Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T05:01:37.724Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Perspective taking in Korsakoff's syndrome: the role of executive functioning and task complexity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 June 2014

Joukje M. Oosterman
Affiliation:
Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
Maartje de Goede
Affiliation:
TNO Defence, Security and Safety, Business Unit Human Factors, Soesterberg, The Netherlands
Arie J. Wester
Affiliation:
Korsakoff Clinic, Vincent van Gogh Institute for Psychiatry, Venray, The Netherlands
Martine J. E. van Zandvoort
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Instituut, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands Department of Neurology, University Medical Center, Utrecht, The Netherlands
Roy P. C. Kessels
Affiliation:
Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands

Extract

Oosterman JM, de Goede M, Wester AJ, van Zandvoort MJE, Kessels RPC. Perspective taking in Korsakoff's syndrome: the role of executive functioning and task complexity.

Objective: The ability to make inferences about knowledge, thoughts and feelings of others, i.e. perspective taking, is a key element of social cognition. Clinical observations indicate that Korsakoff patients may have impairments in social cognition, but studies are scarce. Also, executive dysfunction is present in Korsakoff patients, which may hamper perspective taking directly.

Methods: Twenty-three patients with Korsakoff's syndrome and 15 healthy matched controls were examined on a story comprehension task, in which inferences had to be made that either relied on perspective taking or not. The effects of task complexity were taken into account and executive function was assessed using an extensive neuropsychological test battery.

Results: The performance of Korsakoff patients declined with increasing complexity, but the pattern of decline for perspective-taking and non-perspective-taking stories was similar compared to that of the control group. Furthermore, the performance decline with increasing task complexity was directly related to the overall decline in executive functioning.

Conclusion: Executive dysfunction, not deficits in perspective taking per se, appears to underlie difficulties in story comprehension in patients with Korsakoff's syndrome.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

1.Visser, PJ, Krabbendam, L, Verhey, FRJ et al. Brain correlates of memory dysfunction in alcoholic Korsakoff's syndrome. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;67:774778.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2.Reed, LJ, Lasserson, D, Marsden, P et al. FDG-PET findings in the Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome. Cortex 2003;39:10271045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3.Montagne, B, Kessels, RPC, Wester, AJ et al. Processing of emotional facial expressions in Korsakoff's syndrome. Cortex 2006;42:705710.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4.Snits, BE, Hellinger, A, Daum, J.Impaired processing of affective prosody in Korsakoff's syndrome. Cortex 2002;38:797803.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
5.Corcoran, R.Autonoetic awareness, executive social skills, and the appreciation of intention: figurative reasoning in amnesia, confabulation, and schizophrenia. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 1999;4:5580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6.Egger, JIM, Wester, AJ, De Mey, HRA et al. Korsakoff's syndrome on the MMPI-2. Acta Neuropsychiatr 2002;14: 231236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7.Schiffman, J, Lam, CW, Jiwatram, T et al. Perspective-taking deficits in people with schizophrenia spectrum disorders: a prospective investigation. Psychol Med 2004;34:15811586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8.Reed, T.Visual perspective taking as a measure of working memory in participants with autism. J Dev Phys Disabil 2002;14:6376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9.D'Argembeau, A, Ruby, P, Collette, F et al. Distinct regions of the medial prefrontal cortex are associated with self-referential processing and perspective taking. J Cogn Neurosci 2007;19:935944.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10.Samson, D, Apperly, IA, Humphreys, GW.Error analyses reveal contrasting deficits in “theory of mind”: neuropsychological evidence from a 3-option false belief task. Neuropsychologia 2007;45:25612569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11.Uekermann, J, Daum, I.Social cognition in alcoholism: a link to prefrontal cortex dysfunction? Addiction 2008;103:726735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12.Van Oort, R, Kessels, RPC.Executive dysfunction in Korsakoff's syndrome: time to revise the DSM criteria for alcohol-induced persisting amnestic disorder? Int J Psychiatry Clin Pract 2008;63:13441349.Google Scholar
13.Joyce, EM, Robbins, TW.Frontal lobe function in Korsakoff and non-Korsakoff alcoholics: planning and spatial working memory. Neuropsychologia 1991;29:709723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14.Kessels, RPC, Kortrijk, HE, Wester, J et al. Confabulation behavior and false memories in Korsakoff's syndrome: role of source memory and executive functioning. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2008;62:220225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, Text revision, 4th edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 2000.Google Scholar
16.Kopelman, MD.Disorders of memory. Brain 2002;125:21522190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17.Oslin, D, Atkinson, RM, Smith, DM et al. Alcohol related dementia: proposed clinical criteria. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1998;13:203212.3.0.CO;2-B>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18.Wechsler, D.Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd edn. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation, 1997.Google Scholar
19.Luteijn, F, Van der Ploeg, FAE.Groninger intelligentie test (GIT). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger, 1983.Google Scholar
20.Wechsler, D.Manual of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1974.Google Scholar
21.Burgess, PW, Shallice, T.Hayling and Brixton tests. Bury St Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company, 1997.Google Scholar
22.Ward, LC, Ryan, JJ, Axelrod, BN.Confirmatory factor analyses of the WAIS-III standardization data. Psychol Assess 2000;12:341345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23.Happé, FG.An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters' thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J Autism Dev Disord 1994;24:129154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24.Mar, RA.The neuropsychology of narrative: story comprehension, story production and their interrelation. Neuropsychologia 2004;42:14141434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25.Happé, F, Malhi, GS, Checkley, S.Acquired mind-blindness following frontal lobe surgery? A single case study of impaired ‘theory of mind' in a patient treated with stereotactic anterior capsulotomy. Neuropsychologia 2001;39:8390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
26.Rowe, AD, Bullock, PR, Polkey, CE et al. ‘Theory of mind' impairments and their relationship to executive functioning following frontal lobe excisions. Brain 2001;124:600616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27.Couture, SM, Granholm, EL, Fish, SC.A path model investigation of neurocognition, theory of mind, social competence, negative symptoms and real-world functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2011;125:152160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28.Couture, SM, Penn, DL, Roberts, DL.The functional significance of social cognition in schizophrenia: a review. Schizophr Bull 2006;32(Suppl. 1):S44S63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed