Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T05:10:46.115Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Optimal interval for hot water immersion tail-flick test in rats

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 November 2013

Quanhong Zhou
Affiliation:
Department of Anesthesiology, Shanghai N0.6 People's Hospital, Shanghai Jiaotong University, Shanghai, China
Yuhua Bao
Affiliation:
Department of Anesthesiology, Shanghai N0.6 People's Hospital, Shanghai Jiaotong University, Shanghai, China
Xin Zhang
Affiliation:
Department of Anesthesiology, Shanghai N0.6 People's Hospital, Shanghai Jiaotong University, Shanghai, China
Lulu Zeng
Affiliation:
Department of Anesthesiology, Shanghai N0.6 People's Hospital, Shanghai Jiaotong University, Shanghai, China
Li Wang
Affiliation:
Department of Anesthesiology, Shanghai N0.6 People's Hospital, Shanghai Jiaotong University, Shanghai, China
Jing Wang
Affiliation:
Department of Anesthesiology, Shanghai N0.6 People's Hospital, Shanghai Jiaotong University, Shanghai, China
Wei Jiang*
Affiliation:
Department of Anesthesiology, Shanghai N0.6 People's Hospital, Shanghai Jiaotong University, Shanghai, China
*
Dr. Wei Jiang, Department of Anesthesiology, Shanghai N0.6 People's Hospital, Shanghai Jiaotong University, Shanghai, China. Tel: +0086 21 64369181 58328; Fax: +0086-21-64369181; E-mail: jiangw@sjtu.edu.cn

Abstract

Background

The hot water tail-flick test is widely used to measure the degree of nociception experienced by laboratory animals. This study was carried out to optimise interval times for the hot water immersion tail-flick tests in rats.

Method

Ten different intervals from 10 s to 1 h were tested in 60 Sprague–Dawley male rats. At least eight rats were tested for each interval in three consecutive hot water tail-flick tests. Dixon's up-and-down method was also used to find the optimal intervals. The same rats were then divided into two groups. In Group N, naloxone was injected to reverse the prolonged latency times, whereas saline was used in the control Group S.

Results

Intervals of 10 s, 20 s, 30 min and 1 h did not significantly impact latencies, yielding similar results in three consecutive tests (p > 0.05). However, interval times of between 30 s and 20 min, inclusively, caused significantly prolonged latencies in the second and third tests (p < 0.001). Dixon's up-and-down method showed that 95% of the rats had prolonged latencies in hot water tail-flick tests at intervals longer than 32 s. Naloxone reversed prolonged latencies in Group N, whereas the latencies in Group S were further prolonged in 5 min interval tests.

Conclusion

The optimal intervals for hot water tail-flick tests are either shorter than 20 s or longer than 20 min. The prolonged latencies after repetitive tests were attributable to an endocrine opioid.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Scandinavian College of Neuropsychopharmacology 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

1. D'Amour, FE, Smith, DL. A method for determining loss of pain sensation. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1941;72:7479.Google Scholar
2. Hargreaves, K, Dubner, R, Brown, F, Flores, C, Joris, J. A new and sensitive method for measuring thermal nociception in cutaneous hyperalgesia. Pain 1988;32:7788.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3. Tjolsen, A, Lund, A, Berge, OG, Hole, K. An improved method for tail-flick testing with adjustment for tail-skin temperature. J Neurosci Methods 1989;26:259265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4. Janssen, PA, Niemegeers, CJ, Dony, JG. The inhibitory effect of fentanyl and other morphine-like analgesics on the warm water induced tail withdrawal reflex in rats. Arzneimittelforschung 1963;13:502507.Google Scholar
5. Levine, JD, Murphy, DT, Seidenwurm, D, Cortez, A, Fields, HL. A study of the quantal (all-or-none) change in reflex latency produced by opiate analgesics. Brain Res 1980;201:129141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6. Eide, PK, Berge, OG, Tjolsen, A, Hole, K. Apparent hyperalgesia in the mouse tail-flick test due to increased tail skin temperature after lesioning of serotonergic pathways. Acta Physiol Scand 1988;134:413420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7. Lichtman, AH, Smith, FL, Martin, BR. Evidence that the antinociceptive tail-flick response is produced independently from changes in either tail-skin temperature or core temperature. Pain 1993;55:283295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8. Yoburn, BC, Morales, R, Kelly, DD, Inturrisi, CE. Constraints on the tailflick assay: morphine analgesia and tolerance are dependent upon locus of tail stimulation. Life Sci 1984;34:17551762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9. Bannon, AW, Malmberg, AB. Models of nociception: hot-plate, tail-flick, and formalin tests in rodents. Curr Protoc Neurosci 2007;41:8.9.1-8.9.16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
10. Jung, H, Choi, SC. Sequential method of estimating the LD50 using a modified up-and-down rule. J Biopharm Stat 1994;4:1930.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11. Lal, H, Miksic, S, Smith, N. Naloxone antagonism of conditioned hyperthermia: an evidence for release of endogenous opioid. Life Sci 1976;18:971975.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12. Carstens, E, Wilson, C. Rat tail flick reflex: magnitude measurement of stimulus-response function, suppression by morphine and habituation. J Neurophysiol 1993;70:630639.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13. Le Bars, D, Gozariu, M, Cadden, SW. Animal models of nociception. Pharmacol Rev 2001;53:597652.Google ScholarPubMed
14. Hole, K, Tjolsen, A. The tail-flick and formalin tests in rodents: changes in skin temperature as a confounding factor. Pain 1993;53:247254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15. Carrive, P, Churyukanov, M, Le Bars, D. A reassessment of stress-induced “analgesia” in the rat using an unbiased method. Pain 2011;152:676686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16. Berge, OG, Garcia-Cabrera, I, Hole, K. Response latencies in the tail-flick test depend on tail skin temperature. Neurosci Lett 1988;86:284288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17. Bodnar, RJ, Kelly, DD, Brutus, M, Glusman, M. Stress-induced analgesia: neural and hormonal determinants. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1980;4:87100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18. Fechir, M, Breimhorst, M, Kritzmann, S et al. Naloxone inhibits not only stress-induced analgesia but also sympathetic activation and baroreceptor-reflex sensitivity. Eur J Pain 2012;16:8292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
19. Mogil, JS, Sorge, RE, LaCroix-Fralish, ML et al. Pain sensitivity and vasopressin analgesia are mediated by a gene–sex–environment interaction. Nat Neurosci 2011;14:15691573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed