Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T15:36:07.510Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Life beyond a jar: Effects of tank size and furnishings on the behaviour and welfare of Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 December 2024

Naomi Clark-Shen
Affiliation:
Independent researcher, Singapore
Juliette Tariel-Adam
Affiliation:
Macquarie University, School of Natural Sciences, Australia
Anya Gajanur
Affiliation:
Virginia Institute of Marine Science, William & Mary, USA
Culum Brown*
Affiliation:
Macquarie University, School of Natural Sciences, Australia
*
Corresponding author: Culum Brown; Email: culum.brown@mq.edu.au
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Globally, Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens) continue to be sold and kept in small, barren jars or tanks, with little concern for their welfare. This study aimed to examine the impact of housing size and furnishings (i.e. live plants, refuges) on the behaviour of Siamese fighting fish, to understand optimal tank conditions. Thirteen male Siamese fighting fish were rotated between five different housing conditions: ‘jar’ (1.5 L); ‘small’ (3.3 L); ‘medium’ (5.6 L); ‘large’ (19.3 L); and ‘large-barren’ (19.3 L). All tanks had gravel and furnishings, except the large-barren tank which was devoid of these. Overall, tank size influenced behaviour. Fish were significantly more active and spent significantly less time resting and performing ‘abnormal’ behaviours (hovering and stereotypic swimming), in the large tank compared to the smaller tanks. Tank furnishings also influenced behaviour. Fishes in the large-barren tank performed more ‘abnormal’ behaviours (hovering, stereotypic swimming, interaction with the walls), compared to the large tank which had furnishings. These results suggest that the small, barren jars and tanks that Siamese fighting fish are often housed in are detrimental to their welfare, and larger, furnished tanks are more optimal. Behavioural variations were observed between the fish in this study, highlighting individual fish personality. We recommend a minimum tank size of 5.6 L for the display and sale of Siamese fighting fish, and tanks larger than this for keeping Siamese fighting fish at home. All tanks should contain gravel, live plants and refuges.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

Fishes are rarely afforded the same level of compassion, welfare and legislation as other vertebrates (Brown Reference Brown2015). However, there is growing recognition that fishes possess impressive memory and cognitive abilities (White & Brown Reference White and Brown2014; Brown Reference Brown2015), social intelligence (Oliveira et al. Reference Oliveira, McGregor and Latruffe1998; Bshary Reference Bshary, Brown, Krause and Laland2011), individual personalities (Byrnes & Brown Reference Byrnes and Brown2016), and the capacity to feel pain (Sneddon Reference Sneddon2003; Reilly et al. Reference Reilly, Quinn, Cossins and Sneddon2008). With increasing recognition of their sentience (Brown Reference Brown2015), concerns for their welfare are growing (Berlinghieri et al. Reference Berlinghieri, Panizzon, Penry-Williams and Brown2021), as is the need to improve their treatment across industries – from capture fisheries (Veldhuizen et al. Reference Veldhuizen, Berentsen, de Boer, Van de Vis and Bokkers2018), to aquaculture (Kleiber et al. Reference Kleiber, Stomp, Rouby, Ferreira, Begout, Benhaim, Labbe, Tocqueville, Levadoux, Calandreau, Guesdon and Colson2023), in laboratories (Smith Reference Smith, Bayne and Turner2014), and as pets (Sermwatanakul Reference Sermwatanakul2019).

Fishes are the most numerous pet in the world (Brown Reference Brown2019), with an estimated value of up to US$20 billion, and over 6,000 species traded internationally (Moorhead & Zeng Reference Moorhead and Zeng2010; Novak et al. Reference Novak, Kalous and Patoka2020). The Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens) is one of the most popular pet fishes. Originating from rivers in Southeast Asia, they have been bred for exquisite colours and elongated fins (Monvises et al. Reference Monvises, Nuangsaeng, Sriwattanarothai and Panijan2009). Thailand is one of the biggest suppliers of Siamese fighting fish; in 2018, there were over 1,000 Siamese fighting fish farms in Thailand with exports numbering over 20 million individual fish (Sermwatanakul Reference Sermwatanakul2019). The USA, China, Singapore, France and Iran are top importers (Sermwatanakul Reference Sermwatanakul2019). Siamese fighting fish are typically sold and then kept in jars or small ‘betta vases’, often with no tank accessories, both of which are acknowledged as potential welfare concerns (Sermwatanakul Reference Sermwatanakul2019). However, recommendations for appropriate tank size are vague. For example, one Siamese fighting fish handbook states that “a large aquarium is preferred over a small one” (Goldstein Reference Goldstein2004), while another source states “a minimum of four litres”. These suggested conditions arise out of concerns over water quality, not behavioural indicators of poor welfare (Pleeging & Moons Reference Pleeging and Moons2017). Singapore’s ‘Pet Shop Licence Conditions (5) Display and Sale of Fancy Fish’ Act states “Fishes must be kept in tanks of adequate size” (Animal & Veterinary Services 2023) but does not state what this is and still allows Siamese fighting fish to be sold in small jars. Although one study has shown that Siamese fighting fish swim more in larger tanks compared to smaller ones (Oldfield & Murphy Reference Oldfield and Murphy2024), the general lack of research and attention in this area has made is difficult to change consumer behaviour and policy to improve welfare.

Behaviour is recognised as a key indicator of an animal’s emotional state in captivity (Dawkins Reference Dawkins2003). For example, abnormal repetitive behaviours, such as stereotypic pacing, are defined as functionless and may represent an inability to cope with the captive environment, while increased behavioural diversity is a potentially positive indicator of welfare (Miller et al. Reference Miller, Vicino, Sheftel and Lauderdale2020). Studies on captive animals reveal that in general, more naturalistic living conditions promote the freedom to express natural behaviours, which can contribute to greater welfare (Kleiman et al. Reference Kleiman, Thompson and Baer2010; Alligood et al. Reference Alligood and Leighty2015). Compared to terrestrial animals in zoos, there are fewer studies examining the welfare of fishes in captivity, however those that have been conducted reveal parallels (Toni et al. Reference Toni, Manciocco, Angiulli, Alleva, Cioni and Malavasi2018; Barreto et al. Reference Barreto, Planellas, Yang, Phillips and Descovich2020). A study on black rockfish (Sebastes melanops), for example, found that an absence of plant and structural enrichment significantly increased stress, as determined through higher cortisol levels (Zhang et al. Reference Zhang, Bai, Xu, Guo and Zhang2020). In a study on sharks, rays and teleost fishes in an aquarium, enhanced environmental complexity and reduced visitor exposure resulted in increased natural behaviours and decreased abnormal repetitive behaviours (Lawrence et al. Reference Lawrence, Sherwen and Larsen2021). Similarly, small tanks triggered stereotypy and reduced boldness in zebrafish (Danio rerio) (Abudusaimaiti et al. Reference Abudusaimaiti, Wang, Luo and Li2020). Other studies have found that big enclosures make fish swim faster (Tang & Boisclair Reference Tang and Boisclair1993), grow better and result in higher survival rates (Jha et al. Reference Jha, Barat and Nayak2006). Collectively these studies suggest that improved housing conditions have positive outcomes on behaviour and welfare. However, different species of fish have unique behavioural repertoires, so species-specific assessments to determine optimal housing are preferential (Smith Reference Smith2023).

This study aimed to examine the impact of housing size and furnishings (e.g. plants, refuges) on the behaviour of Siamese fighting fish. A greater understanding of Siamese fighting fish behaviour in relation to tank size and enrichment (i.e. furnishings) can help to guide stakeholders (e.g. sellers, buyers, policy-makers) toward improved welfare for these animals. Specifically, the outcomes will guide recommended minimum tank size to promote better welfare for Siamese fighting fish. The study was conducted in Singapore, which has been the number one exporter of ornamental fishes since the 1980s, and where fish-keeping is a popular local hobby (Hua Yue Reference Hua Yue2019), with Siamese fighting fish sold in numerous pet shops and fish shops. Thus, while Singapore is a suitable country to conduct this research and drive change, the impacts of this research are global.

Materials and methods

Ethical considerations

As this study was not conducted within a university, formal animal ethics approval was not attained, however, we followed the recommendations as outlined by the Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour (ASAB Ethical Committee/ABS Animal Care Committee 2023) and an expert in the field of fish welfare (CB) was consulted throughout. All the tanks used in this study were larger than the small cups/jars in which the fish were sold, thus affording them more space than they had previously been accustomed to. Additionally, a fish shop in Singapore which is known for promoting high welfare for fish-keeping was consulted to ascertain how to monitor water quality (through test kits) and reduce stress (i.e. through use of ‘black water’ and ‘smoothing fluids’). When the study was completed, three fish were adopted by NCS and kept in various tanks > 5.6 L in size and with plants, gravel and hideouts. All the remaining fish (n = 10), were adopted out to friends, or friends-of-friends, with strict conditions regarding tank size and furnishing (> 5.6 L, larger than the ‘medium’ tank used in this experiment, and with plants and gravel). All adopters were asked to send photographs of their tank set-up, and photographs/videos and updates following adoption, to ensure the fish were kept in the tanks that were promised and were doing well. NCS maintained contact with all adopters to provide continuous advice on fish-keeping.

Fifteen male Siamese fighting fish (labelled as ‘Half-moon bettas’) were purchased from an ornamental fish farm in Singapore in December 2021 (seven fish) and January 2022 (eight fish). At the ornamental fish farm from which they were bought, they were displayed, and sold in, barren circular jars (8.5 cm × 11.6 cm; height × width; approximately 900 ml water volume). Following purchase, they were randomly assigned to an experimental housing tank (Table 1), to begin the experimental trial.

Table 1. Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens) were housed individually and rotated between five tank treatments during this study. The tank sizes, furnishings and the duration that they were kept in each treatment during the study is presented

Experimental tank set-up

Five tanks of varying sizes and furnishings were used: ‘jar’; ‘small’; ‘medium’; ‘large’; and ‘large-barren’ (Table 1, Figure 1). To prevent fish getting distracted by human activity, a white sheet was placed about 1 m in front of the tanks and held in place throughout the experiment. Small square flaps were cut in the sheet to allow a camera (iPhone) to record the tanks on ‘recording’ days. Outside of recording days, the square flaps were pinned back in place to ensure there were no gaps in the sheet. Fish only experienced interaction with people during feeding and cleaning (see Tank maintenance). All tanks were covered (using grey industrial tape) at both ends to prevent fish in adjacent tanks from seeing each other. Of the 15 fish, seven had a filter in their tank during the small, medium and large trials, while eight did not have a filter during any of the tank trials. The filter was not used for all trials since typically Siamese fighting fish are not housed with filters.

Figure 1. Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens) were housed individually in and rotated between jar (1.5 L; three small surface plants and pebbles), small (3.3 L; one medium plant and pebbles), medium (5.6 L; one medium plant and pebbles), large (19.3 L; one large plant, pebbles and one barrel for refuge), and large-barren (19.3 L; no plants or furnishings).

Rotation of fish

Fish stayed in each of the four tanks that had furnishings (jar, small, medium, large; Table 1, Figure 1) for seven days to allow fish to acclimate, with filming taking place on the seventh day. Fish were then rotated among these four tanks until they had experienced all of them. Rotation order between tanks was generally as follows: those in jar and medium were swapped, and those in large and small were swapped. Fish were then rotated to the other tanks they had not experienced and swapped in the same manner. This order of rotation was chosen to minimise a drastic downgrade in condition (e.g. from large to jar) which may have induced heightened stress. Once fish had spent seven days in each of these four tanks, they were rotated into the barren tank (large-barren), but to reduce potential risk of stress or mortality from the sudden change in tank condition (i.e. from having furnishings to having none, which can also affect water quality), fish only stayed in this tank for three days, with filming on the third day. When fish were rotated into each of the five tanks (jar, small, medium, large, large-barren) they were observed for 5 min and if any fish reacted in a way that indicated an inability to cope (e.g. they were suffering; referring to unpleasant mental states such as fear or pain; Brown Reference Brown2015) they were given an ‘exemption’ from that particular tank (e.g. a total skip of that tank) and placed in a ‘holding’ tank (which fell between the sizes of small and medium, and contained pebbles and a plant) for seven days until they could be rotated back into the trial. Table 2 outlines the order in which each fish experienced each tank.

Table 2. The order in which the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens; n = 15) were rotated between five tank treatments during this study (Jar, Small, Medium, Large, and Large-Barren). 1 = first, 2 = second, 3 = third, 4 = fourth, 5 = fifth. ‘EX’ is used where fish were given an exemption from a tank due to exhibiting what was perceived to be a potential inability to cope, ‘(F)’ indicates if a filter was present in the tank, and ‘(D)’ is used if and when a fish passed away

Recording behaviours

On the day of recording (day seven in jar, small, medium, large, and day three in large-barren), fish were recorded for a 10-min period between 0700–0730h (dependent on when the sun had risen and there was sufficient daylight), and again at 1000, 1400 and 1800h, resulting in a total of 40-min footage per fish per tank. These recordings were then subsequently reviewed, and ‘continuous’ behavioural observations were recorded (Table 3).

Table 3. Behavioural categories recorded for Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens; n = 15) rotated between five tank treatments. The behavioural categories chosen were based on observed behaviours shown during the recordings themself. The total duration of each behaviour was recorded in seconds

Tank maintenance

Fish were fed twice per day, at 0800 and 1820h. Larger fish were given six pellets per feeding and smaller fish were given four pellets per feeding. The jar, small, medium, and large tanks underwent a full water change on the seventh day when fish were rotated, but pebbles were not rinsed through as the build-up of bacteria in these pebbles can help maintain water stability. The large-barren tanks were given a full water change on the third day, when fish were rotated. All tank water was replaced with water that had been pre-mixed in a bucket with ‘black water’, which acts as an anti-chlorine, and ‘water smoother’, which softens water (9.56 ml black water and 86 ml of smoothing fluid pre-mixed per 43-L bucket). Fish were rotated into their newly cleaned tanks via a small scoop that contained water (< 1 L of water) from their previous tank. No artificial lights were used, and at night the room lights were switched off, allowing for near-total darkness. A water-quality monitoring kit (Pro JBL Aquatest Easy 7 in 1, JBL, Neuhofen, Germany) was used in each tank, on the day before recording, to test for ammonia, nitrate, nitrogen dioxide, GH (hardiness), alkalinity, pH, carbon dioxide and chlorine gas, all of which can affect fish behaviour and survival.

Data analysis

While 15 fish started the trial only 13 completed it (see Table 2) and statistical analyses were only performed on data pertaining to these 13 fish. Statistical analyses were carried out to test the effect of tank on fish behaviour, while controlling for fish identity, tank order (Table 2), time of the day and presence of filter. Times where fish were ‘out-of-view’ or if the type of behaviour could not be determined (‘unsure’), were not included in the analysis (Table 3). The times spent ‘resting’, ‘hovering’, ‘sinking or floating’, ‘swimming’, ‘stereotypical swimming’, ‘interaction with the walls’, ‘nest building’ and ‘foraging’ during a trial were first analysed with a Principal Component Analysis (PCA). The PCA was performed: (1) to reduce the number of behaviours to analyse; (2) to look at the correlation between behaviours; and (3) to assess which behaviour(s) were the most important to explain the behavioural variability between trials. The PCA did not help to reduce the number of behaviours to analyse. Only the first four components were kept as they had an eigenvalue > 1 and explained 70% of the total variance. However, the fourth component was not readily associated with the initial behaviours and therefore cannot be biologically interpreted (Figure 3). After excluding the fourth component, the first three components collectively explained only 57% of the variance. We thus decided to perform individual linear models on each behaviour rather than on these first four components. ‘Sinking or floating’ did not not undergo further analysis since it was explaining less than 2% of the total behavioural variance.

Figure 2. Behaviours observed (A) in each of the five tank treatments (jar, small, medium, large, and large-barren), (B) across different times, (C) with and without a filter in the tank and (D) in the order in which fish were rotated into the tank. Each bar represents an average time spent displaying behaviours by the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens; n = 13) which completed the rotation between the five tank trials.

Figure 3. Results of Principle Component Analysis for the times that the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens; n = 13) spent performing each behaviour across the five tank treatments (jar, small, medium, large, and large-barren).

The time spent swimming and the time spent resting were analysed separately with a linear mixed model (LMM) including tank (factorial variable with five levels: jar, small, medium, large, large-barren), time of the day (factorial variable with 4 levels: 0700, 1000, 1400, 1800h), filter (factorial variable with 2 levels: presence of filter, absence of filter) and tank order (factorial variable with five levels: 1, 2, 3, 4, 5) as fixed effects and fish identity (factorial variable with 13 levels: Dragon, Elf, Fairy, Ghost, Goblin, Kinara, Kraken, Merlion, Naga, Orange Pendek, Pegasus, Phoenix, Wizard) as a random effect. Tank order was a categorical rather than a continuous variable as the relationship between time spent swimming/resting and tank order was not linear. The models were rank deficient for tank and tank order: The level ‘5’ of tank order was confounded with the level large-barren of tank as large-barren was the fifth tank in which fish went into for all fish (Table 2). This means that we could not statistically disentangle the effect of ‘barren’ from the effect of the fifth tank order in the analysis.

The times spent hovering, stereotypically swimming, interacting with the surface, nest building or foraging did not follow a normal distribution as there were too many zeros (i.e. trials where the fish did not perform this behaviour at all). These behaviours were thus analysed as occurrences/binary variables: (1) if a fish performed the behaviour during the trial, (0) if not. The occurrences of hovering/stereotypically swimming/interacting with the surface/nest building/foraging during a trial were separately analysed with a generalised linear mixed model (GLMM) with tank, time of the day, filter and tank order as fixed effects and fish identity as random effect. As there were still a lot of trials during which fish hovered (204 out of 252 trials), an LMM on the time spent hovering was conducted in addition to the GLMM on the occurrence of hovering. The LMM included only the trials during which the fish hovered. It had tank, time of the day and filter as fixed effects and fish identity as random effect. The model was fitted with weighted least squares using the package nlme to account for the residual variance increasing with the fitted hovering time values. For this reason, it was not possible to estimate the variance explained by the fish random effect or the repeatability. In addition, the nlme package does not allow for rank deficiency so we did not include tank order in the hovering LMM. Included or not, the tank order did not change the significance of the other fixed effects in the previous LMMs and GLMMs.

Additional analysis was performed to test whether fish had a preference for resting on furnishings (plants, hideout barrel or filter) over the floor or at the water surface. The resting place was scored whenever a fish was resting and categorised as resting on the floor, water surface, plants, in or on hideout barrel and filter. The analysis was restricted to the small, medium and large tanks because they were the only tanks with furnishing inside. Plants, filter and hideout barrel were concatenated into one category ‘furnishings’ to have the same three categories for all tanks (i.e. floor, furnishings and water surface) as the hideout barrel was specific to the large tank and filter was specific to the tanks with filter inside. The analysis included 152 trials during which the fish rested at least 1 s. A multinomial regression model was employed to analyse the proportions of resting time in the three places. This model is the extension of the binomial logit model to more than two categories. The response variables were the number of seconds resting on the floor, the number of seconds resting on furnishings and the number of seconds resting on the water surface during the trial. Due to the specifics of the multinomial regression model, tank, order, time, and filter were combined into a single random effect termed ‘experimental setting’. This approach was chosen to avoid testing the difference in resting time between resting places across all levels of tank, order, time, and filter, which was not the primary biological question. The other random effects included were fish and trial (factorial variable with 152 levels corresponding to the trial ID) to account for the non-independence of seconds belonging to the same trial.

Type II F-tests with Kenward and Roger’s method were used to test statistical significance of fixed effects in the LMMs. Likelihood ratio tests (χ2) were used to test statistical significance of the fixed effects in the GLMMs and the random effect in both LMMs and GLMMs. For all models, we visually checked normality of residuals and their homoscedasticity across factor levels. We did not remove any outliers. Post hoc pair-wise contrasts between estimated means of Tank were performed to test statistically significant differences between tanks using the Holm P-value correction for multiple tests. To account for rank deficiency, estimated means of jar, small, medium and large were averaged over the levels of tank order 1–4 while the estimated means of barren were calculated on level 5 of tank order. Confidence intervals of contrasts provided in the Results are 95% CI. R version 4.3.1 was used in addition to the following R packages: tidyverse for data manipulation; ggplot2, ggpubr, ggrepel, ggforce, RColorBrewer, virdis for drawing plots; FactoMineR for the PCA; lme4, nlme, lmerTest, emmeans, glmmTMB, fitdistrplus, DHARMa, rptR, performance for the linear models’ estimation and diagnosis.

Results

Influence of tank, time and filter on behaviour

Swimming occurred more in the large tank; resting occurred more in the jar and medium tanks; interaction with the walls occurred more in the jar, medium, and barren tanks; stereotypic swimming occurred more in the small and barren tanks; hovering occurred more in the jar and barren tank (Figure 2A, Table 4). Fish were more active in the morning, with swimming occurring more in the morning and resting occurring more in the afternoon (Figure 2B, Table 4). Hovering and nest building occurred more without a filter in the tank, while swimming occurred more with a filter in the tank (Figure 2C, Table 4). The order in which individuals were rotated between tanks (Table 2) had a significant effect for swimming and resting, with fish swimming more, and resting less, during the second and third tank they were rotated into, after accounting for tank size difference (Table 4).

Table 4. Results of the linear and generalised mixed models on different behaviours across the five tanks (Jar, Small, Medium, Large, and Large-Barren), time (0700, 1000, 1400 and 1800h), and filter (presence and absence) for the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens; n = 13) used in this study

Principle Component Analysis (PCA) results

The first principal component (25% of variance) was driven by resting, swimming, and foraging: if fish spent time swimming and foraging, there was little resting. The second component (16% of variance) was driven by hovering, stereotypic swimming, and interaction with walls: if fish spent a lot of time hovering and interacting with walls, there was little stereotypic swimming. The third component (15% of variance) was driven by nest building, followed by a slight influence of foraging, resting and stereotypic swimming: if fish spent time foraging and resting in a trial, they were doing little nest building and stereotypic swimming. However, these last correlations may be driven by trials in the jar as fish rarely engaged in nest building or stereotypic swimming in this tank.

Commonly observed behaviours

Resting and swimming were the most common behaviours recorded across all treatments. The amount of swimming varied significantly across tank size (Table 4). Individuals in the large tank swam significantly more than in the jar, small, medium, and barren tank (see Table S1; Supplementary material). On average, over each 600-s trial, individuals in the large tank swam 92 s [CI: 46, 138] more than in the jar; 76 s [CI: 30, 121] more than in the small tank; 75 s [CI: 35, 115] more than in the medium tank; and 53 s [CI: 23, 84] more than in the barren tank (Figure 4A).

Congruent with the results for swimming, individuals rested less in the large tank compared to in the jar, small and medium tank (Table 4), but this was only significant between the large tank and the jar and medium tank (Figure 4, Table S1; Supplementary material). Individuals in the large tank rested, on average, 109 s [CI: 40, 178] less than individuals in the jar (over a 600-s trial), and 104 s [CI: 45, 163] less than the medium tank individuals (Figure 4B).

Figure 4. Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens; n = 13) engaging in (A) swimming, (B) resting, (C1, C2) hovering, (D) stereotypic swimming, (E) nest building, (F) foraging and (G) interacting with the surface, across the five tank treatments (jar, small, medium, large, and large-barren). In the graphs, barren refers to large-barren. Each black dot represents the time spent swimming in each trial by each of the Siamese fighting fish. The large red dots represent the average time spent swimming by tank. The scale of the y-axis of each graph differs.

Less commonly observed behaviours

There was no statistically significant effect of tank on the probability that a fish would hover, however, after excluding the 48 trials where fish did not hover, there was a statistically significant effect of tank on the time spent hovering: fish in the jar hovered 53 s [CI: 20, 85] more than in the small tank; 53 s [CI: 20, 86] more than in the medium tank; and 61 s [CI: 28, 93] more than in the large tank. Individuals in the barren tank hovered significantly more than in the large tank (22 s more [CI: 12, 35]) (Table 4). A remarkably consistent pattern was that for all fish (except Goblin), the time spent hovering reduced between the jar and small tank (Figure 7C).

Stereotypic swimming occurred less in the jar compared to in the small, medium and large tank, but this was only significant between the jar and small tank (Figure 4D, Table 4, Table S1; Supplementary material). Individuals in the small tank, on average, were 33 times [CI: 5, 333] more likely to perform stereotypic swimming than individuals in the jar. Individuals in the barren tanks were 3.4 times [CI: 2, 3] more likely to perform stereotypic swimming than individuals in the large tank. For nesting, there were no statistically significant differences between tanks (Table 4). Looking at the trend, it seemed that nest building was highest in the small tank, and lowest in the jar and barren tanks (Figure 4E).

Foraging occurred significantly more in the large tank compared to in the jar, small, and medium tanks (Figure 4F, Table 4; Table S1; Supplementary material). Individuals in the large tank, on average, were 11 times [CI: 2, 67] more likely to forage than in the jar; 33 times [CI: 4, 200] more than in the small tank; four times [CI: 1, 16] more than in the medium tank; and 15 times [CI: 5, 47] more than in the barren tank.

Interaction with the walls occurred significantly more in the jar and medium tank compared to the large tank (Figure 4G, Table 4, Table S1; Supplementary material). Individuals in the jar, on average, were 4 times [CI: 1, 19.7] more likely to interact with the walls than in the large tank; individuals in the medium tank, on average, were 6 times [CI: 2, 22] more likely to interact with the walls than in the large tank; and individuals in the barren tank, on average, were 3 times [CI: 1, 9] more likely than in the large tank.

Resting place

When looking at the main three tanks (small, medium, and large) with furnishings (plants, hideout barrel, filter), fish spent more time resting on the floor or on furnishings than on the water surface (Figure 5, Table S2; Supplementary material) but there was no significant difference between floor and furnishings. On average, fish spent 49% of their resting time on the floor, 47% on furnishings and 4% at the water surface. Out of 152 trials during which fish rested, they rested on the floor in 135 trials, on furnishings in 130 trials, and on the water surface in 36 trials. More precisely, in the large tank, they rested 46% on the floor, 29% in or on the hideout barrel, 21% on plants and 4% at the water surface (Figure 5). Fish avoided resting on the filter: fish rested on the filter in only one of the 45 trials where there was a filter inside the aquarium.

Figure 5. Percentage of total resting time per trial that the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens; n = 13) spent resting in different places in tanks that had furnishings, by (a) tank size (including small, medium and large tanks only, as these were the only ones with furnishings), and (b) individual fish. The small and medium tanks had plants, while the large tank had plants and a barrel hideout.

Individual differences

The likelihood ratio tests indicate statistically significant random effects for all behaviours, suggesting fish consistently behaved differently from each other (Table 4). Some clear differences include that Naga rested more than other fish, Merlion hovered more than other fish (Figure 6), and Merlion and Ghost rested more on/against plants than other individuals (Figure 5). Wizard, Kraken, Phoenix and Fairy displayed relatively high levels of stereotypic swimming while other individuals such as Pegasus, Naga, Merlion, Kinara, Goblin and Ghost did not perform stereotypic swimming (Figure 6). Further, the type of stereotypic pacing differed; Kraken and Phoenix displayed stereotypic circling, zig-zags and pacing, while Wizard only paced, and Fairy predominantly paced with very little circling. Individual differences, and overall trends, relating to swimming, resting and hovering across tank trials are evident (Figure 7).

Figure 6. The amount of ime that the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens; n = 13) engaged in each behaviour across five tank treatments (jar, small, medium, large, large-barren).

Figure 7 The average amount of time that each of the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens; n = 13) spent (A) swimming, (B) resting and (C) hovering, across each of the five tank trials (jar, small, medium, large, and large-barren), showing overall trends as well as individual behavioural differences. The dotted black line represents the average across all fish.

Discussion

Within the pet industry, Siamese fighting fish face particular welfare concerns as they continue to be displayed and kept in small, barren jars (Sermwatanakul Reference Sermwatanakul2019). This study aimed to determine if Siamese fighting fish are affected by their housing conditions and, if so, recommend suitable measures to improve their welfare.

We demonstrated that Siamese fighting fish behaviour varied significantly depending on their housing environment. Fish were less active in the jar and small tank, with more resting and less swimming compared to in larger tanks. This finding aligns with a previous study on Siamese fighting fish that also showed reduced swimming in smaller tanks (Oldfield & Murphy Reference Oldfield and Murphy2024). Inactivity potentially signifies negative affective states such as ‘boredom’ in captive animals (Meagher & Mason Reference Meagher and Mason2012; Fureix & Meagher Reference Fureix and Meagher2015). In our study, fish also spent more time displaying behaviours that could be classified as ‘abnormal’ in the smaller tanks compared to the larger tanks; performing higher levels of stereotypic swimming in the small tank, which is a well-acknowledged negative response to captive conditions (Miller et al. Reference Miller, Vicino, Sheftel and Lauderdale2020), as well as higher levels of hovering in the jar. Hovering has been reported as ‘abnormal’ if atypical for the species’ natural behavioural repertoire (Casamitjana Reference Casamitjana2004). While hovering in Siamese fighting fish can occur during natural nest building (N Clark-Shen, personal observation 2022), this study clearly differentiated the two, with hovering recorded when occurring independently of nest-building activity. Hovering occurred significantly more in the jar than the small tank, and stereotypic swimming occurred significantly more in the small tank than the jar. Based on these observations, we posit that hovering and stereotypic swimming could both represent similar negative reactions to the captive environment (i.e. indicators of poor welfare), but with different expressions possibly driven by space availability (i.e. it is difficult to perform stereotypic pacing where space is particularly limited such as in the jar). Further, hovering was more likely to be performed in the morning, which was also when fish in larger tanks were most active (i.e. swimming), which further supports that hovering may be expressed when fish feel space-constrained and unable to perform more energetic activity. However, hovering also occurred more in the barren tank compared to the large furnished tank, which suggests that hovering may not only take place when space-constrained but as a result of generally less-optimal tank conditions (i.e. an indicator of frustration). In comparison, fish were most active in the large tank, with more swimming and less resting compared to the smaller tanks. Foraging was also exhibited most in the large tank; foraging is considered a natural behaviour indicative of positive welfare (Troxell-Smith et al. Reference Troxell-Smith, Whelan, Mangle and Brown2017) and has been reported to occur more in larger, more enriched environments (Mallapur et al. Reference Mallapur, Waran and Sinha2005). Fish in the large tank also spent the least amount of time engaging in behaviour which could be classified as ‘abnormal’, such as hovering, interacting with the walls and stereotypic swimming. Overall, these findings are similar to other studies which have found increased natural activity among fish in larger tanks (Polverino et al. Reference Polverino, Ruberto, Staaks and Mehner2016) and increased stereotypic pacing among fish in smaller tanks (Abudusaimaiti et al. Reference Abudusaimaiti, Wang, Luo and Li2020). Aside from these somewhat expected observations, there were also a number of unexpected findings; for example, nest building occurred most in the small tank (although this was not significant), and stereotypic swimming (a negative behaviour) still occurred in the large tank, despite this tank being considered fairly large in size and enriching in nature; potentially highlighting the difficulty of providing truly optimal tank conditions.

Our study also suggests that tank complexity (i.e. furnishings) is important for welfare; fish performed more ‘abnormal’ behaviours (stereotypic swimming, interaction with the walls and hovering) and less ‘positive’ behaviours (foraging and swimming) in the barren tank compared to in the large tank. As the large and barren tanks were the same size, we can reasonably assume that these differences are in response to the complexity of the tank environment. Other studies of captive mammals (Azevedo et al. Reference Azevedo, Cipreste, Pizzutto and Young2023), reptiles (Bashaw et al. Reference Bashaw, Gibson, Shower and Kucher2016), and teleost fishes, sharks and rays (Lawrence et al. Reference Lawrence, Sherwen and Larsen2021), have also found a correlation between more enriched environments, increased expression and diversity of natural behaviours and decreased expression of abnormal behaviours. This correlation is ultimately attributed to a more positive experience for the animals (Lawrence et al. Reference Lawrence, Sherwen and Larsen2021). Physiological studies support these behavioural findings: fish with no environmental enrichment have been found to have significantly higher cortisol levels and opercular beat rates (implying higher stress) than fish exposed to environmental enrichment (Zhang et al. Reference Zhang, Bai, Xu, Guo and Zhang2020).

To further highlight the importance of tank complexity, we found that when given the choice (e.g. in small, medium and large tanks), Siamese fighting fish spent a considerable amount of resting time (47% of total resting time) on or against furnishings (e.g. plants, hideout). Additionally, all fish used all resting space available to them (ground, surface, plants, barrel) showing that a diversity of resting places could be important. Where and how animals choose to sleep are determined by multiple factors, including protection from predators, visibility of surrounding areas, shelter from weather, and thermoregulation (Anderson et al. Reference Anderson, Ang, Lock and Weiche2019), and understanding sleeping preferences in the wild can help to elucidate what is needed to improve rest in captivity (Lukas et al. Reference Lukas, Stoinski, Burks, Snyder, Bexell and Maple2003; Stewart Reference Stewart2011; Lock Reference Lock2012). While the sleeping behaviour of wild Siamese fighting fish remains unreported, they are known to live in slow-flowing, shallow waters, often with dense vegetation (Monvises et al. Reference Monvises, Nuangsaeng, Sriwattanarothai and Panijan2009), and it is plausible that sleeping amidst vegetation offers protection from predators and anchorage in slow-flowing water. Domesticated Siamese fighting fish may or may not carry this instinctive behaviour (for example, chimpanzees [Pan troglodytes] born in captivity build and use nests less than wild-born individuals; Videan Reference Videan2006), nevertheless, we posit that resting in connection with objects makes them feel less exposed and thus safer. This theory is supported by observations during this experiment involving Merlion, a particularly skittish fish who, upon seeing people (e.g. during feeding), would quickly swim behind a plant, suggesting it offered a sense of safety. Siamese fighting fish may also rest in connection with an object for comfort, which is recognised as a factor in resting choices in great apes and elephants (Elephas maximus) (Williams et al. 2015; Zamma & Ihobe Reference Zamma, Ihobe, Nakamura, Hosaka, Itoh and Zamma2015), or stability: it was observed in this experiment that fish in barren tanks that rested on the ground would ‘flop’ to one side, suggesting that objects (gravel, plants, etc) may help with stability, possibly because of their unnaturally elongated fins. This study on Siamese fighting fish did not record whether when the fish was sleeping on the ‘ground’, they were in contact with the tank walls, but in hindsight this was often observed, and it is worth noting that tank walls could play a similar role as objects within the tank – for example, captive draughtsboard sharks (Cephaloscyllium isabellum) show a preference for sleeping in contact with corners of the tank, which they may perceive as safer sleep sites, potentially resembling their resting behaviours in caves and crevices in the wild (Horn Reference Horn2016; Kelly et al. Reference Kelly, Spreitzenbarth, Kerr, Hemmi, Lesku, Radford and Collin2021).

Study limitations

This study only analysed the behaviours of 13 Siamese fighting fish, and more fish would have lent more confidence to the findings. However, ethical considerations influenced the authors to produce results with as low a sample size as possible. Completion of this study with 13 fish saw this aim be achieved, hence more fish were not acquired for the project. Additionally, the order in which the fish experienced each tank differed, and the analysis reveals that this order may have influenced behaviour. Fish spent less time in the barren tank compared to the tanks with furnishings (three days vs seven days), and while this decision was guided by concerns for welfare, a standardised length of time across all tanks would have enabled more robust comparisons. Future studies could consider using a larger sample size and a more standardised rotation of fish through each of the tanks. While the variability of behaviour observed between individuals highlights personality, it also somewhat restricts the ability to generalise findings to the species as a whole and thereby compile species-specific recommendations. Lastly, this study did not examine the optimal ratio of furnishings (i.e. live plants, hideouts) to open space within tanks and so we cannot conclude, for example, that a larger tank with fewer plants would be better for welfare than a slightly smaller tank with more dense plants.

Animal welfare implications and conclusion

Fishes and other aquatic animals are rarely afforded the same level of compassion as other vertebrates (Brown Reference Brown2015) with them often excluded from animal welfare legislations around the world (Berlinghieri et al. Reference Berlinghieri, Panizzon, Penry-Williams and Brown2021). This study has shown that Siamese fighting fish are affected by their housing environment in captivity and thus greater consideration of their living conditions is warranted.

Based on the behaviours observed in this study, the following are recommended to improve housing conditions for male Siamese fighting fish housed by themselves. For fish on display for sale, a minimum tank size of 5.6 L (‘medium’ in this experiment) is recommended. This recommendation stems from the results which show more swimming and foraging (positive behaviours), and less stereotypic pacing and hovering (negative behaviours) in the medium tank compared to the jar and small tank. While bigger is still better, space and maintenance in fish/pet shops may be a barrier to adoption, hence 5.6 L is likely a realistic compromise. For fish kept as pets, tanks larger than 5.6 L (larger than ‘medium’) are recommended. This recommendation comes from the results which show a decline in hovering and stereotypic swimming (negative behaviours) in the medium tank, and the highest amounts of swimming and foraging (positive behaviours) in the large tank.

When on display for sale, and kept as pets, tanks for Siamese fighting fish should not be barren, but should contain gravel/pebbles, as well as plants and other furnishings (e.g. refuges), to stimulate more natural behaviours and give fish a choice for sleeping and hiding spots. This recommendation comes from the fact that Siamese fighting fish in this study spent a considerable amount of time resting on or against furnishings. Additionally, fish displayed more stereotypic swimming and interaction with the walls (negative behaviours) in the barren tank compared to the large tank which had furnishings. The presence of people is a source of stress for captive animals (Morgan & Tromborg Reference Morgan and Tromborg2007), and so it is imperative that animals in pet shops have the ability to retreat to ‘safe spaces’ if needed.

This study did not look at optimal depth for tanks however, as Siamese fighting fish live in shallow-water environments in the wild, we speculate that tanks that are wider/longer than they are deep, are likely more suitable. Importantly, this study revealed that while general behavioural trends were apparent, differences were observed between individuals, and carers for Siamese fighting fish should pay attention to pets’ personalities to determine what would make for the most optimal tank, for example, relating to preferred sleeping spots (i.e. plants vs hideouts), the boldness of the fish (i.e. shyer fish may need more plants and furnishings to feel safe), and health and age (i.e. sick or older fish may need smaller, shallower tanks as they are less mobile).

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/awf.2024.67.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the Rumah Foundation for funding this research project, and Fyssen Foundation for supporting Juliette Tariel-Adam’s through a post-doctoral study grant. We would like to acknowledge Sofiya Shukhova for her illustration of the tank setup (Figure 1) and Lynn Drescher for double-checking video analysis. We thank Andrew P Allen for his advice on multinomial regression model. We would also like to acknowledge the Siamese fighting fish who participated in this research; animals unfortunately never have a choice for their participation in such studies, but we endeavoured to mitigate any animal welfare issues as much as possible.

Competing interest

None.

Footnotes

Author contributions: Conceptualisation: NCS, CB; Data curation: NCS; Formal analysis: JTA, AG; Investigation: NCS; Methodology: NCS, CB; Resources: CB; Validation: CB; Supervision: CB; Writing – original draft: NCS; Writing – review & editing: NCS, JTA, AG, CB.

References

Abudusaimaiti, M, Wang, L, Luo, Y and Li, Z 2020 Effect of tank size on zebrafish behaviour and physiology. Animals 10. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10122353Google Scholar
Alligood, C and Leighty, K 2015 Putting the “E” in SPIDER: Evolving trends in the evaluation of environmental enrichment efficacy in zoological settings. Animal Behaviour and Cognition 2: 200217. https://doi.org/10.12966/abc.08.01.2015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, JR, Ang, MYL, Lock, LC and Weiche, I 2019 Nesting, sleeping and nighttime behaviors in wild and captive great apes. Primates 60: 321332. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-019-00723-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Animal and Veterinary Services 2023 Pet Shop Licence Conditions 5 Display and Sale of Fancy Fish. https://www.nparks.gov.sg/avs/-/media/avs/resources/pet-shop/(5)-fish_avs.ashx#:~:text=5.,containers%20and%20given%20appropriate%20treatment (accessed 19 November 2024).Google Scholar
ASAB Ethical Committee/ABS Animal Care Committee 2023 Guidelines for the ethical treatment of nonhuman animals in behavioural research and teaching. Animal Behaviour 195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2022.09.006Google Scholar
Azevedo, CS, Cipreste, CF, Pizzutto, CS and Young, RJ 2023 Review of the effects of enclosure complexity and design on the behaviour and physiology of zoo animals. Animals 13. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13081277CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barreto, MO, Planellas, SR, Yang, Y, Phillips, C and Descovich, K 2020 Emerging indicators of fish welfare in aquaculture. Reviews in Aquaculture 14: 343361. https://doi.org/10.1111/raq.12601CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bashaw, MJ, Gibson, MD, Shower, DM and Kucher, AS 2016 Does enrichment improve reptile welfare? Leopard geckos (Eublepharis macularius) respond to five types of environmental enrichment. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 184 : 150160. https://doi.org.libproxy1.nus.edu.sg/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.08.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlinghieri, F, Panizzon, P, Penry-Williams, IL and Brown, C 2021 Literality and fish welfare – a review. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105239CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, C 2015 Fish intelligence, sentience and ethics. Animal Cognition 18: 117. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-014-0761-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, C 2019 Pain and emotion in fishes – fish welfare implications for fisheries and aquaculture. Animal Studies Journal 8: 175201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bshary, R 2011 Machiavellian intelligence in fishes. In: Brown, C, Krause, J and Laland, K (eds) Fish Cognition and Behavior 13: 277297. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444342536.ch13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrnes, EE and Brown, C 2016 Individual personality differences in Port Jackson sharks Heterodontus portusjacksoni. Journal of Fish Biology 89: 11421157. https://doi.org/10.1111/jfb.12993CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casamitjana, J 2004 A critical study of UK public aquaria in the year 2004. https://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/document?repid=rep1&type=pdf&doi=a25c1e7af9628d655e381a133081cf1fc8e2fc38 (accessed 22 November 2024).Google Scholar
Dawkins, MS 2003 Behaviour as a tool in the assessment of animal welfare. Zoology 106: 383387. https://doi.org/10.1078/0944-2006-00122CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fureix, C and Meagher, RK 2015 What can inactivity (in its various forms) reveal about affective states in non-human animals? A review. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 171: 824. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2015.08.036CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, RJ 2004 The Betta Handbook (Barron’s Pet Handbooks). BES Publishing: USA.Google Scholar
Horn, P 2016 Biology of the New Zealand carpet shark Cephaloscyllium isabellum (Scyliorhinidae). Journal of Ichthyology 56: 336347. https://doi.org/10.1134/S0032945216030048CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hua Yue, G 2019 The ornamental fish industry in Singapore. Journal of Fisheries of China 43: 116127. https://doi.org/10.11964/jfc.20180911442Google Scholar
Jha, P, Barat, S and Nayak, CR 2006 A comparison of growth, survival rate and number of marketable koi carp produced under different management regimes in earthen ponds and concrete tanks. Aquaculture International 14: 615626. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10499-006-9059-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, ML, Spreitzenbarth, S, Kerr, CC, Hemmi, JM, Lesku, JA, Radford, CA and Collin, SP 2021 Behavioural sleep in two species of buccal pumping sharks (Heterodontus portusjacksoni and Cephaloscyllium isabellum). Journal of Sleep Research 30. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.13139CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleiber, A, Stomp, M, Rouby, M, Ferreira, VHB, Begout, ML, Benhaim, D, Labbe, L, Tocqueville, A, Levadoux, M, Calandreau, L, Guesdon, V and Colson, V 2023 Cognitive enrichment to increase fish welfare in aquaculture: A review. Aquaculture 575. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquaculture.2023.739654CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleiman, DG, Thompson, KV and Baer, CK 2010 Wild Mammals in Captivity: Principles and Techniques for Zoo Management. University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawrence, K, Sherwen, SL and Larsen, H 2021 Natural habitat design for zoo-housed Elasmobranch and Teleost fish species improves behavioural repertoire and space use in a visitor facing exhibit. Animals 11: 2979. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11102979CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lock, LC 2012 Nesting and nighttime behaviours of captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Stirling, UK.Google Scholar
Lukas, KE, Stoinski, TS, Burks, K, Snyder, R, Bexell, S and Maple, TL 2003 Nest building in captive Gorilla gorilla gorilla. International Journal of Primatology 24: 103124. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021450613272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mallapur, A, Waran, N and Sinha, A 2005 Factors influencing the behaviour and welfare of captive lion-tailed macaques in Indian zoos. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 91: 337353. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2004.10.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meagher, RK and Mason, G 2012 Environmental enrichment reduces signs of boredom in caged mink. PLoS One 7: e49180. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0049180CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, LJ, Vicino, GA, Sheftel, J and Lauderdale, LK 2020 Behavioural diversity as a potential indicator of positive animal welfare. Animals 10: 1211. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10071211CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monvises, A, Nuangsaeng, B, Sriwattanarothai, N and Panijan, B 2009 The Siamese fighting fish: Well-known generally but little-known scientifically. Scienceasia 35: 816. https://10.2306/scienceasia1513-1874.2009.35.008 NOCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moorhead, JA and Zeng, C 2010 Development of captive breeding techniques for marine ornamental fish: a review. Reviews in Fisheries Science 18: 315343. https://doi.org/10.1080/10641262.2010.516035CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, KN and Tromborg, CT 2007 Sources of stress in captivity. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 102: 262302. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.03CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Novak, J, Kalous, L and Patoka, J 2020 Modern ornamental aquaculture in Europe: early history of freshwater fish imports. Reviews in Aquaculture 12: 20422060. https://doi.org/10.1111/raq.12421CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oldfield, RG and Murphy, EK 2024 Life in a fishbowl: space and environmental enrichment affect behaviour of Betta splendens. Animal Welfare 33. https://doi.org/10.1017/awf.2024.1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oliveira, RF, McGregor, PK and Latruffe, C 1998 Know thine enemy: fighting fish gather information from observing conspecific interactions. Proceedings of the Royal Society Biological Sciences 265: 10451049. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1998.0397CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pleeging, CCF and Moons, CPH 2017 Potential welfare issues of the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens) at the retailer and in the hobbyist aquarium. Vlaams Diergeneeskundig Tijdschrif 86: 213223. https://doi.org/10.21825/vdt.v86i4.16182Google Scholar
Polverino, G, Ruberto, T, Staaks, G and Mehner, T 2016 Tank size alters mean behaviours and individual rank orders in personality traits of fish depending on their life stage. Animal Behaviour 115: 127135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.03.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reilly, SC, Quinn, JP, Cossins, AR and Sneddon, LU 2008 Behavioural analysis of a nociceptive event in fish: comparisons between three species demonstrate specific responses. Applied Animal Behavioural Sciences 114: 248259. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2008.01.016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sermwatanakul, A 2019 Capacitating the local farmers to enhance global marketing of Thailand’s national Aquatic Animal, the Siamese Fighting Fish. SEAFDEC Fish for the People 17. https://repository.seafdec.org/handle/20.500.12066/5516 (accessed 19 November 2024).Google Scholar
Smith, SA 2014 Chapter 17 - Welfare of laboratory fishes. In: Bayne, K and Turner, PV (eds) Laboratory Animal Welfare pp 301311. Elsevier: USA. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-385103-1.00017-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, SA 2023 Fish welfare in public aquarium and zoological collections. Animals 13: 2548. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13162548CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sneddon, LU 2003 The evidence for pain in fish: the use of morphine as an analgesic. Applied Animal Behavioural Sciences 83: 153162. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(03)00113-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, FA 2011 Why sleep in a nest? Empirical testing of the function of simple shelters made by wild chimpanzees. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 146: 313318. https://doi.org.libproxy1.nus.edu.sg/10.1002/ajpa.21580CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tang, M and Boisclair, D 1993 Influence of the size of enclosures on the swimming characteristics of juvenile brook trout (Salvelinus fontinalis). Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 50: 17861793. https://doi.org/10.1139/f93-200CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toni, M, Manciocco, A, Angiulli, E, Alleva, E, Cioni, C and Malavasi, S 2018 Review: assessing fish welfare in research and aquaculture, with a focus on European directives. Cambridge University Press 13: 161170. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1751731118000940Google ScholarPubMed
Troxell-Smith, SM, Whelan, CJ, Mangle, SB and Brown, JS 2017 Zoo foraging ecology: Development and assessment of a welfare tools for captive animals. Animal Welfare 26: 265275. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.26.3.265CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veldhuizen, LJL, Berentsen, PBM, de Boer, IJM, Van de Vis, JW and Bokkers, EAM 2018 Fish welfare in capture fisheries: A review of injuries and mortality. Fisheries Research 204: 4148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.fishres.2018.02.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Videan, EN 2006 Bed-building in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): the importance of early rearing. American Journal of Primatology 68: 745751. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20265CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, GE and Brown, C 2014 A comparison of spatial learning and memory capabilities in intertidal gobies. Behavioural Ecology Sociobiology 68: 13931401. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-014-1747-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zamma, K and Ihobe, H 2015 Bed making and nocturnal behavior. In: Nakamura, M, Hosaka, K, Itoh, N and Zamma, K (eds) Mahale Chimpanzees: 50 Years of Research pp 583598. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, USA.Google Scholar
Zhang, Z, Bai, Q, Xu, X, Guo, H and Zhang, X 2020 Effects of environmental enrichment on the welfare of juvenile black rockfish Sebastes schlegelii: Growth, behavior and physiology. Aquaculture 518. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquaculture.2019.734782CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens) were housed individually and rotated between five tank treatments during this study. The tank sizes, furnishings and the duration that they were kept in each treatment during the study is presented

Figure 1

Figure 1. Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens) were housed individually in and rotated between jar (1.5 L; three small surface plants and pebbles), small (3.3 L; one medium plant and pebbles), medium (5.6 L; one medium plant and pebbles), large (19.3 L; one large plant, pebbles and one barrel for refuge), and large-barren (19.3 L; no plants or furnishings).

Figure 2

Table 2. The order in which the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens; n = 15) were rotated between five tank treatments during this study (Jar, Small, Medium, Large, and Large-Barren). 1 = first, 2 = second, 3 = third, 4 = fourth, 5 = fifth. ‘EX’ is used where fish were given an exemption from a tank due to exhibiting what was perceived to be a potential inability to cope, ‘(F)’ indicates if a filter was present in the tank, and ‘(D)’ is used if and when a fish passed away

Figure 3

Table 3. Behavioural categories recorded for Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens; n = 15) rotated between five tank treatments. The behavioural categories chosen were based on observed behaviours shown during the recordings themself. The total duration of each behaviour was recorded in seconds

Figure 4

Figure 2. Behaviours observed (A) in each of the five tank treatments (jar, small, medium, large, and large-barren), (B) across different times, (C) with and without a filter in the tank and (D) in the order in which fish were rotated into the tank. Each bar represents an average time spent displaying behaviours by the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens; n = 13) which completed the rotation between the five tank trials.

Figure 5

Figure 3. Results of Principle Component Analysis for the times that the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens; n = 13) spent performing each behaviour across the five tank treatments (jar, small, medium, large, and large-barren).

Figure 6

Table 4. Results of the linear and generalised mixed models on different behaviours across the five tanks (Jar, Small, Medium, Large, and Large-Barren), time (0700, 1000, 1400 and 1800h), and filter (presence and absence) for the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens; n = 13) used in this study

Figure 7

Figure 4. Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens; n = 13) engaging in (A) swimming, (B) resting, (C1, C2) hovering, (D) stereotypic swimming, (E) nest building, (F) foraging and (G) interacting with the surface, across the five tank treatments (jar, small, medium, large, and large-barren). In the graphs, barren refers to large-barren. Each black dot represents the time spent swimming in each trial by each of the Siamese fighting fish. The large red dots represent the average time spent swimming by tank. The scale of the y-axis of each graph differs.

Figure 8

Figure 5. Percentage of total resting time per trial that the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens; n = 13) spent resting in different places in tanks that had furnishings, by (a) tank size (including small, medium and large tanks only, as these were the only ones with furnishings), and (b) individual fish. The small and medium tanks had plants, while the large tank had plants and a barrel hideout.

Figure 9

Figure 6. The amount of ime that the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens; n = 13) engaged in each behaviour across five tank treatments (jar, small, medium, large, large-barren).

Figure 10

Figure 7 The average amount of time that each of the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens; n = 13) spent (A) swimming, (B) resting and (C) hovering, across each of the five tank trials (jar, small, medium, large, and large-barren), showing overall trends as well as individual behavioural differences. The dotted black line represents the average across all fish.

Supplementary material: File

Clark-Shen et al. supplementary material

Clark-Shen et al. supplementary material
Download Clark-Shen et al. supplementary material(File)
File 241.9 KB