Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T14:37:21.623Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Retirement and memory in Europe

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 June 2015

LAURA BIANCHINI
Affiliation:
Department ESOMAS, University of Torino and CeRP (Collegio Carlo Alberto), Italy.
MARGHERITA BORELLA*
Affiliation:
Department ESOMAS, University of Torino and CeRP (Collegio Carlo Alberto), Italy. Netspar, Tilburg, The Netherlands.
*
Address for correspondence: Margherita Borella, Department ESOMAS, University of Torino, Corso Unione Sovietica 218 bis, 10134, Torino, Italy E-mail: margherita.borella@unito.it

Abstract

We investigate the effect of retirement on memory using the Survey on Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE). The availability of a panel data-set allows individual heterogeneity to be controlled for when estimating the effect of transitions into retirement on a commonly employed memory measure, word recall. We control for endogeneity of the retirement decision applying an instrumental variable technique to our fixed-effects transformation. Our main finding is that, conditional on the average non-linear memory age path of the typical individual, time spent in retirement has a positive effect on word recall.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Angrist, J. D. and Pischke, J. S. 2009. Mostly Harmless Econometrics. An Empiricist's Companion. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonsang, E., Adam, S. and Perelman, S. 2012. Does retirement affect cognitive functioning? Journal of Health Economics, 31, 3, 490501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Börsch-Supan, A. and Schuth, M. 2013. Early retirement, mental health and social networks. In Discoveries in the Economics of Aging. Wise, David A. eds, National Bureau of Economic Research. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 225–50.Google Scholar
Boudiny, K. 2013. ‘Active ageing’: from empty rhetoric to effective policy tool. Ageing & Society, 33, 6, 1077–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cattell, R. B. 1963. Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: a critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 1, 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, A. and Warren, L. 2007. Hopes, fears and expectations about the future: what do older people's stories tell us about active ageing? Ageing & Society, 27, 4, 465–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coe, N. B., von Gaudecker, H. M., Lindeboom, M. and Maurer, J. 2012. The effect of retirement on cognitive functioning. Health Economics, 21, 8, 913–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coe, N. B. and Zamarro, G. 2011. Retirement effects of health in Europe. Journal of Health Economics, 30, 1, 7786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Comijs, H. C., Dik, M. G., Aartsen, S. J., Deeg, D. J. H. and Jonker, C. 2005. The impact of change in cognitive functioning and cognitive decline on disability, well-being, and use of healthcare services in older persons. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 19, 5-6, 316–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrer, E., Salthouse, T. A., Stewart, W. F. and Schwartz, B. S. 2004. Modelling age and retest processes in longitudinal studies of cognitive abilities. Psychology and Aging, 19, 2, 243–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, W. H. 2001. Fixed and random effects in nonlinear models. Working Paper EC-01-01, Department of Economics, Stern School of Business, New York University, New York.Google Scholar
Grossman, M. 1972 a. The demand for health: a theoretical and empirical investigation. NBER Occasional Paper 119, National Bureau of Economic Research, New York.Google Scholar
Grossman, M. 1972 b. On the concept of health capital and the demand for health. Journal of Political Economy, 80, 2, 223–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossman, M. 2000. The human capital model. In Culyer, A. J. and Newhouse, J. P. (eds), Handbook of Health Economics. Volume 1, North-Holland, Amsterdam, 347408.Google Scholar
Gruber, J. and Wise, D. 2004. Social Security Programs and Retirement Around the World: Micro-estimation. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
Hertzog, C., Kramer, A. F, Wilson, R. S. and Lindenberger, U. 2008. Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: can the functional capacity of older adults be preserved and enhanced? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 9, 1, 165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hospido, L. and Zamarro, G. 2014. Retirement patterns of couples in Europe. IZA Discussion Paper 7926, Institute for the Study of Labor, Bonn, Germany.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., Small, B. J. and Dixon, R. A. 1999. Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychology and Aging, 14, 2, 245–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leist, A. K., Glymour, M. M., Mackenbach, J. P., van Lenthe, F. J. and Avendano, M. 2013. Time away from work predicts later cognitive function: differences by activity during leave. Annals of Epidemiology, 23, 8, 455–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazzonna, F. and Peracchi, F. 2012. Ageing, cognitive abilities and retirement. European Economic Review, 56, 4, 691710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rohwedder, S. and Willis, R. J. 2010. Mental retirement. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 24, 1, 119–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T. A. 1991. Theoretical Perspectives on Cognitive Aging. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, New Jersey.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. 2006. Mental exercise and mental aging: evaluating the validity of the use it or lose it hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 1, 6887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T. A. 2012. Consequences of age-related cognitive declines. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 201–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarmeas, N. and Stern, Y. 2003. Cognitive reserve and lifestyle. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 5, 625–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaie, K. W. 1996. Intellectual Development in Adulthood. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Sperling, R. A., Aisen, P. S., Beckett, L. A., Bennet, D. A., Craft, S., Fagan, A. M., Iwatsubo, T., Jack, C. R. Jr., Kaye, J., Montine, T. J., Park, D. C., Reiman, E. M., Rowe, C. C., Siemers, E., Stern, Y., Yaffe, K., Carillo, M. C., Thies, B., Morrison-Bogorad, M., Wagster, M. V. and Phelps, C. H. 2011. Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging–Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 7, 3, 280–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulving, E. 1993. What is episodic memory? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 3, 6770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, R. S., Mendes de Leon, C. F., Barnes, L., Schneider, J. A., Bienias, J. L., Evans, D. A., David, A. and Bennett, D. A. 2002. Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. Journal of the American Medical Association, 287, 6, 742–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed