Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T04:21:39.606Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Causes of Body Rocking in Chimpanzees (Pan Troglodytes)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2023

R P Spijkerman*
Affiliation:
Primate Centre TNO, Rijswijk, The Netherlands Present address: Azoren 1, 3524 ET, Utrecht, The Netherlands
H Dienske
Affiliation:
Primate Centre TNO, Rijswijk, The Netherlands
J A R A M van Hooff
Affiliation:
Primate Centre TNO, Rijswijk, The Netherlands Ethology and Socio-ecology, Rijksuniversiteit Utrecht, The Netherlands
W Jens
Affiliation:
Primate Centre TNO, Rijswijk, The Netherlands
*
Contact for correspondence and requests for reprints
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

The behavioural development of 90 chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) was followed. Of these, 65 had been separated from their mothers to prevent casualties or, at a later age, to increase breeding success. Some showed body rocking and others did not. To obtain insight into the causes of the onset and development of body rocking, chimpanzees raised with peers, with their mother, or in a semi-natural group were compared Rocking was never observed in the semi-natural group. It was occasionally seen when with the mother. Separation from the mother soon after birth induced anxiety and rocking developed after sitting upright had developed. Rocking levels of three per cent of the time were still present at seven to nine years of age.

The most probable causes of the development of rocking are frustrating social circumstances and the inability to cope with these. Merging groups, disturbances and the introduction of a fearful object increased rocking in the individuals that had developed the habit. However, rockers reacted less to those circumstances than non-rockers, showing smaller increase in body contact and less reduction of play. This finding suggests that rocking, instead of being a bizarre reaction to unsurmountable stress, could be a behaviour that helps an individual to cope with difficulties and stress. In terms of animal welfare, rocking in chimpanzees housed in laboratory conditions is an indication of a less optimal reaction pattern to frustrating circumstances. The development of rocking may be prevented if the babies are left with the mother and in their social group. Rocking after (late) separation may be prevented when transfer takes place together with familiar peers.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 1994 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

References

Adang, O M J 1986 Teasing, Harassment, Provocation: The Development of Quasiaggressive Behaviour in Chimpanzees. PhD Thesis, Utrecht UniversityGoogle Scholar
Adang 0 M J, Wensing JAB and Hooff JARAM van 1987 The Arnhem Zoo colony of chimpanzees Pan troglodytes: development and management techniques. International Zoo Yearbook 26: 236248Google Scholar
Baumeister, A A and Forehand, R 1973 Stereotyped acts. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation 6: 5596CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berkson, G 1967 Abnormal stereotyped motor acts. In Zubin, J and Hunt, H F (eds) Comparative Psychobiology: Animal and Human pp 7694. Grune & Stratton: New YorkGoogle Scholar
Berkson, G 1968 Development of abnormal stereotyped behaviors. Developmental Psychobiology 1: 118132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berkson, G and Mason, W A 1964 Stereotyped behaviors of chimpanzees: relation to general arousal activities. Perceptual and Motor Skills 19: 635652CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berkson, G, Mason, W A and Saxon, S V 1963 Situation and stimulus effects on stereotyped behaviors of chimpanzees. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology 56: 789792CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brody, S 1960 Self-rocking in infancy. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association 8: 464–491CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Capitanio, J P 1986 Behavioral pathology. In Michell, G and Erwin, J (eds) Comparative Primate Biology: 2A Behavior, Conservation and Ecology pp 411454. Alan R Liss: New YorkGoogle Scholar
Cooper, J J and Nicol, C J 1991 Stereotypic behaviour affects environmental preference in bank voles, Clethrionomys glareolus. Animal Behaviour 41: 971977CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davenport, R K 1979 Some behavioral disturbances of great apes in captivity. In Hamburg, D A and McCown, E R (eds) The Great Apes: Perspectives on Human Evolution, Volume V pp 341357. The Benjamin/Cummings Publishing Company: Menlo Park, CaliforniaGoogle Scholar
Davenport, R K and Menzel, E W 1963 Stereotyped behavior of the infant chimpanzee. Archives of General Psychiatry 8: 99104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davenport, R K and Rogers, C M 1970 Differential rearing of the chimpanzee, a project survey. In Bourne, G H (ed) The Chimpanzee 3 pp 337360. S Karger: BaselGoogle Scholar
Dienske, H and Griffin, R 1978 Abnormal behaviour patterns developing in chimpanzee infants during nursery care - a note. Journal Child Psychology and Psychiatry 19: 387391CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dienske H and Vreeswijk W van 1987 Regulation of nursing in chimpanzees. Developmental Psychobiology 20: 7183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T, Ting, G and Shuman, H H 1979 The onset of rhythmic activities in normal and high risk infants. Developmental Psychobiology 12: 97100CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forrester, R 1980 Stereotypies and the behavioural regulation of motivational state. Applied Animal Ethology 6: 386387 (Abstract)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fritz, J and Fritz, P 1985 The hand-rearing unit: management decisions that may affect chimpanzee development. In Graham, C E and Bowen, J A (eds) Monographs in Primatology, Volume 5: Clinical Management of Infant Great Apes pp 134. Alan R Liss Inc: New YorkGoogle Scholar
Fritz, J, Fritz, P, Alford, P L and Bowan, J A 1992 Abnormal behaviors, with a special focus on rocking and reproduction competence in a large sample of captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Primatology 27: 161176CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodall, J 1971 Some aspects of mother-infant relationships in a group of wild chimpanzees. In H R Schaffer (ed) The Origins of Human Social Relations pp 115128. Academic Press: LondonGoogle Scholar
Goosen, C 1981 Abnormal behavior patterns in rhesus monkeys: symptoms of mental disease? Biological Psychiatry 16: 697716Google ScholarPubMed
Goosen C, Schrama A, Brinkhof H, Schonk J and Hoek L A van 1983 Housing conditions and breeding success of chimpanzees at the Primate Center TNO. Zoo Biology 2: 295302CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hooff JARAM van 1973 A structural analysis of the social behaviour of a semi captive group of chimpanzees. In Cranach, M and Vine, I (eds) Social Communication and Movement pp 75162. Academic Press: LondonGoogle Scholar
Kellogg, W N and Kellogg, L A 1933 The Ape and the Child: A Study of Environmental Influences upon Early Behaviour. McGraw-Hill: New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kravitz, H, Rozenthal, V, Teplitz, Z, Murphy, J B and Lesser, R E 1960 Head-banging in infants and children. Diseases of the Nervous Systems 21: 203208Google Scholar
Mason, G J 1991 Stereotypies: a critical review. Animal Behaviour 41: 10151037CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, W A 1968 Early social deprivation in the nonhuman primates: implications for human behavior. In Glass, D C (ed) Environmental Influences pp 70101. The Rockerfeller University Press: New YorkGoogle Scholar
Mason, W A and Berkson, G 1975 Effects of maternal mobility on the development of rocking and other behaviors in rhesus monkeys: a study with artificial mothers. Developmental Psychobiology 8: 197211CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meder, A 1989 Effects of hand-rearing on the behavioral development of infant and juvenile gorillas (Gorilla gorilla). Developmental Psychobiology 22: 357376CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paulk, H H and Dienske, H 1977 Abnormal behavior in relation to cage size in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 81: 8792CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pazol, K A and Bloomsmith, M A 1993 The development of stereotyped body rocking in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) reared in a variety of nursery settings. Animal Welfare 2: 113129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plooij, F X 1979 How wild chimpanzee babies trigger the onset of mother-infant play and what the mother makes of it. In Bullow, M (ed) Before Speech: The Beginnings of Human Communication. Cambridge University Press: LondonGoogle Scholar
Plooij, F X 1984 The Behavioral Development of Free-living Chimpanzee Babies and Infants. Ablex Publishing Company: Norwood, New JerseyGoogle Scholar
Riesen, A H and Kinder, E F 1952 Postural Development of Infant Chimpanzees. Yale University Press: New HavenGoogle Scholar
Sallustro, F and Atwell, C W 1978 Body rocking, head banging, and head rolling in normal children. The Journal of Pediatrics 93: 704708CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegel, S 1956 Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences. McGraw-Hill Inc: New YorkGoogle Scholar
Spijkerman, R P 1987 Behaviour development of infant chimpanzees with the mother and in peer groups. Annual Report, Division of Health Research TNO Primate Center: 309312Google Scholar
Spijkerman, R P, Dienske, H, Hooff, JARAM van and Jens, W (in prep) Differences in behaviour development between young chimpanzees living with peer-only and those living in a semi-natural group.Google Scholar
Spijkerman, R P, Hooff, JARAM van and Jens, W 1990 A case of lethal infant abuse in an established group of chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica 55: 4144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wemelsfelder, F 1990 Boredom and laboratory animal welfare. In Rollin, B E (ed) The Experimental Animal in Biomedical Research pp 243272. CRC Press: Boca Raton, USAGoogle Scholar