Introduction
In order to promote the expression of species-specific behaviours and improve welfare in farmed furbearers such as American mink (Neogale vison), environmental enrichment (EE; defined in welfare science as changes or additions to animals’ environments or husbandry that are biologically relevant to the species; Newberry Reference Newberry1995) is often incorporated in pens. As per the most recent mink farming guidelines in Canada (National Farm Animal Care Council 2013) and the United States (Fur Commission USA 2019), one manipulable EE must be provided in each pen; mink in Europe were required to be housed with EE from 2008 onwards, although records show that 88% of mink in Norway were already provided with ‘activity objects’ by 2001 (European Commission 2001). The inclusion of manipulable EEs in mink pens has been found to prevent or reduce stereotypic behaviour, physiological stress, self-harm behaviours like tail chewing, and negative affective states such as fear or boredom (e.g. Hansen & Jeppesen Reference Hansen and Jeppesen2000; Hansen et al. Reference Hansen, Malmkvist, Palme and Damgaard2007; Dallaire et al. Reference Dallaire, Meagher, Díez-León, Garner and Mason2011; Meagher & Mason Reference Meagher and Mason2012; Díez-León et al. Reference Díez-León, Bowman, Bursian, Filion, Galicia, Kanefsky, Napolitano, Palme, Schulte-Hostedde, Scribner and Mason2013, Reference Díez-León, Bursian, Galicia, Napolitano, Palme and Mason2016; Meagher et al. Reference Meagher, Dallaire, Campbell, Ross, Møller, Hansen, Díez-León, Palme and Mason2014). Manipulable EEs may also have applications for modulating mink temperament: enriched mink have shown reduced fear and aggression in temperament tests and handling scenarios, sometimes paired with increases in explorative or curious responding (Meagher et al. Reference Meagher, Dallaire, Campbell, Ross, Møller, Hansen, Díez-León, Palme and Mason2014; Bak & Malmkvist Reference Bak and Malmkvist2020).
The EE provided is typically a mobile object within the pen that mink can chase and chew, which creates an outlet for innate behaviours (e.g. hunting) that are otherwise restricted in captivity and reduces underlying behavioural frustrations (Maple & Perdue Reference Maple and Perdue2013). The object(s) may target stereotypic behaviour by reducing stress or arousal arising from aversive environments, offering greater opportunity to exert control over the environment, and/or by occupying the animal’s time with other behaviours (Mason et al. Reference Mason, Clubb, Latham and Vickery2007). Stereotypic behaviour may alternatively arise from central nervous system dysfunction or neuroanatomical changes occurring after time spent in captivity, namely in reward-sensitive areas or areas responsible for inhibiting repetitive, habit-like behaviours, in which case EE can be used to protect against these changes (Díez-León et al. Reference Díez-León, Kitchenham, Duprey, Bailey, Choleris, Lewis and Mason2019; Tatemoto et al. Reference Tatemoto, Broom and Zanella2022).
The ontogeny of stereotypic behaviours in captive animals remains poorly understood, but they are estimated to become fully developed by seven months of age in mink (Jeppesen et al. Reference Jeppesen, Heller and Dalsgaard2000). EE is relatively unsuccessful at reducing stereotypic behaviour if provided after animals have reached maturity (Mason Reference Mason1993; Ahola et al. Reference Ahola, Mononen and Mohaibes2011; Campbell et al. Reference Campbell, Dallaire and Mason2013), and animals can show novelty-induced fear responses or reduced motivation to gain access to enrichments when introduced to them as adults (Cooper et al. Reference Cooper, Ödberg and Nicol1996; Tilly et al. Reference Tilly, Dallaire and Mason2010; Fairhurst et al. Reference Fairhurst, Frey, Reichert, Szelest, Kelly and Bortolotti2011). It is therefore recommended that EE for farmed mink be provided early in life. Although these practices have contributed to partial improvements in the occurrence of self-mutilation behaviours (e.g. tail chewing), stereotypic behaviour remains widespread (current prevalence is unknown but was estimated to occur in 35–85% of adult females based on a large-scale Netherlands study; reported by the European Commission 2001). Moreover, there is no mandated age of EE introduction on commercial mink farms in Canada, and optimal durations and/or timing of EE provision have not yet been identified. A recent assessment of mink welfare across fur farms in Europe assigned the lowest scores to the category of ‘Appropriate Behaviour’, which evaluates stereotypic behaviour, fur chewing, and temperament as well as cage enrichment (Henriksen et al. Reference Henriksen, Møller and Malmkvist2022), lending further support to the sub-optimal effectiveness of current enrichment practices on fur farms.
The present study is part of a larger experiment (i.e. on the same farm using the same cohort of mink) with several objectives relating to enrichment of the physical environments of farmed mink at different life stages. In the companion article to this study published at the same time (Clark et al. Reference Clark, Díez-León and Meagher2025), we presented the effects of enriching the perinatal environment with manipulable enrichments and premium bedding materials for pregnant dams compared to housing dams in standard conditions. Here, we aimed to compare the effects of such enrichment to that of supplying mink in a different group with above-standard EE in the early juvenile phase (once kits become mobile). Specifically, we aimed to investigate whether physically enriching the juvenile environment of farmed mink can positively modulate behaviour, temperament, and long-term stereotypic behaviour compared to mink housed with standard enrichment as juveniles. It was hypothesised that enriched kits would demonstrate a reduction in behaviours and temperaments associated with poor welfare (e.g. boredom, stress, fear, and aggression) and reduced stereotypic behaviour development later in life due to the greater behavioural opportunities available. It was demonstrated in a previous study, also based on this cohort of mink (Clark et al. Reference Clark, Díez-León and Meagher2023), that the variety and novelty of EEs provided to the enriched kits were successful at increasing and sustaining their use of enrichments compared to kits housed in standard conditions; thus, the juvenile enrichment condition was predicted to deliver a positive impact on behaviour and welfare.
Materials and methods
Subjects and housing
Male and female mink selected at breeding for potential use in the study were balanced across Dark, Mahogany, Pastel, Demi, and Stardust colour types (strains). Selection was carried out in advance because one treatment (EW) began before whelping, but we needed to account for later exclusions due to potential unsuccessful copulations and/or poor litter health. All dams were housed individually (American mink are solitary in the wild; Dunstone Reference Dunstone1993) indoors at the Canadian Centre for Fur Animal Research (Nova Scotia, Canada) in 75 × 30 × 45 or 40 cm (length × width × height) wire-mesh pens with a wire shelf (25 × 30 × 25 cm), external wooden nest-boxes (25 × 30 × 20 or 18 cm), and a single plastic ring enrichment (3.8 cm thick, 10 cm in diameter) prior to assignment of their respective conditions. Mink were fed with a meat-based paste placed on the mesh roof of the pen; feedings were once daily in the afternoon for non-reproductive adults and twice daily (morning and afternoon) for pregnant dams approaching parturition, lactating dams, and kits. All mink had ad libitum access to drinking water via automatic drinkers. The research was approved by the Dalhousie University Faculty of Agriculture Animal Care & Use Committee (#1033575) and the Clinical Research Ethics Review Board of the Royal Veterinary College (URN 2021 2034-3).
Housing in the peri-whelping period
Dams who bred successfully (n = 242) were randomly assigned to one of three experimental groups: standard housing (SH; n = 59), enriched at whelping (EW; n = 119; relevant to Clark et al. Reference Clark, Díez-León and Meagher2025), or enriched kits (EK; n = 64). Groups were balanced for colour type and parity and pens were evenly distributed throughout the barn to account for potential effects of variable lighting, temperatures, noise levels, etc. The conditions for the EW group are described in the companion article to this study (see Clark et al. Reference Clark, Díez-León and Meagher2025), but in brief, dams assigned to the SH and EK housing conditions were given standard nest-building materials (chopped straw with wood chip bedding) while EW dams were given standard nest-building materials in addition to two ‘premium’ nest-building materials and a hanging sisal rope enrichment. These EW enrichments were in effect until post-whelping day 8.
When kits were approximately four weeks of age (28 [± 7] days post-whelping, when kits were expected to become mobile; Jonasen Reference Jonasen1987), kits assigned to the EK condition were provided with a hanging plastic chain approximately 38–43 cm in length depending on cage height (selected based on beneficial effects demonstrated in a previous study; Meagher et al. Reference Meagher, Dallaire, Campbell, Ross, Møller, Hansen, Díez-León, Palme and Mason2014) in addition to a standard plastic ring enrichment (previously described). Dams and litters in SH and EW had continued access to a standard plastic ring enrichment at this stage (for a timeline of housing interventions for each group, see Figure 1). Litters were excluded from further testing if fewer than four kits survived to this point.
Housing in the post-weaning period: Group housing
Kits were weaned at six weeks of age as per standard farm protocols, at which point the dam was removed from the whelping pen and housed in a separate standard pen (number of litters surviving to this stage after exclusions: nSH, nEK, and nEW = 36, 33, and 47, respectively). Kits remained in the whelping pen and were housed in groups of four to six (nSH, nEK, and nEW = 218, 183, and 207 kits, respectively); if there were more than eight kits in a litter, they were split across two pens (applied to ten SH pens, three EW pens, and seven EK pens). Enrichment provision for EK kits through this period is detailed in Clark et al. (Reference Clark, Díez-León and Meagher2023), but in brief, access to the hanging chain and standard EE was maintained in addition to introduction of a second mobile enrichment (wiffle ball or golf ball); enrichments were chosen based on beneficial effects demonstrated in previous studies (Díez-León et al. Reference Díez-León, Bowman, Bursian, Filion, Galicia, Kanefsky, Napolitano, Palme, Schulte-Hostedde, Scribner and Mason2013; Meagher et al. Reference Meagher, Dallaire, Campbell, Ross, Møller, Hansen, Díez-León, Palme and Mason2014; Díez-León & Mason Reference Díez-León and Mason2016). A schedule of enrichment exchange was implemented for EK kits such that mobile EEs were exchanged bi-weekly and hanging EEs were exchanged monthly to maintain object novelty (see Clark et al. Reference Clark, Díez-León and Meagher2023). Access to a standard ring enrichment was maintained for SH and EW kits with no enrichment exchange.
Housing in the post-weaning period: Pair housing
At ten weeks of age (four weeks post-weaning), kits were moved to single- or pair-housing pens according to standard farm protocol. One male and one female from each litter were chosen for pair housing and remained in the whelping pen (dimensions of 75 × 30 × 45 or 40 cm [length × width × height]; nSH, nEK, and nEW = 46, 37, and 42 pairs, respectively). A single female from each litter was moved to a drop-in cage (a cage with a wooden nest-box connected at the back near the ceiling, such that mink have to jump up into them; dimensions of 76 × 25 × 45 cm; nSH, nEK, and nEW = 21, 23, and 27 females, respectively). Male-female pairs and single-housed females in EK continued to have access to rotating enrichments (a standard ring, a hanging EE now in the form of a hanging sisal rope, and a second mobile EE now in the form of a pig’s ear or hockey ball; for detailed methods, see Clark et al. Reference Clark, Díez-León and Meagher2023) until 15 weeks of age, at which point these were removed and only a standard enrichment remained. Male-female pairs and single-housed females in SH and EW maintained access to a standard ring enrichment throughout this period.
Kit behaviour observations
Kit behaviour scans in group housing
Following weaning, instantaneous scan-sampling observations of all pens were conducted three consecutive days per week for the following four weeks (from weaning to 10 weeks of age; constituting 12 days of observation per subject with multiple scans per day) by one experienced observer and two trained observers (note: observers were not blind to housing conditions or hypotheses being tested). Inter-rater reliability was assessed using Cohen’s kappa before observers began conducting observations independently (minimum score of 0.61–0.80; for scores in the moderate reliability range [e.g. a score of 0.57 for EE use], discrepancies were reviewed to improve reliability going forward). Scan-sampling observation methods are detailed elsewhere (Clark et al. Reference Clark, Díez-León and Meagher2023). Kits’ interactions with enrichments were recorded in addition to social play and resting (serving as ‘good’ welfare behaviours), aggression, defensiveness, lying awake, or general inactivity (serving as ‘poor’ welfare behaviours; locations in the pen were noted for the latter two behaviours to assess degree of ‘hiding’ in nest-box; Meagher et al. Reference Meagher, Campbell, Dallaire, Díez-León, Palme and Mason2013), and activity, which served as a control behaviour (for ethogram, see Table 1). Pen observation order was reversed each scoring day to prevent the systematic scoring of certain pens earlier than others.
Kit behaviour scans in single or pair housing
All male-female pairs remaining in the study (nSH, nEW, and nEK = 55, 43, and 44 pairs, respectively) continued to be observed according to the group-housing observation protocol until 13 weeks post-whelping. Females placed in single housing were not observed at this time, but EE rotation for EK females continued.
Behavioural observations in kits as adults
Temperament testing
Following the juvenile observation period (and after additional EEs had been removed from EK pens), all single- and pair-housed kits remaining in the trial were tested for temperament by two trained observers using the stick test; both observers were blind to the previous housing conditions of the kits. This test involves inserting a popsicle stick into the pen and recording kits’ behavioural responses (Meagher et al. Reference Meagher, Duncan, Bechard and Mason2011; Mononen et al. Reference Mononen, Møller, Hansen, Hovland, Koistinen, Lidfors, Malmkvist, Vinke and Ahola2012). Reaction categories included ‘curious’, ‘aggressive’, ‘fearful’, or ‘unresponsive’ (for ethogram, see Table 2). Tests were repeated over two consecutive days to determine the reliability of temperament scores for each subject (i.e. whether kits’ responses to the stick test were consistent across tests), and two rounds were conducted per day to revisit any kits who were sleeping in the first round. Kits with conflicting responses across testing days were excluded from analysis; responses were considered conflicting if kits responded as ‘curious’ on the first test and ‘fearful’ or ‘aggressive’ on the second test (Meagher et al. Reference Meagher, Duncan, Bechard and Mason2011). However, a ‘fearful’ reaction followed by a ‘curious’ reaction, an ‘aggressive’ reaction followed by a ‘curious’ reaction, or a ‘curious’ reaction followed by an ‘unresponsive’ reaction were accepted, as some decrease in fear/aggression or interest in the stick due to habituation was expected over repeated tests. In these cases, only the subject’s initial response was kept. Stick testing was conducted in the late summer when kits were 14–16 weeks old (nSH, nEK, and nEW = 113, 101, and 107 kits, respectively) and repeated in the fall at 27–29 weeks (nSH, nEK, and nEW = 111, 100, and 106 kits, respectively; some kits were lost between tests due to mortality or morbidity) to account for potential changes in kit temperament with age.
Fear behaviour observations during pelt grading
As part of standard farm processes, pelt grading was conducted in the late fall (30–32 weeks of age). All pair- and single-housed trial kits were captured and restrained on a pelt grading table to assess their pelt quality. During this process, the number of discrete vocalisations emitted by mink were counted and other behavioural instances including physical struggling, attempts to bite the handler, and urination were recorded using 1–0 sampling as additional measures of temperament and fear behaviour (Zieliński et al. Reference Zieliński, Ślaska, Rozempolska-Rucińska and Zoń2019).
Behaviour scans in kits as adults
One year following the conclusion of juvenile observations and the removal of additional EEs for the EK group (at approximately 16 months of age), stereotypic behaviour (SB) of various subtypes (i.e. locomotor, whole-body stationary, head-based, scrabbling, and wire-gnawing; see Table 1) were scored in the remaining females on trial (nSH, nEK, and nEW = 31, 37, and 36, respectively). Behaviours such as activity, resting, lying awake, or inactivity, and relevant locations of the latter two behaviours in the pen (Meagher et al. Reference Meagher, Campbell, Dallaire, Díez-León, Palme and Mason2013), were also recorded (see Table 1). All mink were single housed by this stage, with access to a single standard ring enrichment. Instantaneous scan-sampling observations were conducted by an experienced observer with previous training in mink behaviour over five non-consecutive days in a two-week period from October to November (note: locations of inactivity sub-types in the pen were only noted during the latter three days of analysis). The observer was blinded to the females’ previous housing conditions. Pens were observed four times per day for a total of 20 scans per female; all rounds were conducted between 1200 and 1500h (morning feeding occurred at 0800h and afternoon feedings were at approximately 1500h). The behaviour being exhibited by mink upon observation was recorded before moving to the next pen, with a 30-s habituation period if necessary (i.e. if mink appeared vigilant of the observer and/or if a behaviour could not be classified without prolonged observation, such as a stereotypic behaviour that must be repeated a certain number of times). One round of sampling took 30 (± 15) min, thus instantaneous scans for each female were approximately 30 mins apart.
Statistical analysis
All statistical analyses were conducted with jamovi statistical software (the jamovi project, 2023; v 2.3.18.0 for Mac). Figures were generated using Prism (GraphPad Software, 2023; v 10.02 for Mac). Significance level was set at P < 0.05. Assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variances for parametric analyses were assessed using Shapiro-Wilk and Levene’s tests, respectively. Transformations were performed as necessary (either square-root or log10 transformations, as appropriate) with mean and 95% confidence interval (CI) subsequently back-transformed for presentation. Where parametric analyses were not appropriate, non-parametric alternatives were used.
Analysis of housing effects on kit behaviours as juveniles
Behavioural scan data pertaining to kit activity, social play, aggression, resting, inactivity, and lying awake were formatted for analysis by calculating the average proportion of observations where a behaviour occurred based on the total number of observations (see equation below). This calculation was necessary because kits did not all receive the same number of scoring days due to scheduling constraints among the researchers and farm staff (as a result of farm events, etc).
Average proportion of observations where behaviour occurred:
Average proportions of observations where behaviours occurred were analysed using one-way ANOVA (Welch’s, to account for unequal variances) and Tukey’s post hoc comparisons with housing condition as a factor when assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variances were met. Kit sex or colour type were not included as factors in these parametric analyses as there is little precedent in the mink literature to suggest that the behaviours measured vary between colour types or sexes at this age. Where data were not normally distributed and transformations were not successful, analyses were conducted using non-parametric one-way ANOVA and Dwass-Steel-Critchlow-Flinger pair-wise comparisons.
Analysis of housing effects on kit temperament as adults
Counts of temperament categorisations from summer and fall stick tests were compared across pair-housed kits and single-housed female kits using separate Chi-squared tests of association, with housing condition across rows and response categories across columns. Fisher’s exact test was used as needed to account for cells with counts below five. The same method was applied for occurrences (yes/no) of attempts to bite the handler, physical struggling, and urination during pelt grading. Number of vocalisations emitted by pair- and single-housed kits across housing conditions were compared using non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis one-way ANOVA.
Analysis of housing effects on kit behaviours as adults
Behavioural scan data pertaining to female kits’ stereotypic behaviour performance, resting, lying awake, or inactivity as adults, as well as locations of inactivity subtypes, were formatted for analysis by calculating the average proportion of observations where a behaviour occurred based on the total number of observations (see equation below).
Average proportion of observations where behaviour occurred:
Average proportions of observations where behaviours occurred were analysed using one-way ANOVA (Welch’s, to account for unequal variances) and Tukey’s post hoc comparisons with housing condition as a factor when assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variances were met. Where data were not normally distributed and transformations were not successful, analyses were conducted using non-parametric one-way ANOVA and Dwass-Steel-Critchlow-Flinger pair-wise comparisons.
Results
Housing effects on kit behaviours as juveniles
Group housing
There was no effect of housing on any measures of kit behaviour in the group-housing phase (for a summary of statistical results, see Table 3[a]).
Pair housing
Social play differed significantly across housing conditions in the pair-housing phase; EW kits were observed performing social play more often on average than EK kits (t 122 = –2.54; P = 0.033; Table 3[b]), although this behaviour did not differ between EW kits and SH kits (t 122 = –1.11; P = 0.508) or between SH and EK kits (t 122 = –1.52; P = 0.285; Table 3[b]). Aggressive and defensive behaviours remained similar across housing conditions in pair housing, as did activity levels, resting, and lying awake in the nest-box (statistics summarised in Table 3[b]). However, lying awake in the pen showed effects of housing condition towards more lying awake in EK kits than in SH kits (W = 3.35; P = 0.047); lying awake did not differ between EK and EW kits (W = –1.96; P = 0.348) or SH and EW kits (W = 1.87; P = 0.382). Inactivity in the nest-box also differed by housing condition in the pair-housing phase: EK kits were inactive in the nest-box less often on average compared to SH kits (t 122 = 3.61; P = 0.001) and EW kits (t 122 = –3.859; P < 0.001), while SH and EW kits did not differ in this behaviour (t 122 = –0.337; P = 0.939; Table 3[b]).
Housing effects on kit temperament
There was no effect of housing condition on the temperaments of pair-housed kits as determined by the stick test in the summer (χ24 = 2.31; P = 0.679) or fall months (χ26 = 4.68; P = 0.620; Table 4). Likewise, temperaments of single-housed females did not differ across conditions in either season (χ26 = 5.09; P = 0.552 and χ26 = 3.95; P = 0.729, respectively; Table 4). Number of vocalisations during handling for pelt grading did not differ across conditions in pair-housed (χ22 = 3.95; P = 0.832) or single-housed kits (χ22 = 2.21; P = 0.332; Table 5).
Attempts to bite the handler occurred at similar levels between kits of each condition in pair and single housing (χ22 = 0.490; P = 0.783 and χ22 = 3.17; P = 0.205), as did occurrences of physical struggling (χ22 = 4.33; P = 0.115 and χ22 = 1.08; P = 0.582) and urination (χ22 = 0.520; P = 0.742 and χ22 = 3.13; P = 0.194, respectively; Table 5).
Housing effects on kit behaviour as adults
Performance of locomotor SB and whole-body SB did not differ across adult females reared in different housing conditions (χ22 = 3.878; P = 0.144 and χ22 = 1.770; P = 0.413, respectively; Figure 2); there were no observations of head-based SB, scrabbling, or wire gnawing. There was also no difference in observed activity, resting, lying awake, or inactivity across housing conditions (Table 6).
a Locations were only recorded on three out of five days of behaviour scans.
Discussion
As summarised in a previous article based on this study cohort, EK kits showed more overall and more sustained enrichment use across the juvenile period compared to kits in standard housing (Clark et al. Reference Clark, Díez-León and Meagher2023). However, as shown here, the predicted benefits of EK housing on other kit behaviours were largely unsupported. During the group-housing phase, neither positive welfare-related behaviours (social play or resting) nor behaviours potentially associated with boredom, fear, and/or stress (forms of waking inactivity and aggression) differed between SH, EK, and EW kits. During pair housing, EK kits did demonstrate reduced inactivity in the nest-box compared to SH and EW kits and increased lying awake in the pen compared to SH kits (10–13 weeks post-whelping). However, each of these forms of inactivity are suggested to be associated with negative states in mink (fear and boredom, respectively; Meagher & Mason Reference Meagher and Mason2012; Meagher et al. Reference Meagher, Campbell, Dallaire, Díez-León, Palme and Mason2013, Reference Meagher, Campbell and Mason2017; Fureix & Meagher Reference Fureix and Meagher2015), and thus it is difficult to determine whether EK enrichments were able to improve kit affective states in this period. Moreover, social play occurred more often in EW kits than in EK kits during pair housing. This may be indicative of reduced stress in EW kits compared to EK kits, as social play is known to occur primarily in healthy, unstressed animals (Burghardt Reference Burghardt2005). However, as discussed in the companion paper to this study (Clark et al. Reference Clark, Díez-León and Meagher2025), cortisol concentrations were not assessed during juvenile behavioural observations, and when assessed at six months of age, there was no difference in basal faecal cortisol metabolite concentrations between kits of different conditions.
The temperaments of kits reared in different conditions, as measured by the stick test, also appeared unaffected, and fear behaviours during handling for pelt grading were performed at similar levels across conditions. Occurrences of these fear behaviours were relatively low in our sample overall, as approximately twenty percent of kits demonstrated struggling, biting, or urination during handling, and a mean of only 1.05 vocalisations per kit were emitted across all conditions combined. As discussed in the companion article to this study (Clark et al. Reference Clark, Díez-León and Meagher2025), the mink on the research farm where experiments were conducted may have had low levels of fear as a population (fear traits are known to be highly heritable and subject to selection; Hansen Reference Hansen1996; Malmkvist & Hansen Reference Malmkvist and Hansen2001, Reference Malmkvist and Hansen2002; Berg et al. Reference Berg, Hansen, Hansen and Malmkvist2002; Thirstrup et al. Reference Thirstrup, Villumsen, Malmkvist and Lund2019). Similarly, any effects of housing condition on fear behaviours may have been masked by individual variability in responses to handling or immobilisation stress of certain durations and intensities; individual mink have been observed to show high variation in stress responses to such events (Malmkvist et al. Reference Malmkvist, Díez-León and Christensen2024). Alternatively, it is possible that the enrichment methods implemented in this study (the timing or duration of provision, rotation of items, etc) may have been ineffective in modulating mink temperament. In previous studies where minks’ temperament or fear responses to handling were improved by enriched housing, the mink were housed with enrichments consistently through to the time of assessment or temporally close to the time of assessment (Meagher et al. Reference Meagher, Dallaire, Campbell, Ross, Møller, Hansen, Díez-León, Palme and Mason2014; Bak & Malmkvist Reference Bak and Malmkvist2020), unlike in the present study where only a standard EE was available for all conditions at the time of assessment.
The main shortcoming of the EK intervention was that it was not successful at reducing the development of SB in the kits as adults. In fact, EK kits showed signs of developing more whole-body SB than SH or EW kits. Although there was no statistical difference in overall performance of whole-body SB, the interquartile range for this subtype in EK mink was larger than that of mink from other housing conditions. Types of SB were distinguished in the present study due to previous evidence that sub-types of SB are heterogeneous in mink, although thus far only scrabbling and head-based forms have been suggested to be distinct from locomotor forms (both stationary and those involving translocation) in their causation and treatments (Dallaire et al. Reference Dallaire, Meagher, Díez-León, Garner and Mason2011; Díez-León et al. Reference Díez-León, Bowman, Bursian, Filion, Galicia, Kanefsky, Napolitano, Palme, Schulte-Hostedde, Scribner and Mason2013, Reference Díez-León, Kitchenham, Duprey, Bailey, Choleris, Lewis and Mason2019; Polanco et al. Reference Polanco, Campbell, Díez-León and Mason2017, Reference Polanco, Díez-León and Mason2018). Given that locomotor forms and whole-body, stationary forms are known to co-occur in mink (Polanco et al. Reference Polanco, Campbell, Díez-León and Mason2017; Malmkvist et al. Reference Malmkvist, Díez-León and Christensen2024), the greater tendency of EK mink to display both is interesting. Overall activity also did not differ between groups, so this is an unlikely explanation for any differences in SB expression. Increases in enclosure complexity have been suggested to impede the space available to perform SB, or make them more complex in appearance (e.g. an established route-tracing stereotypy in a cape hunting dog [Lycaon pictus] was impeded by introduction of a hanging chain and the route was then adjusted to avoid the chain; Fentress Reference Fentress, Bateson and Hinde1976; also cf Bergeron et al. [Reference Bergeron, Badnell-Waters, Lambton, Mason, Mason and Rushen2006] on oral behaviours), so it is possible that the extra mobile and hanging EEs for EK kits acted as obstacles that caused alternative, stationary forms of SB to develop.
Moreover, our finding that performance of locomotor SB or any form of SB in general was not reduced in EK mink contradicts the findings of previous studies where access to a multitude of physical enrichments in early life or adulthood positively impacted SB (Hansen et al. Reference Hansen, Malmkvist, Palme and Damgaard2007; Meagher & Mason Reference Meagher and Mason2012; Dallaire et al. Reference Dallaire, Meagher and Mason2012; Campbell et al. Reference Campbell, Dallaire and Mason2013; Meagher et al. Reference Meagher, Campbell, Dallaire, Díez-León, Palme and Mason2013; Díez-León et al. Reference Díez-León, Bursian, Galicia, Napolitano, Palme and Mason2016). However, these impacts were often delivered through EE being present in the subjects’ pens at the time of SB assessment; or, in the case of Díez-León et al. (Reference Díez-León, Bursian, Galicia, Napolitano, Palme and Mason2016), the earliest SB assessment was conducted after only five weeks of removal from EE. The present study is the first to investigate whether providing EE to mink exclusively as juveniles can attenuate performance of SB up to one year following removal. In bank voles (Myodes glareolus; Ödberg Reference Ödberg1987) and deer mice (Peromyscus maniculatus; Powell et al. Reference Powell, Newman, McDonald, Bugenhagen and Lewis2000; Hadley et al. Reference Hadley, Hadley, Ephraim, Yang and Lewis2006), rearing in EE is shown to have protective effects against the development of SB even after long-term placement in standard pens. In other species, meanwhile, removal of temporary EE can exacerbate stereotypy (e.g. in primates: Bayne et al. Reference Bayne, Hurst and Dexter1992; in pigs [Sus scrofa]: Day et al. Reference Day, Burfoot, Docking, Whittaker, Spoolder and Edwards2002; in CD-1 mice [Mus musculus]: Latham & Mason Reference Latham and Mason2010). The latter scenario is thought to result from frustration due to placement in environments with fewer behavioural opportunities relative to their former more stimulating, rewarding environments (a so-called ‘negative contrast’ effect), whereas animals who have not experienced these enriched conditions do not have the same frustrations (Crespi Reference Crespi1942; Pecoraro et al. Reference Pecoraro, Timberlake and Tinsley1999; Wiegmann et al. Reference Wiegmann, Wiegmann and Waldron2003). It is therefore plausible that EK mink became accustomed to greater, more diverse enrichment and experienced frustration after subsequent placement in standard EE with only one, permanently present enrichment, resulting in development of frustration-induced SB. However, behavioural frustration in negative contrast scenarios typically also correlates with increased glucocorticoid output (Latham & Mason Reference Latham and Mason2010), which was not observed in EK kits relative to kits of other conditions during faecal cortisol sampling two months after EE removal (data presented in Clark et al. Reference Clark, Díez-León and Meagher2025).
Moreover, in Díez-León et al. (Reference Díez-León, Bursian, Galicia, Napolitano, Palme and Mason2016), mink reared in EE from birth and then placed in standard housing did not demonstrate exacerbated SB following EE removal and in fact showed reductions in locomotor SB compared to mink that had never experienced enrichment. This may be due to the timing of EE removal: enriched mink were placed in standard pens for five weeks in mid-November, when mink were six or seven months of age, and their SB was assessed in these standard pens prior to them being returned to enriched pens. SB does not fully develop in mink until seven months of age (Jeppesen et al. Reference Jeppesen, Heller and Dalsgaard2000), at which point extra EEs for EK mink in the present study had already been removed. Thus, EE may only have protective effects against SB development in mink when supplied from rearing until approximately seven months of age, a potentially critical age in SB development. Interestingly, Axelsson et al. (Reference Axelsson, Aldén and Lidfors2009) introduced enrichments to mink at seven months of age when subjects had already begun exhibiting SB, and one of the two farms housing trial mink did show an enrichment-related decrease in SB while the other did not. Thus, it is possible that seven months is a critical age for the provision of EE in mink, but provision leading up to this age is more effective than introduction once SBs have already begun to develop. In the aforementioned studies with deer mice, EE was also provided early in life (from 4–14 or 14–21 weeks of age: Hadley et al. Reference Hadley, Hadley, Ephraim, Yang and Lewis2006; detailed timing of provision is unknown in Powell et al. Reference Powell, Newman, McDonald, Bugenhagen and Lewis2000), and SB in deer mice is thought to become stable by six weeks of age (Tanimura et al. Reference Tanimura, Yang, Ottens and Lewis2010). Thus, the mice also had access to enrichments prior to and during the period in which SB becomes fully developed, and this EE strategy was also protective against future development of SB.
Of course, the lack of SB reduction in EK mink, or lack of statistical difference in whole-body SB of mink across housing conditions, may also have resulted from a false negative. Our sample of mink followed for SB development as adults was limited due to pelting of select individuals in their first year of life (according to standard farm practices) and other factors such as mortality, thus affecting our statistical power to detect differences. In future research it would be beneficial to follow a larger sample of females (and potentially males, as well) through to adulthood to observe their SB development after differential housing as juveniles. Alternatively, the amount of behavioural data collected from the available mink could be increased (i.e. observations could be conducted across a longer period than five days, or the number of scans per day could be increased). Regarding our speculations about timing of removal for EK enrichments, we recommend that future studies attempt to house mink with the nature of EE provided in this study until seven months of age to determine if this duration and timing of juvenile enrichment is sufficient to deliver lasting preventative effects on SB, even after long-term placement in standard housing.
Animal welfare implications
Although used, the manipulable play objects provided to enriched mink kits in this study did not deliver many positive impacts on behavioural measures of welfare. These enrichments may have the potential to reduce stereotypic behaviour development if access is maintained beyond 15 weeks of age. The results of this study provide a basis for the hypothesis that environmental enrichment may be more effective at modulating stereotypic behaviour if provided during critical periods of stereotypic behaviour development, which may have applications for welfare intervention strategies in various species that tend to perform locomotor and whole-body forms of stereotypic behaviour in captivity.
Conclusion
Providing farmed mink kits with an array of manipulable EEs periodically exchanged to maintain novelty was successful at reducing a potential behavioural indicator of fear in the late juvenile period (inactivity in the nest-box), but a potential indicator of boredom was simultaneously increased (lying awake in the pen). Stereotypic behaviour development in the kits as adults was also not affected by EK housing; this demonstrates that additional enrichment as juveniles may not attenuate stereotypic behaviour performance in adult mink after one year of placement in standard housing. It would be valuable to determine in future studies if juvenile enrichment of this nature has long-term benefits when maintained until seven months of age, when stereotypic behaviours are known to become established in mink.
Acknowledgements
We thoroughly appreciate the assistance of the Canadian Centre for Fur Animal Research staff and undergraduate research assistant Meridyth Hardy in conducting this study. We would also like to thank Mitacs and the Canada Mink Breeders Association for funding.
Competing interest
None.