, 11(2), 127138. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2787.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K., Adams, R. A., Perrinet, L., & Breakspear, M. (2012). Perceptions as hypotheses: Saccades as experiments. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 151. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K. J. (2005). Hallucinations and perceptual inference. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(6), 764766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, C. D. (2007). The social brain? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological Sciences, 362(1480), 671678. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.2003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Girshick, A. R., Landy, M. S., & Simoncelli, E. P. (2011). Cardinal rules: Visual orientation perception reflects knowledge of environmental statistics. Nature Neuroscience, 14(7), 926932. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2831.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldreich, D., & Peterson, M. A. (2012). A Bayesian observer replicates convexity context effects in figure-ground perception. Seeing and Perceiving, 25(3–4), 365395. https://doi.org/10.1163/187847612X634445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gombrich, E. H. (2023). Art and illusion: A study in the psychology of pictorial representation. New York: Phaidon Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, N., Tsuchiya, N., Koenig-Robert, R., & Hohwy, J. (2019). Expectation and attention increase the integration of top-down and bottom-up signals in perception through different pathways. PLoS Biology, 17(4), e3000233. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graziano, M. S. A. (2013). Consciousness and the social brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gregory, R. L. (1980). Perceptions as hypotheses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological Sciences, 290(1038), 181197. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1980.0090.Google ScholarPubMed
Grill-Spector, K., & Malach, R. (2004). The human visual cortex. Annual Review Neuroscience, 27(1), 649677.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, A. M., Dux, P. E., & Mattingley, J. B. (2020). Awareness is related to reduced post-stimulus alpha power: A no-report inattentional blindness study. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 52(11), 44114422. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13947.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D., Summerfield, C., & Botvinick, M. (2017). Neuroscience-inspired artificial intelligence. Neuron, 95(2), 245258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haun, A. M., Tononi, G., Koch, C., & Tsuchiya, N. (2017). Are we underestimating the richness of visual experience? Neuroscience of Consciousness, 2017(1), niw023. https://doi.org/10.1093/nc/niw023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
He, S., Cavanagh, P., & Intriligator, J. (1996). Attentional resolution and the locus of visual awareness. Nature, 383(6598), 334337. https://doi.org/10.1038/383334a0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helmholtz, H. von (1867/1925). Treatise on physiological optics (from 3rd German edition, Trans.). Cambridge, MA. Harvard University Press. https://doi.org/10.1037/13536-000.Google Scholar
Hohwy, J. (2009). The neural correlates of consciousness: New experimental approaches needed? Consciousness and Cognition, 18(2), 428438. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2009.02.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hohwy, J. (2012). Attention and conscious perception in the hypothesis testing brain. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 96. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00096.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hohwy, J. (2013). The predictive mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hohwy, J. (2020). New directions in predictive processing. Mind & Language, 35(2), 209223. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollingworth, A., & Henderson, J. M. (1999). Object identification is isolated from scene semantic constraint: Evidence from object type and token discrimination. Acta Psychologica, 102(2–3), 319343. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0001-6918(98)00053-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsieh, P. J., Colas, J. T., & Kanwisher, N. (2011). Pop-out without awareness: Unseen feature singletons capture attention only when top-down attention is available. Psychological Science, 22(9), 12201226. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611419302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1959). Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat’s striate cortex. The Journal of Physiology, 148(3), 574591. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1959.sp006308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1962). Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex. The Journal of Physiology, 160(1), 106154. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1962.sp006837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson-Nielsen, M., Cohen, M. A., & Pitts, M. A. (2017). Perception of ensemble statistics requires attention. Consciousness and Cognition, 48, 149160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.11.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Dover.Google Scholar
Jeffreys, H. (1998). Theory of probability. Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, M. S., Yao, R., Street, W. N., & Simons, D. J. (2011). Change blindness and inattentional blindness: Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive Science, 2(5), 529546. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.130.Google Scholar
Jiang, Y., Costello, P., & He, S. (2007). Processing of invisible stimuli: Advantage of upright faces and recognizable words in overcoming interocular suppression. Psychological Science, 18(4), 349355. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01902.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jiang, J., Summerfield, C., & Egner, T. (2013). Attention sharpens the distinction between expected and unexpected percepts in the visual brain. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(47), 1843818447. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3308-13.2013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jimenez, M., Poch, C., Villalba-García, C., et al. (2021). The level of processing modulates visual awareness: Evidence from behavioral and electrophysiological measures. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 33(7), 12951310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jolicoeur, P., & Dell’Acqua, R. (1998). The demonstration of short-term consolidation. Cognitive Psychology, 36(2), 138202. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1998.0684.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, M., & Wilkinson, S. (2020). From prediction to imagination. In Abraham, A. (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the imagination (pp. 94110). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kant, I. (2001). Prolegomena to any future metaphysics (J. W. Ellington, Trans.). Indianapolis, IN: Hackett. (Original work published 1783).Google Scholar
Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(11), 43024311. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kastner, S., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2000). Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 315341. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.315.Google ScholarPubMed
Kentridge, R. W., Heywood, C. A., & Weiskrantz, L. (1999). Attention without awareness in blindsight. Proceedings: Biological Sciences, 266(1430), 18051811. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1999.0850.Google ScholarPubMed
Kentridge, R. W., Heywood, C. A., & Weiskrantz, L. (2004). Spatial attention speeds discrimination without awareness in blindsight. Neuropsychologia, 42(6), 831835.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kentridge, R. W., Nijboer, T. C. W., & Heywood, C. A. (2008). Attended but unseen: Visual attention is not sufficient for visual awareness. Neuropsychologia, 46(3), 864869. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.11.036.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kersten, D., Mamassian, P., & Yuille, A. (2004). Object perception as Bayesian inference. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 271304. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, C. Y., & Blake, R. (2005). Psychophysical magic: Rendering the visible ‘invisible’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(8), 381388. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.06.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kloosterman, N. A., Meindertsma, T., van Loon, A. M., et al. (2015). Pupil size tracks perceptual content and surprise. European Journal of Neuroscience, 41(8), 10681078. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.12859.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knill, D. C., & Pouget, A. (2004). The Bayesian brain: The role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation. Trends in Neurosciences, 27(12), 712719. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2004.10.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koch, C. (2004). The quest for consciousness: A neurobiological approach. Englewood, CO: Roberts.Google Scholar
Koch, C., & Tsuchiya, N. (2007). Attention and consciousness: Two distinct brain processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(1), 1622. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koch, C., Massimini, M., Boly, M., & Tononi, G. (2016). Neural correlates of consciousness: Progress and problems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(5), 307321. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koivisto, M., & Revonsuo, A. (2005). Preconscious and conscious processing of color and form: Evidence from event-related potentials. Consciousness and Cognition, 14(1), 232246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2004.08.001.Google Scholar
Koivisto, M., & Revonsuo, A. (2007). Electrophysiological correlates of visual consciousness and selective attention. Neuroreport, 18(8), 753756. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e3280c143c8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koivisto, M., Lähteenmäki, M., Sørensen, T. A., et al. (2008). The earliest electrophysiological correlate of visual awareness? Brain and Cognition, 66(1), 91103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2007.05.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koivisto, M., & Revonsuo, A. (2008). Comparison of event-related potentials in attentional blink and repetition blindness. Brain Research, 1189, 115126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.10.082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koivisto, M., Kainulainen, P., & Revonsuo, A. (2009). The relationship between awareness and attention: Evidence from ERP responses. Neuropsychologia, 47(13), 28912899. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koivisto, M., & Revonsuo, A. (2010). Event-related brain potential correlates of visual awareness. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(6), 922934. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koivisto, M., Railo, H., Revonsuo, A., Vanni, S., & Salminen-Vaparanta, N. (2011). Recurrent processing in V1/V2 contributes to categorization of natural scenes. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(7), 24882492. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3074-10.2011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kok, P., Jehee, J. F., & de Lange, F. P. (2012). Less is more: Expectation sharpens representations in the primary visual cortex. Neuron, 75(2), 265270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kok, P., Rahnev, D., Jehee, J. F., Lau, H. C., & de Lange, F. P. (2012). Attention reverses the effect of prediction in silencing sensory signals. Cerebral Cortex, 22(9), 21972206. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kok, P., Brouwer, G. J., van Gerven, M. A., & de Lange, F. P. (2013). Prior expectations bias sensory representations in visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(41), 1627516284. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0742-13.2013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kok, P., Failing, M. F., & de Lange, F. P. (2014). Prior expectations evoke stimulus templates in the primary visual cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(7), 15461554. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kok, P., Jehee, J. F., & de Lange, F. P. (2012). Less is more: Expectation sharpens representations in the primary visual cortex. Neuron, 75(2), 265270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kouider, S., de Gardelle, V., Sackur, J., & Dupoux, E. (2010). How rich is consciousness? The partial awareness hypothesis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(7), 301307. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kouider, S., Long, B., Le Stanc, L., et al. (2015). Neural dynamics of prediction and surprise in infants. Nature Communications, 6, 8537. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms9537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, R. L. (2024). A landscape of consciousness: Toward a taxonomy of explanations and implications. Progress in Biophysics and Molecular Biology, 190, 28169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbiomolbio.2023.12.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kveraga, K., Boshyan, J., & Bar, M. (2007). Magnocellular projections as the trigger of top-down facilitation in recognition. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(48), 1323213240. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3481-07.2007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamme, V. A. (2003). Why visual attention and awareness are different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(1), 1218. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(02)00013-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamme, V. A. (2004). Separate neural definitions of visual consciousness and visual attention; a case for phenomenal awareness. Neural Networks, 17(5–6), 861872. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2004.02.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamme, V. A. (2010). How neuroscience will change our view on consciousness. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(3), 204220. https://doi.org/10.1080/17588921003731586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamme, V. A. (2018). Challenges for theories of consciousness: Seeing or knowing, the missing ingredient and how to deal with panpsychism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1755), 20170344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landman, R., Spekreijse, H., & Lamme, V. A. (2003). Large capacity storage of integrated objects before change blindness. Vision Research, 43(2), 149164. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0042-6989(02)00402-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laureys, S. (2005). The neural correlate of (un)awareness: Lessons from the vegetative state. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(12), 556559. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laureys, S., Gosseries, O., & Tononi, G. (Eds.). (2015). The neurology of consciousness: Cognitive neuroscience and neuropathology (2nd ed.). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 7582. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeCun, Y., Bengio, Y., & Hinton, G. (2015). Deep learning. Nature, 521, 436444. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature14539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenharo, M. (2023). Decades-long bet on consciousness ends – and it’s philosopher 1, neuroscientist 0. Nature. 619(7968), 1415. https://doi.org/10.1038/d41586-023-02120-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levin, D. T., Momen, N., Drivdahl, S. B., & Simons, D. J. (2000). Change blindness blindness: The metacognitive error of overestimating change-detection ability. Visual Cognition, 7(1–3), 397412. https://doi.org/10.1080/135062800394865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, F. F., VanRullen, R., Koch, C., & Perona, P. (2002). Rapid natural scene categorization in the near absence of attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99(14), 95969601. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.092277599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logothetis, N. K., Leopold, D. A., & Sheinberg, D. L. (1996). What is rivalling during binocular rivalry? Nature, 380(6575), 621624. https://doi.org/10.1038/380621a0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luck, S. J., Chelazzi, L., Hillyard, S. A., & Desimone, R. (1997). Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 77(1), 2442. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1997.77.1.24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luck, S., & Vogel, E. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature, 390, 279281. https://doi.org/10.1038/36846.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mack, A., & Clarke, J. (2012). Gist perception requires attention. Visual Cognition, 20(3), 300327. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2012.666578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mack, A., Erol, M., & Clarke, J. (2015). Iconic memory is not a case of attention-free awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 291299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.12.016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mack, A., Erol, M., Clarke, J. , & Bert, J. (2016). No iconic memory without attention. Consciousness and Cognition, 40, 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.12.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mack, A., Clarke, J., Erol, M., & Bert, J. (2017). Scene incongruity and attention. Consciousness and Cognition, 48, 87103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.10.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. Cambridge, MA: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Martens, S., & Wyble, B. (2010). The attentional blink: Past, present, and future of a blind spot in perceptual awareness. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(6), 947957. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martínez, A., Teder-Salejarvi, W., & Hillyard, S. A. (2007). Spatial attention facilitates selection of illusory objects: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Brain Research, 1139, 143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.12.056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mashour, G. A., Roelfsema, P., Changeux, J. P., & Dehaene, S. (2020). Conscious processing and the global neuronal workspace hypothesis. Neuron, 105(5), 776798.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McMains, S. A., & Kastner, S. (2010). Defining the units of competition: Influences of perceptual organization on competitive interactions in human visual cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(11), 24172426. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNair, N. A., Goodbourn, P. T., Shone, L. T., & Harris, I. M. (2017). Summary statistics in the attentional blink. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79, 100116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehrpour, V., Martinez-Trujillo, J. C., & Treue, S. (2020). Attention amplifies neural representations of changes in sensory input at the expense of perceptual accuracy. Nature Communications, 11(1), 2128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melloni, L., Mudrik, L., Pitts, M., et al. (2023). An adversarial collaboration protocol for testing contrasting predictions of global neuronal workspace and integrated information theory. PloS One, 18(2), e0268577. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Memmert, D. (2006). The effects of eye movements, age, and expertise on inattentional blindness. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(3), 620627. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2006.01.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Millidge, B., Seth, A., & Buckley, C. L. (2022). Predictive coding: A theoretical and experimental review. arXiv preprint arXiv:2107.12979.Google Scholar
Most, S. B., Chun, M. M., Widders, D. M., & Zald, D. H. (2005). Attentional rubbernecking: Cognitive control and personality in emotion-induced blindness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(4), 654661. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03196754.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Most, S. B., Scholl, B. J., Clifford, E. R., & Simons, D. J. (2005). What you see is what you set: Sustained inattentional blindness and the capture of awareness. Psychological Review, 112(1), 217242. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.112.1.217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Most, S. B., Simons, D. J., Scholl, B. J., Jimenez, R., Clifford, E., & Chabris, C. F. (2001). How not to be seen: The contribution of similarity and selective ignoring to sustained inattentional blindness. Psychological Science, 12(1), 917.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mudrik, L., Breska, A., Lamy, D., & Deouell, L. Y. (2011). Integration without awareness: Expanding the limits of unconscious processing. Psychological Science, 22(6), 764770. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611408736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mudrik, L., & Koch, C. (2013). Differential processing of invisible congruent and incongruent scenes: A case for unconscious integration. Journal of Vision, 13(13), 24. https://doi.org/10.1167/13.13.24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mumford, D. (1992). On the computational architecture of the neocortex. II. The role of cortico-cortical loops. Biological Cybernetics, 66(3), 241251. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00198477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Munneke, J., Brentari, V., & Peelen, M. V. (2013). The influence of scene context on object recognition is independent of attentional focus. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 552. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naccache, L. (2018). Why and how access consciousness can account for phenomenal consciousness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological sciences, 373(1755), 20170357. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? The Philosophical Review, 83(4), 435450. https://doi.org/10.2307/2183914.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Neisser, U., & Becklen, R. (1975). Selective looking: Attending to visually specified events. Cognitive Psychology, 7(4), 480494. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(75)90019-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noah, S., & Mangun, G. R. (2020). Recent evidence that attention is necessary, but not sufficient, for conscious perception. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1464(1), 5263. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.14030.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noë, A. (Ed.). (2002). Is the visual world a grand illusion? Exeter, UK: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
Oliva, A. (2005). Gist of the scene. Neurobiology of Attention, 251256. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012375731-9/50045-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oliva, A., & Torralba, A. (2006). Building the gist of a scene: The role of global image features in recognition. Progress in Brain Research, 155, 2336. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(06)55002-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oliva, A., & Torralba, A. (2007). The role of context in object recognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(12), 520527. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olivers, C. N., & Meeter, M. (2008). A boost and bounce theory of temporal attention. Psychological Review, 115(4), 836863. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olkkonen, M., Hansen, T., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2008). Color appearance of familiar objects: Effects of object shape, texture, and illumination changes. Journal of Vision, 8(5), 116. https://doi.org/10.1167/8.5.13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Regan, J. K. (1992). Solving the ‘real’ mysteries of visual perception: The world as an outside memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 46(3), 461488. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Otten, M., Seth, A. K., & Pinto, Y. (2023). Seeing Ɔ, remembering C: Illusions in short-term memory. PloS One, 18(4), e0283257. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0283257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Overgaard, M. (2018). Phenomenal consciousness and cognitive access. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1755), 20170353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Overgaard, M., & Overgaard, R. (2010). Neural correlates of contents and levels of consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, S. E. (1975). The effects of contextual scenes on the identification of objects. Memory & Cognition, 3(5), 519526. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panichello, M. F., Cheung, O. S., & Bar, M. (2013). Predictive feedback and conscious visual experience. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 620. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peelen, M. V., Berlot, E., & de Lange, F. P. (2024). Predictive processing of scenes and objects. Nature Reviews Psychology, 3, 1326. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-023-00254-0.Google ScholarPubMed
Persuh, M., Genzer, B., & Melara, R. D. (2012). Iconic memory requires attention. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, Article 126. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Persuh, M., & Melara, R. D. (2016). Barack Obama Blindness (BOB): Absence of visual awareness to a single object. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 118. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinto, Y., van Gaal, S., de Lange, F. P., Lamme, V. A., & Seth, A. K. (2015). Expectations accelerate entry of visual stimuli into awareness. Journal of Vision, 15(8), 13. https://doi.org/10.1167/15.8.13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pitts, M. A., Martínez, A., & Hillyard, S. A. (2012). Visual processing of contour patterns under conditions of inattentional blindness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(2), 287303. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pitts, M. A., Lutsyshyna, L. A., & Hillyard, S. A. (2018). The relationship between attention and consciousness: An expanded taxonomy and implications for ‘no-report’ paradigms. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological sciences, 373(1755), 20170348. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0348.Google ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 325. https://doi.org/10.1080/00335558008248231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Potter, M. C., Wyble, B., Hagmann, C. E., & McCourt, E. S. (2014). Detecting meaning in RSVP at 13 ms per picture. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 76(2), 270279. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-013-0605-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Powers III, A. R., Kelley, M., & Corlett, P. R. (2016). Hallucinations as top-down effects on perception. Biological psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 1(5), 393400. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2016.04.003.Google ScholarPubMed
Powers, A. R., Mathys, C., & Corlett, P. R. (2017). Pavlovian conditioning- induced hallucinations result from overweighting of perceptual priors. Science, 357(6351), 596600. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aan3458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Press, C., Kok, P., & Yon, D. (2020). The perceptual prediction paradox. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(1), 1324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.11.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raffone, A., Srinivasan, N., & van Leeuwen, C. (2014). The interplay of attention and consciousness in visual search, attentional blink and working memory consolidation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological Sciences, 369(1641), 20130215. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Railo, H., Koivisto, M., & Revonsuo, A. (2011). Tracking the processes behind conscious perception: A review of event-related potential correlates of visual consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(3), 972983. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.03.019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rao, R. P., & Ballard, D. H. (1999). Predictive coding in the visual cortex: A functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nature Neuroscience, 2(1), 7987.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., & Arnell, K. M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(3), 849860. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.18.3.849.Google Scholar
Rees, G., Kreiman, G., & Koch, C. (2002). Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Nature reviews: Neuroscience, 3(4), 261270. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rees, G., Russell, C., Frith, C. D., & Driver, J. (1999). Inattentional blindness versus inattentional amnesia for fixated but ignored words. Science, 286(5449), 2504-2507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. K., & Clark, J. J. (1997). To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8(5), 368373. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00427.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rensink, R. A. (2000). The dynamic representation of scenes. Visual Cognition, 7(1–3), 1742. https://doi.org/10.1080/135062800394667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rensink, R. A. (2004). Visual sensing without seeing. Psychological Science, 15(1), 2732. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01501005.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reynolds, J. H., & Chelazzi, L. (2004). Attentional modulation of visual processing. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 611647. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reynolds, J. H., & Heeger, D. J. (2009). The normalization model of attention. Neuron, 61(2), 168185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rousselet, G., Joubert, O., & Fabre-Thorpe, M. (2005). How long to get to the ‘gist’ of real-world natural scenes? Visual Cognition, 12(6), 852877.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rousselet, G. A., Macé, M. J., & Fabre-Thorpe, M. (2003). Is it an animal? Is it a human face? Fast processing in upright and inverted natural scenes. Journal of Vision, 3(6), 440455. https://doi.org/10.1167/3.6.5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saenz, M., Buracas, G. T., & Boynton, G. M. (2002). Global effects of feature-based attention in human visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 5(7), 631632. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schelonka, K., Graulty, C., Canseco-Gonzalez, E., & Pitts, M. A. (2017). ERP signatures of conscious and unconscious word and letter perception in an inattentional blindness paradigm. Consciousness and Cognition, 54, 5671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scholl, B. J., Simons, D. J., & Levin, D. T. (2004). ‘Change blindness’ blindness: An implicit measure of a metacognitive error. In Levin, D. T. (Ed.), Thinking and seeing: Visual metacognition in adults and children (pp. 145164). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sergent, C., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Is consciousness a gradual phenomenon? Evidence for an all-or-none bifurcation during the attentional blink. Psychological Science, 15(11), 720728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sergent, C., Baillet, S., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Timing of the brain events underlying access to consciousness during the attentional blink. Nature Neuroscience, 8(10), 13911400. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sergent, C., Wyart, V., Babo-Rebelo, M., Cohen, L., Naccache, L., & Tallon-Baudry, C. (2013). Cueing attention after the stimulus is gone can retrospectively trigger conscious perception. Current Biology, 23(2), 150155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seriès, P., & Seitz, A. R. (2013). Learning what to expect (in visual perception). Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, Article 668. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seth, A. K. (2021). Being you: A new science of consciousness. London: Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
Seth, A. K., & Hohwy, J. (2021). Predictive processing as an empirical theory for consciousness science. Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(2), 8990. https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2020.1838467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seth, A. K., & Bayne, T. (2022). Theories of consciousness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 23, 439452. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-022-00587-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shafto, J. P., & Pitts, M. A. (2015). Neural signatures of conscious face perception in an inattentional blindness paradigm. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35(31), 1094010948. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0145-15.2015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simons, D. J., & Levin, D.T. (1998). Failure to detect changes to people during a real-world interaction. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 644649. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03208840.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28(9), 10591074. https://doi.org/10.1068/p281059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simons, D. J., & Jensen, M. S. (2009). The effects of individual differences and task difficulty on inattentional blindness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(2), 398403. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.2.398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simons, D. J., & Levin, D. T. (1997). Change blindness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(7), 261267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simons, D. J., & Rensink, R. A. (2005). Change blindness: Past, present, and future. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(1), 1620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sligte, I. G., Vandenbroucke, A. R., Scholte, H. S., & Lamme, V. A. (2010). Detailed sensory memory, sloppy working memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 175. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperling, G. (1960). The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 74(11), 129. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterzer, P., Haynes, J. D., & Rees, G. (2008). Fine-scale activity patterns in high-level visual areas encode the category of invisible objects. Journal of Vision, 8(15), 112. https://doi.org/10.1167/8.15.10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sterzer, P., Kleinschmidt, A., & Rees, G. (2009). The neural bases of multistable perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(7), 310318. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.04.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Summerfield, C., & de Lange, F. P. (2014). Expectation in perceptual decision making: Neural and computational mechanisms. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15(11), 745756. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3838CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Summerfield, C., & Egner, T. (2009). Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(9), 403409. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suzuki, K., Roseboom, W., Schwartzman, D. J., & Seth, A. K. (2017). A deep-dream virtual reality platform for studying altered perceptual phenomenology. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 15982. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-16316-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, L. R. (2016). The predictive processing paradigm has roots in Kant. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 10, 79. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2016.00079.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tachmatzidou, O., & Vatakis, A. (2023). Attention and schema violations of real world scenes differentially modulate time perception. Scientific Reports, 13(1), 10002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tong, F., Meng, M., & Blake, R. (2006). Neural bases of binocular rivalry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(11), 502511. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tononi, G., & Koch, C. (2008). The neural correlates of consciousness: An update. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(1), 239261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12(1), 97136. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C. (2014). On the relationship between consciousness and attention. In Gazzaniga, M. S., & Mangun, G. R. (Eds.), The cognitive neurosciences (5th ed., pp. 839853). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9504.003.0092.Google Scholar
Tsuchiya, N., Wilke, M., Frässle, S., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2015). No-report paradigms: Extracting the true neural correlates of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(12), 757770. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valsecchi, M., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2016). Dynamic re-calibration of perceived size in fovea and periphery through predictable size changes. Current Biology: CB, 26(1), 5963. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.10.067.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vandenbroucke, A. R. E., Sligte, I. G., Barrett, A. B., et al. (2014). Accurate metacognition for visual sensory memory representations. Psychological Science, 25(4), 861873. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613516146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vandenbroucke, A. R., Sligte, I. G., Fahrenfort, J. J., Ambroziak, K. B., & Lamme, V. A. (2012). Non-attended representations are perceptual rather than unconscious in nature. PLoS One, 7(11), e50042.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Velmans, M. (2010). Understanding consciousness. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Ward, E. J., & Scholl, B. J. (2015). Inattentional blindness reflects limitations on perception, not memory: Evidence from repeated failures of awareness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(3), 722727. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0745-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, E. J. (2018). Downgraded phenomenology: How conscious overflow lost its richness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological sciences, 373(1755), 20170355. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webster, K., Clarke, J., Mack, A., & Ro, T. (2018). Effects of canonical color, luminance, and orientation on sustained inattentional blindness for scenes. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80(7), 18331846. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-018-1558-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Witzel, C., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2013). Categorical sensitivity to color differences. Journal of Vision, 13(7), Article 1. https://doi.org/10.1167/13.7.1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolfe, J. M. (1999). Inattentional amnesia. In Coltheart, V. (Ed.), Fleeting memories (pp. 7194). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wood, K., & Simons, D. J. (2017). The role of similarity in inattentional blindness: Selective enhancement, selective suppression, or both?†. Visual Cognition, 25(9–10), 972980. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2017.1365791.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeshurun, Y., & Carrasco, M. (1998). Attention improves or impairs visual performance by enhancing spatial resolution. Nature, 396(6706), 7275. https://doi.org/10.1038/23936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeshurun, Y., & Levy, L. (2003). Transient spatial attention degrades temporal resolution. Psychological Science, 14(3), 225231. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.02436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yon, D., & Frith, C. D. (2021). Precision and the Bayesian brain. Current Biology, 31(17), R1026–R1032. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2021.07.044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yon, D., & Press, C. (2017). Predicted action consequences are perceptually facilitated before cancellation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(6), 10731083. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000385.Google ScholarPubMed
Zivony, A., & Lamy, D. (2022). What processes are disrupted during the attentional blink? An integrative review of event-related potential research. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 29(2), 394414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save element to Kindle

To save this element to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Constructing Experience
  • Jason Clarke, University of West London
  • Online ISBN: 9781009588577
Available formats
×

Save element to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Constructing Experience
  • Jason Clarke, University of West London
  • Online ISBN: 9781009588577
Available formats
×

Save element to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Constructing Experience
  • Jason Clarke, University of West London
  • Online ISBN: 9781009588577
Available formats
×