Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T12:24:48.448Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Discordant Twin Pairs: The Relation Between Gestational Age and Neonatal Temperament Differences in Cotwins

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 August 2014

M.L. Riese*
Affiliation:
Louisville Twin Study, Child Development Unit, Health Sciences Center, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY 40292-0001, USA
*
Child Development Unit, Dept Pediatrics, Health Sciences Center, University of Louisville, Louisville, Kentucky 40292, U.S.A.

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Temperament differences between cotwins at least 15% discordant for birth-weight were compared for a group of 66 preterm twin pairs and a group of 70 fullterm twin pairs, assessed in the neonatal period. The assessment focused on irritability, resistance to soothing, activity, reactivity, and reinforcement value. For the most part, significant differences were not obtained between discordant preterm cotwins. Previous research [10] had demonstrated that the larger fullterm twin of discordant pairs was more irritable, more difficult to soothe, more active while awake and during sleep, less reactive to visual and auditory stimuli, and less reinforcing to the examiner than the smaller cotwin. It was concluded that, for the behavioral variables measured, the fullterm group is at higher risk than the preterm group, and that both the lighter and the heavier discordant twins displayed signs of early behavioral risk. The findings suggested that the continued gestation for cotwins discordant in weight may be a risk period for early temperament development of the twins.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The International Society for Twin Studies 1994

References

REFERENCES

1. Babson, SG, Kangas, J, Young, N, et al (1964): Growth and development of twins of dissimilar size at birth. Pediatrics 33:327333.Google Scholar
2. Babson, SG, Phillips, DS (1973): Growth and development of twins dissimilar in size at birth. N Engl J Med 289:937940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3. Churchill, JA (1965): The relationship between intelligence and birthweight in twins. Neurology 15:341347.Google Scholar
4. Field, T, Walden, T, Widmayer, S, et al (1982): The early development of preterm discordant twin pairs: bigger is not always better. In Lipsett, LP, Field, TM (eds): Infant Behavior and Development: Perinatal Risk and Newborn Behavior. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, pp. 153163.Google Scholar
5. Fujikura, T, Fröehlich, LA (1974): Mental and motor development in monozygotic cotwins with dissimilar birthweights. Pediatrics 53:884889.Google Scholar
6. Henrichsen, L, Skinhoj, K, Andersen, GE (1986): Delayed growth and reduced intelligence in 9-17 year-old intrauterine-growth-retarded children compared with their monozygous cotwins. Acta Genet Med Gemellol 75:3135.Google ScholarPubMed
7. Matheny, AP Jr, Brown, AM (1971): The behavior of twins: effects of birthweight and birth sequence. Child Dev 42:251257.Google Scholar
8. O'Brien, P, Hay, DA (1990): Birthweight differences, the transfusion syndrome and the cognitive development of monozygotic twins. Acta Genet Med Gemellol 36:181196.Google Scholar
9. Patterson, RM, Wood, RC (1990): What is twin birthweight discordance? Am J Perinatol 7:217219.Google Scholar
10. Riese, ML (1994): Neonatal temperament in fullterm twin pairs discordant for birthweight. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 15:342347.Google Scholar
11. Stauffer, A, Burns, WJ, Burns, KA, et al (1988): Early developmental progress of preterm twins discordant for birthweight and risk. Acta Genet Med Gemellol 37:8187.Google Scholar
12. Willerman, L, Churchill, JA (1967): Intelligence and birthweight in identical twins. Child Dev 38: 623629.Google Scholar
13. Wilson, RS (1978): Synchronies in mental development: An epigenetic perspective. Science 202: 939948.Google Scholar