Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-29T03:57:40.918Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Speech and Language Development in Preschool Twins

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 August 2014

D.A. Hay*
Affiliation:
Department of Genetics and Human Variation, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Victoria, Australia
M. Prior
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Victoria, Australia
S. Collett
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Victoria, Australia
M. Williams
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Victoria, Australia
*
Department of Psychology, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Victoria 3083, Australia

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

While language, articulation and reading problems have been well documented in young twins, it is not clear how extensive such problems can be or how early in childhood they become evident. At the age of 30 months, twin boys in the La Trobe Twin Study were 8 months behind matched singletons and twin girls on expressive language and 6 months behind on verbal comprehension. They were also 5 months behind on symbolic play and this delay was closely related to language. “Secret” language characterised most of the twin boys but not the girls and the relation of this to articulation delays is discussed. To examine if exposure to other children helps the twin boys, 38-53 month old twins and singletons were matched on the Columbia Mental Maturity Scale at the time of starting preschool. The twin boys had more articulation problems and all twins scored much lower on a Sociability questionnaire completed by the teacher. After 8 months at preschool, all children had advanced in Sociability, but the twins remained just as far behind with poor Sociability relating to poor articulation. The role of intervention programs is discussed.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The International Society for Twin Studies 1987

References

REFERENCES

1.Bornstein, MH (1985): How infant and mother jointly contribute to developing cognitive competence in the child. Proc Nat Acd Sci USA 82:74707473.Google Scholar
2.Burlingham, D (1952): Twins: Study of Three Pairs of Identical Twins. London: Imago Press.Google Scholar
3.Conway, D, Lytton, H, Pysh, F (1980): Twin-singleton language differences. Canad J Behav Sci Rev Canad Sci Comp 12:264271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
4.Costello, AJ (1978): Deprivation and family structure with particular reference to twins. In Anthony, EJ, and Coupernick, C (eds): The Child in His Family, Vol 4. New York: Wiley, pp 5971.Google Scholar
5.Day, EJ (1932): The development of language by twins: I. A comparison of twins and single children. Child Dev 3: 179194.Google Scholar
6.Doll, EA (1965): Vineland Social Maturity Scale: Condensed Manual of Directions. Minnesota: American Guidance Service Inc.Google Scholar
7.Douglas, JE, Sutton, A (1978): The development of speech and mental processes in a pair of twins: a case study. J Child Psychol and Psychiat 19:4956.Google Scholar
8.Hay, DA, O'Brien, PJ (1983): The La Trobe Twin Study: a genetic approach to the structure and development of cognition in twin children. Child Dev 54:317330.Google Scholar
9.Hay, DA, Collett, SM, Johnston, CJ, O'Brien, PJ, Prior, M (1986): Do twins and singletons have similar language and reading problems? In Pratt, C, Garton, AF, Turner, WE, Nesdale, AR (eds) Research Issues in Child Development. Sydney: Allen and Unwin, pp 125134.Google Scholar
10.Hay, DA, O'Brien, PJ, Johnston, CJ, Prior, M (1984): The high incidence of reading disability in twin boys and its implications for genetic analyses. Acta Genet Med Gemellol 33:223236.Google ScholarPubMed
11.Johnston, CJ, Prior, M, Hay, DA (1984): Prediction of reading disability in twin boys. Dev Med Child Neurol 26:588595.Google Scholar
12.Kim, CC, Dales, RJ, Connor, R, Walter, J, Witherspoon, R (1969): Social interaction of like-sex twins and singletons in relation to intelligence, language and physical development. J Genet Psychol 144:203214.Google Scholar
13.Koch, HL (1966): Twin and Twin Relations. Chicago: Univ of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
14.Lowe, M (1975): Trends in the development of representational play in infants from one to three years - An observational study. J Child Psychol and Psychiat 16:3347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15.Lowe, M, Costello, AJ (1976): Manual for the Symbolic Play Test. London: NFER - Nelson.Google Scholar
16.Luria, AR, Yudovich, FI (1959): Speech and the Development of Mental Processes in the Child. London: Staples Press.Google Scholar
17.McDairmid, J McK, Silva, PA (1979): Three year old twins and singletons: a comparison of some perinatal, environmental, experiential and developmental characteristics. Austral Paediatr J 15:243247.Google Scholar
18.Piaget, J (1952): Play, Dreams and Imitation in Childhood. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
19.Roper, R, Hinde, RA (1979): Teachers' questionnaire for individual differences in social behaviour. J Child Psychol 20:287298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20.Savic, S (1980): How Twin Learn to Talk: A Study of the Speech Development of Twins from 1 to 3. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
21.Tomasello, M, Mannie, S, Kruger, AC (1986): Linguistic environment of 1- to 2-year old twins. Develop Psychol 22:169176.Google Scholar
22.Watts, D, Lytton, H (1981): Twinship as handicap: fact or fiction? In Gedda, L, Parisi, P, Nance, WE (eds): Twin Reseach: Part A, Psychology and Methodology. New York: Alan R Liss, pp 5762.Google Scholar
23.Wilson, RS (1975): Twins: Patterns of cognitive development as measured on the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence. Develop Psychol 11:126134.Google Scholar
24.Zazzo, R (1960): Les Jumeaux: Le Couple et la Personne. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar