Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T08:34:32.513Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Behavioural diversity as a potential welfare indicator for professionally managed chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Exploring variations in calculating diversity using species-specific behaviours

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

K Hall*
Affiliation:
Sedgwick County Zoo, 5555 W Zoo Boulevard, Wichita, KS 67212, USA Chicago Zoological Society - Brookfield Zoo, 3300 Golf Road, Brookfield, IL 60513, USA
J Bryant
Affiliation:
Chicago Zoological Society - Brookfield Zoo, 3300 Golf Road, Brookfield, IL 60513, USA
M Staley
Affiliation:
Chicago Zoological Society - Brookfield Zoo, 3300 Golf Road, Brookfield, IL 60513, USA Department of Biology, Loyola University Chicago, Chicago, IL 60660-1537, USA
JC Whitham
Affiliation:
Chicago Zoological Society - Brookfield Zoo, 3300 Golf Road, Brookfield, IL 60513, USA
LJ Miller
Affiliation:
Chicago Zoological Society - Brookfield Zoo, 3300 Golf Road, Brookfield, IL 60513, USA
*
* Contact for correspondence: KatieHallPhD@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Behavioural diversity may serve as a positive indicator of animal welfare that can be applied in long-term monitoring schemes in managed settings (eg zoos, laboratories, farms). Behavioural diversity is often higher when animals live in stimulating environments and experience positive events. Unfortunately, welfare researchers have not adopted consistent, standardised approaches to measuring behavioural diversity. The goal of this exploratory study was to utilise data from 41 adult chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) housed across 16 zoological institutions to examine various models of Shannon's Diversity Index. Specifically, we investigated the impact of: combining versus splitting behaviours, including only positive behaviours, including human interaction, and considering recipient behaviours. We evaluate how the inclusion or exclusion of different behaviours impacts the relationship of behavioural diversity with: (i) concentrations of faecal glucocorticoid metabolites (GCM), a common indicator of adrenal activity; (ii) concentrations of immunoglobulin-A (IgA), an indicator of immune function and potential indicator of positive welfare; and (iii) stereotypic behaviour, a validated indicator of poor welfare. Most indices had significant negative relationships with faecal GCM. Animals that express a variety of behaviours from their species-typical repertoire have lower average faecal GCM concentrations and are likely experiencing better welfare. We did not find significant relationships between the behavioural diversity indices and IgA concentrations. Two indices were inversely associated with stereotypic behaviour. Our findings provide additional support for using Shannon's Diversity Index to calculate behavioural diversity as a robust, valid measure of positive welfare. However, future publications must justify the process for including or excluding behaviours from calculations.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2021 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

References

Abbott, DH, Keverne, EB, Bercovitch, FB, Shively, CA, Mendoza, SP, Saltzman, W, Snowdon, CT, Ziegler, TE, Banjevic, M, Garland, T Jr and Sapolsky, RM 2003 Are subor-dinates always stressed? A comparative analysis of rank differences in cortisol levels among primates. Hormones and Behavior 43(1): 6782. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0018-506x(02)00037-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, KC 1997 Human interaction as enrichment for captive chimpanzees: A preliminary report. American Journal of Primatology 42(2): 92Google Scholar
Bashaw, MJ, Sicks, F, Palme, R, Schwarzenberger, F, Tordiffe, AS and Ganswindt, A 2016 Non-invasive assessment of adreno-cortical activity as a measure of stress in giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis). BMC Veterinary Research 12(1): 235. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12917-016-0864-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayne, K 2002 Development of the human-research animal bond and its impact on animal well-being. ILAR Journal 43(1): 49. https://doi.org/10.1093/ilar.43.1.4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, NC and Gleeson, M 2009 Acute and chronic effects of exercise on markers of mucosal immunity. Frontiers in Bioscience 14: 44444456. https://doi.org/10.2741/3540CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blecha, F 2000 The Biology of Animal Stress pp 111122. CABI: Wallingford, UKCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloomsmith, MA, Lambeth, SP, Stone, AM and Laule, GE 1997 Comparing two types of human interaction as enrichment for chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology 42(2): 96Google Scholar
Boinski, S, Swing, SP, Gross, TS and Davis, JK 1999 Environmental enrichment of brown capuchins (Cebus apella): Behavioral and plasma and fecal cortisol measures of effectiveness. American Journal of Primatology 48(1): 4968. https://doi.org/10.1002/(sici)1098-2345(1999)48:1<49::aid-ajp4>3.0.co;2-63.0.CO;2-6>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butterworth, A, Mench, J and Wielebnowski, N 2011 Practical strategies to assess (and improve) welfare. In: Appleby, MC, Mench, JA and Olsson, IAS and Hughes, BO (eds) Animal Welfare pp 200214. CABI: Wallingford, UK. https://doi.org/10.1079/9781845936594.0200CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlstead, K and Brown, JL 2005 Relationships between pat-terns of fecal corticoid excretion and behavior, reproduction, and environmental factors in captive black (Diceros bicornis) and white (Ceratotherium simum) rhinoceros. Zoo Biology 24(3): 215232. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.20050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlstead, K, Brown, JL and Seidensticker, J 1993 Behavioral and adrenocortical responses to environmental changes in leop-ard cats (Felis bengalensis). Zoo Biology 12(4): 321331. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.1430120403CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlstead, K, Paris, S and Brown, JL 2019 Good keeper-ele-phant relationships in North American zoos are mutually benefi-cial to welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 211: 103111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.11.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chinnadurai, SK, Millspaugh, JJ, Matthews, WS, Canter, K, Slotow, R, Washburn, BE and Woods, RJ 2009 Validation of fecal glucocorticoid metabolite assays for South African herbi-vores. The Journal of Wildlife Management 73(6): 10141020. https://doi.org/10.2193/2008-430CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Claxton, AM 2011 The potential of the human-animal relation-ship as an environmental enrichment for the welfare of zoo-housed animals. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 133(1-2): 110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2011.03.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J 1988 Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition. Erlbaum: Hillsdale, USAGoogle Scholar
Crockford, C, Wittig, RM, Langergraber, K, Ziegler, TE, Zuberbühler, K and Deschner, T 2013 Urinary oxytocin and social bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 280: 20122765. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.2765CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cronin, KA and Ross, SR 2019 Technical contribution: A cautionary note on the use of behavioural diversity (H-Index) in ani-mal welfare science. Animal Welfare 28: 157164. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.28.2.157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, IJ and Petherick, JC 1991 The implications of cognitive processes for animal welfare. Journal of Animal Science 69: 50175022. https://doi.org/10.2527/1991.69125017xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elsasser, TH, Klasing, KC, Filipov, N and Thompson, F 2000 The Biology of Animal Stress pp 77110. CABI: Wallingford, UKCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eriksson, E, Royo, F, Lyberg, K, Carlsson, HE and Hau, J 2004 Effect of metabolic cage housing on immunoglobulin A and corti-costerone excretion in faeces and urine of young male rats. Experimental Physiology 89: 427433. https://doi.org/10.1113/exp-physiol.2004.027656CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraser, D and Duncan, IJH 1998 ‘Pleasures’, ‘pains’ and animal welfare: Toward a natural history of affect. Animal Welfare 7: 383396Google Scholar
Friard, O and Gamba, M 2016 BORIS: a free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 7: 13251330 doi:10.1111/2041-210X.12584CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fureix, C and Meagher, RK 2015 What can inactivity (in its var-ious forms) reveal about affective states in non-human animals? A review. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 171: 824. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2015.08.036CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fureix, C, Walker, M, Harper, L, Reynolds, K, Saldivia-Woo, A and Mason, G 2016 Stereotypic behaviour in standard non-enriched cages is an alternative to depression-like responses in C57BL/6 mice. Behavioural Brain Research 305: 186190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2016.02.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gourkow, N, Hamon, SC and Phillips, CJ 2014 Effect of gentle stroking and vocalization on behavior, mucosal immunity and upper respiratory disease in anxious shelter cats. Preventative Veterinary Medicine 117: 266275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pre-vetmed.2014.06.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gourkow, N and Phillips, CJ 2015 Effect of interactions with humans on behavior, mucosal immunity and upper respiratory dis-ease of shelter cats rated as contented on arrival. Preventative Veterinary Medicine 121: 288296. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pre-vetmed.2015.07.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gourkow, N and Phillips, CJ 2016 Effect of cognitive enrich-ment on behavior, mucosal immunity and upper respiratory dis-ease of shelter cats rated as frustrated on arrival. Preventative Veterinary Medicine 131: 103110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pre-vetmed.2016.07.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greco, BJ, Meehan, CL, Hogan, JN, Leighty, KA, Mellen, J, Mason, GJ and Mench, JA 2016 The days and nights of zoo ele-phants: using epidemiology to better understand stereotypic behavior of African elephants (Loxodonta africana) and Asian ele-phants (Elephas maximus) in North American zoos. PLoS One 11: e0144276. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0144276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guhad, FA and Hau, J 1996 Salivary IgA as a marker of social stress in rats. Neuroscience Letters 216: 137140. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3940(96)13037-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunn, D and Morton, DB 1995 Inventory of the behaviour of New Zealand White rabbits in laboratory cages. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 45: 277292. https://doi.org/10.1016/0168-1591(95)00627-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heintz, MR, Santymire, RM, Parr, LA and Lonsdorf, EV 2011 Validation of a cortisol enzyme immunoassay and characterization of salivary cortisol circadian rhythm in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Primatology 73: 903908. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20960CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Held, SD and Špinka, M 2011 Animal play and animal welfare. Animal Behaviour 81: 891899. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.01.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hosey, G 2008 A preliminary model of human-animal relation-ships in the zoo. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 109: 105127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2007.04.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hosey, G and Melfi, V 2012 Human-animal bonds between zoo professionals and the animals in their care. Zoo Biology 31: 1326. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.20359CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hosey, G and Melfi, V 2014 Human-animal interactions, relation-ships and bonds: a review and analysis of the literature. International Journal of Comparative Psychology 27: 117142. https://doi.org/10.46867/ijcp.2014.27.01.01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kalthoff, A, Schmidt, C and Sachser, N 2001 Influence of zoo visitors on behaviour and salivary corticosterone concentrations of zoo animals. KTBL SCHRIFT 403: 104112Google Scholar
Kowalski, J and Tu, XM 2008 Modern Applied U-statistics, Volume 714. John Wiley and Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USAGoogle Scholar
Lane, J 2006 Can non-invasive glucocorticoid measures be used as reliable indicators of stress in animals? Animal Welfare 15: 331342Google Scholar
Langergraber, KE, Mitani, JC and Vigilant, L 2009 Kinship and social bonds in female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Primatology 71: 840851. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20711CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lantz, EL, Lonsdorf, EV, Heintz, MR, Murray, CM, Lipende, I, Travis, DA and Santymire, RM 2016 Non-invasive quantification of immunoglobulin A in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schwein-furthii) at Gombe National Park, Tanzania. American Journal of Primatology 80: e22558. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22558CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehmann, J and Boesch, B 2009 Sociality of the dispersing sex: the nature of social bonds in West African female chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Animal Behavior 77: 377387. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.09.038CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, G 2006 Stereotypic behaviour in captive animals: Fundamentals and implications for welfare and beyond. In: Mason, G and Rushen, J (eds) Stereotypic Animal Behaviour: Fundamentals and Applications to Welfare pp 325367. CAB International: Wallingford, UK. https://doi.org/10.1079/9780851990040.0325CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, GJ, Cooper, J and Clarebrough, C 2001 Frustrations of fur-farmed mink. Nature 410: 35. https://doi.org/10.1038/35065157CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mason, GJ and Latham, NR 2004 Can't stop, won't stop: is stereo-typy a reliable animal welfare indicator? Animal Welfare 13: 5769Google Scholar
Meagher, RK, Campbell, DL and Mason, GJ 2017 Boredom-like states in mink and their behavioural correlates: a replicate study. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 197: 112119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2017.08.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meagher, RK and Mason, GJ 2012 Environmental enrichment reduces signs of boredom in caged mink. PLoS One 7(11): e49180. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0049180CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melfi, V 2013 Is training zoo animals enriching? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 147: 299305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2013.04.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellen, JD 1991 Factors influencing reproductive success in small captive exotic felids (Felis spp): a multiple regression analysis. Zoo Biology 10: 95110. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.1430100202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, LJ, Mellen, J, Greer, T and Kuczaj, SA 2011 The effects of education programmes on Atlantic bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) behaviour. Animal Welfare 20: 159172Google Scholar
Miller, LJ, Pisacane, CB and Vicino, GA 2016 Relationship between behavioural diversity and faecal glucocorticoid metabo-lites: a case study with cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus). Animal Welfare 25: 325329. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.25.3.325CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, LJ, Vicino, GA, Sheftel, J and Lauderdale, LK 2020 Behavior diversity as a potential indicator of positive animal wel-fare. Animals 10: 1211. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10071211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, JC 2009 Male chimpanzees form enduring and equitable social bonds. Animal Behavior 77: 633640. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.11.021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moberg, GP 1987 Influence of the adrenal axis upon the gonads. Oxford Reviews of Reproductive Biology 9: 456Google ScholarPubMed
Moberg, GP and Mench, JA 2000 The Biology of Animal Stress: Basic Principles and Implications for Animal Welfare. CABI: Wallingford, UK. https://doi.org/10.1079/9780851993591.0000CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mormède, P, Andanson, S, Aupérin, B, Beerda, B, Guémené, D, Malmkvist, J, Manteca, X, Manteuffel, G, Prunet, P, van Reenen, CG, Richard, S and Veissier, I 2007 Exploration of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal function as a tool to evaluate ani-mal welfare. Physiology and Behavior 92: 317339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.12.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, CM, Heintz, MR, Lonsdorf, EV, Parr, LA and Santymire, RM 2013 Validation of a field technique and charac-terization of fecal glucocorticoid metabolite analysis in wild chim-panzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Primatology 75: 5764. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22078CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palme, R, Touma, C, Arias, N, Dominchin, MF and Lepschy, M 2013 Steroid extraction: getting the best out of faecal samples. Wien Tierarztl Monatsschr 100: 238246Google Scholar
Pressman, SD and Cohen, S 2005 Does positive affect influence health? Psychological Bulletin 131: 925. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.925CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Razal, CB, Pisacane, CB and Miller, LJ 2016 Multifaceted approach to personality assessment in cheetahs (Acinonyx juba-tus). Animal Behavior and Cognition 3: 2231. https://doi.org/10.12966/abc.02.02.2016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Renner, MJ and Lussier, JP 2002 Environmental enrichment for the captive spectacled bear (Tremarctos ornatus). Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 73: 279283. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0091-3057(02)00786-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rivier, C 1995 Luteinizing-hormone-releasing hormone, gonadotropins, and gonadal steroids in stress. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 771: 187191. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb44680.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, SR and Lukas, KE 2001 The care and management of captive chimpanzees workshop: Managing social behavior. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 4: 299301. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0404_07CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rushen, J, Lawrence, AB and Terlouw, EMC 1993 The motivational basis of stereotypies. In: Lawrence, A and Rushen, J (eds) Stereotypic Animal Behaviour pp 4164. CABI: Wallingford, UKGoogle Scholar
Sapolsky, RM 2002 Endocrinology of the stress-response. In: Becker, J, Breedlove, S, Crews, D and McCarthy, M (eds) Behavioral Endocrinology, Second Edition pp 409450. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USAGoogle Scholar
Sarrafchi, A and Blokhuis, HJ 2013 Equine stereotypic behav-iors: causation, occurrence, and prevention. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 8: 386394. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2013.04.068CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schatz, S and Palme, R 2001 Measurement of faecal cortisol metabolites in cats and dogs: a non-invasive method for evaluating adrenocortical function. Veterinary Research Communications 25: 271287CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shannon, CE and Weaver, W 1949 The mathematical theory of communication. University of Illinois Press I, 11: 117Google Scholar
Shepherdson, D, Lewis, KD, Carlstead, K, Bauman, J and Perrin, N 2013 Individual and environmental factors associated with stereotypic behavior and fecal glucocorticoid metabolite lev-els in zoo housed polar bears. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 147: 268277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2013.01.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepherdson, DJ, Carlstead, K, Mellen, JD and Seidensticker, J 1993 The influence of food presentation on the behavior of small cats in confined environments. Zoo Biology 12: 203216. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.1430120206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegford, JM 2013 Multidisciplinary approaches and assessment techniques to better understand and enhance zoo nonhuman ani-mal welfare. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 16: 300318. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2013.827914CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simpson, EH 1949 Measurement of diversity. Nature 163: 688CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skandakumar, S, Stodulski, G and Hau, J 1995 Salivary IgA: a possible stress marker in dogs. Animal Welfare 4: 339350.Google Scholar
Staley, M, Conners, MG, Hall, K and Miller, LJ 2018 Linking stress and immunity: Immunoglobulin A as a non-invasive physio-logical biomarker in animal welfare studies. Hormones and Behavior 102: 5568. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2018.04.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stolba, A, Wood-Gush, DG and Baker, N 1983 The characterisation of stereotyped behaviour in stalled sows by informational redundancy. Behaviour 87: 157182. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853983x00417CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swaisgood, R and Shepherdson, D 2006 Environmental enrich-ment as a strategy for mitigating stereotypies in zoo animals: a lit-erature review and meta-analysis. In: Mason G and Rushen J (eds) Stereotypic Animal Behaviour: Fundamentals and Applications to Welfare, Second Edition pp 256285. CABI: Wallingford, UK. https://doi.org/10.1079/9780851990040.0256CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swaisgood, RR, White, AM, Zhou, X, Zhang, G and Lindburg, DG 2005 How do giant pandas (Ailuropoda melanoleuca) respond to varying properties of enrichments? A comparison of behavioral profiles among five enrichment items. Journal of Comparative Psychology 119: 325. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.119.3.325CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swaisgood, RR, White, AM, Zhou, X, Zhang, H, Zhang, G, Wei, R, Hare, VJ, Tepper, EM and Lindburg, DG 2001 A quantitative assessment of the efficacy of an environmental enrichment pro-gramme for giant pandas. Animal Behaviour 61: 447457. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2000.1610CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tang, W, He, H and Tu, X 2012 Applied Categorical and Count Data Analysis. Chapman and Hall/CRC: Boca Raton, FL, USACrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terio, KS and Munson, L 2000 Gastritis in cheetahs and related-ness to adrenal function. Felid Taxon Advisory Group Action Plan. American Zoo and Aquarium Association: Wheeling:, Virginia, USAGoogle Scholar
Tsujita, S and Morimoto, K 1999 Secretory IgA in saliva can be a useful stress marker. Environmental Health and Preventive Medicine 4: 18. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02931243CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
VanderSchuren, LJMJ, Niesink, RJM and van Ree, JM 1997 The neurobiology of social play behavior in rats. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 21: 309326. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0149-7634(96)00020-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vickery, S and Mason, G 2004 Stereotypic behavior in Asiatic black and Malayan sun bears. Zoo Biology 23: 409430. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.20027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waitt, C, Buchanan-Smith, HM and Morris, K 2002 The effects of caretaker-primate relationships on primates in the labo-ratory. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 5: 309319. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0504_05CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watanuki, S and Kim, YK 2005 Physiological responses induced by pleasant stimuli. Journal of Physiological Anthropology and Applied Human Science 24: 135138. https://doi.org/10.2114/jpa.24.135CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wemelsfelder, F, Haskell, M, Mendl, MT, Calvert, S and Lawrence, AB 2000 Diversity of behaviour during novel object tests is reduced in pigs housed in substrate-impoverished conditions. Animal Behaviour 60: 385394. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2000.1466CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitham, JC and Wielebnowski, N 2013 New directions for zoo animal welfare science. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 147: 247260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2013.02.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wielebnowski, NC, Fletchall, N, Carlstead, K, Busso, JM and Brown, JL 2002 Non-invasive assessment of adrenal activity asso-ciated with husbandry and behavioral factors in the North American clouded leopard population. Zoo Biology 21: 7798. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.10005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Würbel, H, Stauffacher, M and von Holst, D 1996 Stereotypies in laboratory mice: Quantitative and qualitative description of the ontogeny of ‘wire-gnawing'and ‘jumping'in Zur: ICR and Zur: ICR nu. Ethology 102: 371385. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1996.tb01133.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Hall et al. supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 121.7 KB
Supplementary material: File

Hall et al. supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 291.6 KB