Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T20:53:40.751Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Metabolic and behavioural effects of hermit crab shell removal techniques: Is heating less invasive than cracking?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 February 2023

Luis M Burciaga
Affiliation:
Departamento de Ecología y Recursos Naturales, Facultad de Ciencias, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Ciudad de México 04510, México
Guillermina Alcaraz*
Affiliation:
Departamento de Ecología y Recursos Naturales, Facultad de Ciencias, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Ciudad de México 04510, México
*
Author for correspondence: Guillermina Alcaraz, Email:alcaraz@ciencias.unam.mx
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Hermit crabs (Paguroidea; Latreille 1802) offer great opportunities to study animal behaviour and physiology. However, the animals’ size and sex cannot be determined when they are inside their shell; information crucial to many experimental designs. Here, we tested the effects of the two most common procedures used to make crabs leave their shells: heating the shell apex and cracking the shell with a bench press. We compared the effects of each of the two procedures on the metabolic rate, hiding time, and duration of the recovery time relative to unmanipulated hermit crabs. The hermit crabs forced to abandon their shell through heating increased their respiratory rate shortly after the manipulation (1 h) and recovered their metabolic rate in less than 24 h, as occurs in individuals suddenly exposed to high temperatures in the upper-intertidal zone. Hermit crabs removed from their shells via cracking spent more time hiding in their new shells; this effect was evident immediately after the manipulation and lasted more than 24 h, similar to responses exhibited after a life-threatening predator attack. Both methods are expected to be stressful, harmful, or fear-inducing; however, the temperature required to force the crabs to abandon the shell is below the critical thermal maxima of most inhabitants of tropical tide pools. The wide thermal windows of intertidal crustaceans and the shorter duration of consequences of shell heating compared to cracking suggest heating to be a less harmful procedure for removing tropical hermit crabs from their shells.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

Hermit crabs (Paguroidea; Latreille Reference Latreille1802) offer many opportunities to test biological hypotheses that are much more difficult or impossible to test in other groups of animals. Although many animals are used as models to study agonistic interactions, hermit crabs are unique. They are easy to maintain in laboratory conditions and manipulate for experimentation, they are abundant in nature, have well-defined weapons, are combative, and readily fight under laboratory conditions (Gherardi Reference Gherardi2006). Furthermore, the primary resource that they fight over is access to gastropod shells; these shells are a discrete unit whose intrinsic and relative value as a resource are easy to assess. This usage makes hermit crabs uniquely suited for many biological studies. The shell may be a valuable tool for experimental manipulation however it can also represent something of an obstacle given that hermit crabs’ sex and size cannot be determined when they are inside the shell; information which is often deemed necessary to know at the start and/or conclusion of experimentation.

An easy, practical, and non-invasive method of motivating hermit crabs to abandon their shell and switch to an alternative (the characteristics of which are specified according to the researchers’ aims) is to attach a hair clamp to the shell. This increases the shell’s weight and modifies its centre of mass, making walking uncomfortable for the crab and most probably costly energetically; consequently, the hermit crabs ‘voluntarily’ swap to the alternative refuge provided, even when this is not ideal (e.g. too small or large; Arce & Alcaraz Reference Arce and Alcaraz2013; Alcaraz et al. Reference Alcaraz, Toledo and Burciaga2020). However, there are experimental procedures which require the hermit crabs to be ‘naked’ for specific measurements or manipulations prior to starting the trials or after experimentation. Since hermit crabs’ fitness is strongly reliant upon gastropod shells, compelling them to abandon their shell requires exposure to adverse sensations or cracking of the shell to render the crabs naked. Some of the adverse sensations deployed by researchers include induction of osmotic stress by brief immersion of individuals in freshwater (Vance Reference Vance1972; Scully Reference Scully1979), placing them in boiling water (~100ºC; Dominciano & Mantelatto Reference Dominciano and Mantelatto2004), or using electric shocks of gradually increasing intensity (Appel & Elwood Reference Appel and Elwood2009a,Reference Appel and Elwoodb). However, the most frequently used procedures are gradual heating of the shell apex, forcing hermit crabs to abandon their shells (Fotheringham Reference Fotheringham1976a; Alcaraz & Kruesi Reference Alcaraz and Kruesi2009) and cracking of the shell with a bench vice (Fotheringham Reference Fotheringham1976b; Arnott & Elwood Reference Arnott and Elwood2007).

Noxious stimuli, such as heating and stress caused by shell breakage by mechanical pressure, can affect hermit crabs in different ways. A progressive increase in temperature can bring individuals to their maximal tolerance limit, identified as a specific thermal point: the critical thermal maximum (CTMax; Cowles & Bogert Reference Cowles and Bogert1944). Once the CTMax is reached, the individual’s physiological integrity rapidly decreases, such that animals are only able to withstand this temperature for a short time (Lutterschmidt & Hutchison Reference Lutterschmidt and Hutchison1997; Lagerspetz & Vainio Reference Lagerspetz and Vainio2006). CTMax values differ between various species (Beitinger & Lutterschmidt Reference Beitinger, Lutterschmidt and Farrell2011).

The standardised protocol for determining CTMax involves increasing the temperature gradually until an endpoint is reached (Cowles & Bogert Reference Cowles and Bogert1944). For swimming animals, the CTMax can be readily identifiable as a loss of the ability to maintain an appropriate position in response to the gravitation field (Becker & Genoway Reference Becker and Genoway1979); for benthic crustaceans, the CTMax is recognised as the temperature at which individuals lose the ability to maintain customary contact of their appendages with the substrate or the loss of righting response after balance has been lost (Lagerspetz & Vainio Reference Lagerspetz and Vainio2006). When individuals are returned to acclimation temperature just as they reach CTMax, they will recover their functional integrity, evidenced by recovery of the righting response (Lagerspetz & Vainio Reference Lagerspetz and Vainio2006). Similarly to many mobile aquatic animals, crustaceans display an escape response prior to their physiological thermal tolerance limits being reached (Tattersall et al. Reference Tattersall, Sinclair, Withers, Fields, Seebacher, Cooper and Maloney2012); for instance, hermit crabs were seen to abandon their shells before reaching adverse conditions (overheating) when exposed to a gradual temperature increase, as previously reported for pagurids (Taylor Reference Taylor1982).

Shell cracking is the most frequently used procedure for forcible eviction of hermit crabs from their shells (Table 1). Since the gastropod shell is the only defensive barrier offering protection to the hermit crab’s soft abdomen (Hazlett Reference Hazlett1981), naked individuals are extremely vulnerable to predation and damage. Therefore, shell cracking in nature frequently indicates a predation attempt. Fear, pain perception, and stressful experiences commonly generate changes in the individual’s motivational and physiological states (Elwood et al. Reference Elwood, Barr and Patterson2009; Adolphs Reference Adolphs2013; Elwood Reference Elwood2022); as those noxious stimuli become more intense, their effects are accentuated and become more long-lasting (Clinchy et al. Reference Clinchy, Sheriff and Zanette2013; Brown et al. Reference Brown, Demers, Joyce, Ferrari and Chivers2015). Future behavioural decisions in animals depend upon prior experiences and information gained through past events (Denti et al. Reference Denti, Dimant and Maldonado1988; Daws et al. Reference Daws, Grills, Konzen and Moore2002). For instance, injuries sustained and extreme energetic demands during a long-lasting fight led to longer-lasting loser effects compared with defeats after a short fight in which no injuries were sustained (Hsu & Wolf Reference Hsu and Wolf1999; Vieira & Peixoto Reference Vieira and Peixoto2013).

Table 1. Main procedures reported in the literature to evict hermit crabs from their shells

Given the critical importance of the shell for hermit crabs, they are expected to abandon their shells only when exposed to stimuli that under natural contexts could lead to physical injury or death or indicate conditions that are dangerous or strongly unfavourable. Thus, it can be inferred that both heating and cracking are experienced by hermit crabs as strongly negative stimuli. Although these techniques are frequently used, their physiological and behavioural effects have not been evaluated. This study compared the functional and behavioural responses of hermit crabs removed from their shells by heating and cracking, and the recovery time required to return to baseline behaviours following each procedure. Ethically we should use the least harmful manipulations prior to behavioural experiments, but we must always consider the effects of any such manipulations on results. Recent evidence has shown that crustacean decapods, like many animals, display a relatively complex cognitive capacity and respond to noxious stimuli in ways consistent with the experience of pain (Elwood Reference Elwood2019; Conte et al. Reference Conte, Voslarova, Vecerek, Elwood, Coluccio, Pugliese and Passantino2021). Although we cannot be sure whether crustaceans feel pain (Junaid Reference Junaid2015; Diggles Reference Diggles2019), removing the crabs from their shells induces fear-associated behaviours, so we must respect these animals’ experiences and consider their welfare.

Materials and methods

Ethical approval

This study protocol was approved by the Commission of Ethics and Scientific Responsibility, Faculty of Sciences, UNAM (CEARC/Bioética/0307202).

Hermit crab capture

We captured 80 Calcinus californiensis (Bouvier Reference Bouvier1898) hermit crabs of similar size occupying shells of Nerita scabricosta (Lamarck Reference Lamarck1822) with no apparent damage or epibionts in Troncones, Guerrero, Mexico. The length of the chela (4–6 mm) was used as a preliminary measure to estimate the body size of the hermit crabs (Alcaraz et al. Reference Alcaraz, Toledo and Burciaga2020). We kept the hermit crabs in an individual glass flask, which was also used as respirometric chamber (0.03 L), in a circulating seawater system at 27°C and 34 practical salinity unit (PSU).

Preliminary tests

Before starting the experiments, 20 hermit crabs were removed from their shells via two separate procedures: shell heating and cracking (see below). Once the procedures were standardised, the time required to remove hermit crabs from their shells by heating (n = 10) and by cracking the shell (n = 10) was measured. These mean times were used to establish the time for the third treatment: a sham manipulation of the individuals in their shells (n = 10). Hermit crabs’ oxygen consumption and hiding times (described below) were compared among the three groups using one-way ANOVAs. We calculated the effect size (η2) to estimate sample size. The respiratory rate and hiding time of the individuals of the three groups differed significantly (F 2,27 = 8.87; P = 0.001, η 2 = 0.40; F = 10.18; P < 0.001, η 2 = 0.39, respectively). The sample size was then calculated using the wp.rmanova() function of the R package ‘WebPower’ using an alpha level of 0.05, a statistical power of 0.80, and an effect size of 0.40 (Zhang Reference Zhang, Zhang and Yuan2018).

To estimate the temperature that the hermit crabs would experience when being removed from their shells using heat, the temperature increase over time in empty shells was measured. As part of standardising the procedure to force the hermit crabs to abandon their shell by heat, we measured the temperature increase of three empty wet shells following the previously standardised process. First, we drilled a 3.2-mm hole in an empty N. scabricosta shell on the ventral area of the apex using a Dremel multitool (Bosch Tool Corporation, Illinois, US). The shell was rinsed with seawater to remove any dust and residual material. Then, a temperature probe (Elitech GSP-6, Elitech, London, UK) was placed into the hole, with the probe tip resting on the interior wall of the shell apex, just on the other side of the shell wall from the surface where heat was applied (dorsal surface). The shell was held by the experimenter using two fingers while the tip of a hot glue gun (15 W; Truper, Mexico City, Mexico) was placed over the dorsal area of the shell apex. Even in hermit crabs that are completely retracted into their shells, most of the abdomen still remains below the upper coil of the shell (Krans & Chapple Reference Krans and Chapple2005) which is where we applied heat and where the temperature probe was placed. Therefore, the temperature measured by the probe is almost certainly an overestimation of that radiated throughout the rest of the shell, including where most of the individual’s abdomen resides. Furthermore, the hermit crabs’ abdomens may not be in direct contact with the inner wall of the shell. Thus, the temperature estimates represent the maximum temperature to which the individuals could be exposed. We recorded the increase in temperature (T) over time (t) measured by the temperature probe in three different nerite shells of similar size. The shells used were dried for 24 h in an oven at 60°C. Next, we compared the linear regressions of the temperature increase of the interior of the shell as a function of time (heating rates) using an ANCOVA, with shell mass as a covariate. Since the heating rate of the three shells did not differ, the resulting equation was used to estimate the maximum temperature that each individual could be exposed to before abandoning the shell as a function of the time spent heating the shell.

Experimental groups and treatment descriptions

One day after collection, 60 hermit crabs were randomly assigned into four experimental groups in which they were: (i) forced to abandon by shell heating (n = 15); (ii) removed by cracking their shell (n = 15); (iii) handled (n = 15); and (d) unhandled (hereafter control; n = 15; Figure 1). The crabs assigned to the shell-heating group were removed from the water with dissection forceps, then held by the shell with two fingers while the tip of a hot glue gun (15W; Truper) was placed on the dorsal area of the shell apex (see previous section). The heat was applied until the hermit crab abandoned its shell and the time taken for the crab to abandon its shell was measured (from the start of heat application until the hermit crab left the shell and all of its appendages had made contact with the experimenter’s finger). Immediately following abandonment of the shell, the naked hermit crab was placed into the water column of a container (0.04 L) that also served as a respirometer. Individuals were allowed to sink to the bottom and it was noted whether righting position was restored (appendages making contact with the substrate). Each naked crab was then immediately provided with a new, similarly sized (shell length) refuge via placement of the new shell onto the bottom of the tank with forceps.

Figure 1. Effect of treatments on the respiratory rate of hermit crabs. Oxygen consumption before applying any treatment (1 h; Pre-treatment), and after one, 24, and 48 h of the treatment application (Post-treatment; Post-1, Post-24, and Post-48, respectively). The hermit crabs were forced to abandon their shell by heating (squares), removed from their shell by cracking it (triangles), handled by being air-exposed (circles), and unmanipulated control (diamonds). Bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Significant differences are shown with asterisks (P < 0.05).

The crabs assigned to the shell-cracking group were evicted from their shells by placing the shell in a C-clamp (4’, Truper) which was tightened gradually until a cracking sound denoted that cracking had occurred. This procedure left fragments of shell attached to the crab’s body (without apparent damage to the tissue) which the experimenter carefully removed with blunt-tip dissection forceps. The time required to crack the shell, including removing any shattered fragments (from the time the experimenter placed the shell in the clamp until the crab was fully naked and free of fragments) was recorded. As before, it was noted whether the hermit crab recovered its righting position on release into the container of water. Finally, as previously described, a new empty shell of a similar size to the original was made available.

The handling group treatment consisted of taking a hermit crab from its container and keeping it out of the water with the shell aperture (opening) facing upwards for 1 min, which was the mean time required for hermit crabs to be forced to abandon their shell via heat application. The experimenter then placed the crab in an individual respirometric chamber, where its ability to recover righting position was recorded. This experimental group allowed evaluation of the effect of manipulating individuals out of the water. Finally, the crabs assigned to the control group were not manipulated; they were kept in their respirometric chamber without being disturbed.

Measurements before and after treatment application

The immediate and lasting metabolic and behavioural consequences of removing hermit crabs from their shells prior to manipulation and at three points following the treatment was measured. The first measurement was taken 1 h before treatment application (Pre-1) and three measurements were taken after the manipulations, 1, 24, and 48 h after carrying out the assigned treatment (Post-1, Post-24, and Post-48; shell heating, shell cracking, handling, and control; Figure 1).

Pre-treatment measurements (Pre-1)

The same day the hermit crabs were collected they were fed with Tetramin pellets (Spectrum Brands, Wisconsin, US) (± 10% wet weight). After 3 h of feeding any remaining food was removed with a siphon. Individuals were then placed into a clean glass container (0.03 L) immersed in a recirculated seawater system where they remained overnight. The following morning hermit crabs were carefully transferred to clean glass respirometers (0.03 L) and acclimated to the respirometer for 3 h before having their metabolic rate measured. Following this, individuals were removed from the respirometer with dissection forceps and relocated to the tank designed to measure the hiding response (see below). Then, the individuals were returned to the well-aerated recirculating seawater bath (30 L; 27°C, 34 PSU) in an individual’s glass flask, where they remained for 2 h (Figure 1).

Post-treatment measurements (Post-1, Post-24, and Post-48)

After 2 h, the specific treatment was applied to the hermit crab with the individual then placed in a clean respirometer with fresh aerated seawater (27°C and 35 PSU). Next, the respirometer was hermetically sealed and oxygen consumption was measured for 20 min. The hermit crab was then removed from the respirometer and placed into the experimental system designed to measure the hiding response (Post-1 measurements). Finally, the individual was placed into a clean individual container covered with a mesh immersed in the recirculating system (30 L).

Tested hermit crabs were fed for 3 h before being placed into a clean glass container in a recirculated seawater system, where they remained overnight. The following morning, the hermit crabs were carefully moved to clean glass respirometers, where they were kept for 3 h before having their metabolic rate and hiding time measured, as the previous day (Post-24; Figure 1). The next day this procedure was repeated to obtain metabolic rate and hiding time measurements 48 h post-treatment (Figure 1).

Small, magnetic stirrers were used to facilitate the water exchange with the main container during the hermit crabs’ maintenance and acclimation to the respirometer. The crabs remained in these respirometric chambers for 12–15 h prior to being measured and 1 h before taking the measurement the water was gradually changed and faeces were removed with a siphon. The procedure was repeated to measure crab responses to the different treatments at 24 and 48 h post-treatment (Post-24 and Post-48, respectively; Figure 1).

Respiratory rate measurements

The oxygen consumption was measured using a closed respirometric system (Cech & Brauner Reference Cech, Brauner and Farrell2011) using optical oxygen sensors connected to a Witrox 4 (Loligo Systems, Denmark). These sensors were calibrated using a solution of 1% sodium sulphite (0% saturation) and oxygen-saturated seawater (100%) at 27°C. Oxygen saturation remained over 90% during the measurements (Chabot et al. Reference Chabot, Steffensen and Farrell2016). Dissolved oxygen concentration (mg L−1) underwent continuous measurement every second for 20 min and small magnetic stirrers enclosed in plastic mesh at the bottom of the respirometer were used to facilitate water movement.

The rate of oxygen consumption was estimated using the slope of the relationship of oxygen concentration as a function of time; the first 2 min of the data were discarded to avoid variation from placing the hermit crabs into the respirometer (Paschke et al. Reference Paschke, Agüero, Gebauer, Díaz, Mascaró, López-Ripoll and Rosas2018). An empty chamber was used as a control; oxygen consumption values from the empty chamber were subtracted from the respiratory measurements of the hermit crabs (Cech & Brauner Reference Cech, Brauner and Farrell2011). Oxygen consumption was measured during the period of lowest metabolism reported for this hermit crab species (Alcaraz & Kruesi Reference Alcaraz and Kruesi2012) and during the hours of low tide.

Hiding time measurements

Hiding response times were measured immediately following the respiratory measurements. Experiments were carried out in an acrylic tank (41 × 19 × 25 cm; length × width × depth) with a mirror placed at the bottom angled at 45° relative to the horizon thereby enabling reflection of the tank floor to ascertain where the crabs placed their appendages after emerging from their shells. The hermit crabs were removed from the respirometer by hand and held for 10 s with the shell aperture facing upwards to promote complete retraction into their shell (Chávez-Solís & Alcaraz Reference Chávez-Solís and Alcaraz2015). After that, the individuals were placed into the experimental tank with the aperture facing downward. Hiding time was defined as the period between placing the hermit crab on the floor and observing all its appendages making contact with the floor (Briffa et al. Reference Briffa, Rundle and Fryer2008). These experiments were video-recorded which helped enable quantification of hiding time via the reflection from the mirror on the tank floor.

At the end of the experiments, the crabs were extracted from their shells by heating. Cephalothorax length and weight were measured, and sexing based on the position of the gonopores took place. We weighed the shells occupied by the hermit crabs when they were collected (plate balance OHAUS [± 0.01] g; OHAUS Corporation, New Jersey, US) and we calculated the shell size fit to the individual’s body mass (Shell Adequacy Index; SAI; Asakura Reference Asakura1992, Reference Asakura1995). At the end of the experiments, the hermit crabs were provided with a new, suitable shell and returned to the collection site.

Statistical analysis

Body masses of hermit crabs from the four experimental groups were compared using a one-way analysis of variance. It was evaluated whether the time animals took to abandon their shells when exposed to heat depended on their shell fit or sex using a linear model. Time was considered the response variable, sex as a grouping variable, and SAI as a covariate (SAI; Asakura Reference Asakura1992, Reference Asakura1995). This analysis allowed us to assess whether increased value of shells with suitable SAI increases the temperature threshold crabs are willing to endure before abandoning it.

The effect of the treatments on the respiratory rate and the hiding time were compared using linear mixed models (LMMs; lmer package in R) with different analyses performed for the metabolic and behavioural responses. For both analyses, we included the experimental group (shell heating, shell cracking, handling, and control), the time (Pre-1, Post-1, Post-24, and Post-48), and the sex (males and females) as fixed effects; we included individuals in the model as random effects (repeated measures). When significant differences were found, we proceeded to compare between groups with Dunnett’s post hoc test to compare the values of the groups ‘shell heating’, ‘shell cracking’, and ‘handling’ to the ‘control’ group (unmanipulated individuals); individuals were considered to have recovered from the effects of manipulation when they no longer differed significantly from the control group in Dunnett’s tests. Similar analyses were performed to test for differences in the respiratory rate and hiding time and assumptions of normality and homoscedasticity of the variances were verified using graphic exploration of the residuals. The analyses were carried out in R v.3.6.2 (R Core Team 2020).

Results

Preliminary tests

The heating rate of the shell was similar for the three shells tested (ANCOVA; F 2,24 = 0.33; P = 0.72). The equation that described the heating rate was: T = 0.11 × t + 27.13 (R2 = 0.97; P < 0.01), where T is the temperature recorded from the inner wall of the shell and t is the time spent applying heat to the shell apex. Shell mass did not influence heating rate (covariate; F 2,24 = 0.62; P = 0.55). We used the equation to estimate the inner shell temperature when individuals abandoned it as a function of the heating time.

Measurements pre- and post-treatment

The mean (± SEM) body size (mass) of the hermit crabs that were randomly allocated to the four treatment groups was similar (0.22 [± 0.01] g; ANOVA, F 3,56 = 0.03; P = 0.53). The shell temperature at which the individuals abandoned their shell was not related to the Shell Adequacy Index (SAI; R 2 = 0.14; P = 0.75); therefore, the temperature endured by the hermit crabs was not related to the shell quality in terms of fit to body size or to their sex (F 1,11 = 0.87; P = 0.37).

Respiratory rate

The respiratory rate of the individuals from the four experimental groups did not differ before the treatment (Pre-treatment; Dunnett; P > 0.05; Table 2). In all treatments, including the unmanipulated control, mean respiratory rate decreased over time (time; LMM, χ 23,43 = 91.85; P < 0.001; Figure 1). However, the trajectory of this change over time differed among the treatments (significant time × treatment interaction; LMM, χ29,43 = 6.57; P < 0.01). The respiratory rates of the handling and the shell-cracking groups were similar to the control (unmanipulated individuals) over the course of the four records (Dunnett; P > 0.05); however, the respiratory rate of the shell-heating group was higher than the control group 1-h post-treatment (Post-1; Dunnett; P = 0.01; Figure 1). The four treatments did not affect the respiratory rates of males and females differently (LMM, χ 21,43 = 6.57; P = 0.09).

Table 2. Results of Dunnett’s tests comparing the respiratory rate and the hiding time of unmanipulated (control; n = 15) hermit crabs to those exposed to three manipulation treatments: (i) forced to abandon their shell by heating (n = 15); (ii) removed from their shell by cracking it (n = 15); and (iii) handled by being air-exposed (n = 15)

Since it was the reference group, values for the control group are not shown. The metabolic and behavioural responses were measured in the same individuals over time: before being treated (Pre-treated) and 1, 24, and 48 h after the treatment (Pos-1, Pos-24, and Pos-48). The t-value for each Dunnett comparison test is shown; the significant differences are shown in bold (P < 0.05).

Hiding time

The hiding time in response to the startle stimulus of the hermit crabs of the four experimental groups did not differ before applying the treatment (Pre-treatment; Dunnett; P > 0.05; Table 2). The mean time that the hermit crabs remained hidden in their shells decreased over time (time; LMM, χ 23,43 = 19.54; P < 0.001; Figure 2) and hiding time changes differed depending on the treatment applied (interaction: time × treatment; LMM; χ 29,43 = 28.22; P < 0.01). The hiding time of the hermit crabs in the handling group and those removed from their shell by heating were similar to the control group in all four periods (Dunnett; P > 0.05). In contrast, the hiding time of the hermit crabs that were removed from their shell by cracking hid for longer periods than the control 1 and 24 h after treatment (Post-1, Dunnett; P = 0.04; Post-24, Dunnett; P < 0.01). Their hiding time was similar to the control 48 h after treatment (Post-48; Dunnett; P = 0.49). None of the four treatments affected the hiding time response of males and females differently (LMM, χ 21,43 = 6.57; P = 0.09).

Figure 2. Effect of treatments on hiding time. Hiding time before applying any treatment (1 h; Pre-treatment), and after 1, 24, and 48 h of the treatment application (Post-treatment; Post-1, Post-24, and Post-48, respectively). The hermit crabs were forced to abandon their shell by heating (squares), removed from their shell by cracking it (triangles), handled by being air-exposed (circles), and control (diamonds). Bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Significant differences are shown with asterisks (P < 0.05).

Discussion

Challenging short-term environmental stimuli cause changes in individuals’ physiological state that last until the functional machinery returns to the previous stable state (Ramsay & Woods Reference Ramsay and Woods2016). Additionally, animals can respond to challenging situations by avoiding similar future events, possibly by learning, after which they attempt to avoid repeating the exposure to aversive stimuli, energetically costly activities, or dangerous situations by taking specific actions (Bateson Reference Bateson1991; Broom Reference Broom2016). In this study, the hermit crabs responded differently to the two procedures used to remove them from their shells in ways that seem consistent with the events that each stimulus seems to resemble under natural situations. The hermit crabs’ respiratory rate increased when forced to abandon the shell by heating, but their metabolic rate returned to the previous state in a relatively short time (less than 24 h), as commonly occurs in individuals exposed to sudden peaks of temperature (Calcinus laevimanus; Madeira et al. Reference Madeira, Leal, Diniz, Cabral and Vinagre2018). On the other hand, the hermit crabs responded to shell cracking by increasing hiding time after a startle stimulus, and this effect lasted at least 24 h after the treatment application, similar to the expected response after a predation attempt (Brown et al. Reference Brown, Demers, Goldman, Singh, Chivers and Ferrari2020).

The body temperature and metabolic rate of ectothermic animals are positively correlated, and both are determined primarily by the environment (Watling Reference Watling, Watling and Thiel2013). The oxygen capacity limitation of thermal tolerance hypothesis (OCLTT) proposes that when temperature increases rapidly, the oxygen demand exceeds the capacity of the cardiorespiratory and ventilatory systems to meet physiological demand (Pörtner Reference Pörtner2010; Verberk et al. Reference Verberk, Overgaard, Ern, Bayley, Wang, Boardman and Terblanche2016). Thus, the increase in oxygen consumption by the hermit crabs forced to abandon their shell by heating can be explained as a direct consequence of the heat applied during the procedure. Particularly in hermit crabs, the exposure to a linear increase in temperature causes an increase in oxygen consumption and activity levels and induces individuals to protrude from the shell aperture as an adaptive response to adverse conditions (Taylor Reference Taylor1982; Wada et al. Reference Wada, Arashiro, Takeshita and Shibata2010). However, a progressive increase in temperature that exceeds the individual’s physiological limit (ie beyond the temperature at which righting response is lost) can compromise its physiological integrity, leading to irreparable functional damage unless the temperature decreases (Cowles & Bogert Reference Cowles and Bogert1944; Lagerspetz & Vainio Reference Lagerspetz and Vainio2006; Beitinger & Lutterschmidt Reference Beitinger, Lutterschmidt and Farrell2011). However, all of the hermit crabs forced to abandon their shell by heating righted themselves and placed their appendages on the substrate immediately after reaching the bottom of the flask. These behavioural responses are evidence that the individuals were exposed to temperatures below their critical thermal limit (Lagerspetz & Vainio Reference Lagerspetz and Vainio2006). Accordingly, although the oxygen consumption increased after removing the crabs from their shell by heating them (1 h), individuals recovered their metabolic rate in less than a day.

Similarly to other intertidal macroinvertebrates, hermit crabs can overcome acute exposures to high temperatures (Tomanek & Somero Reference Tomanek and Somero1999; Lagerspetz Reference Lagerspetz2003) and acclimate to them within a few hours as an adaptive response to the drastic physical changes caused by tidal rhythm (Nagabhushanam & Sarojini Reference Nagabhushanam and Sarojini1969). The thermal tolerance of intertidal hermit crabs is high, especially in species that inhabit the upper and medium intertidal zones (Taylor Reference Taylor1981; Turra & Denadai Reference Turra and Denadai2001; Kasuya et al. Reference Kasuya, Sanda, Hamasaki, Dan and Kitada2020). For instance, the Critical Thermal Maximum (CTM) of the tropical hermit crab (Clibanarius albidigitus) is 41.5°C (Vinagre et al. Reference Vinagre, Mendonça, Cereja, Abreu-Afonso, Dias, Mizrahi and Flores2018). The mean estimated temperature of the interior shell at which the hermit crabs abandoned their shells was 38°C (98 s). However, hermit crabs wrap their abdomen around the columella while they are retracted into their shell (Krans & Chapple Reference Krans and Chapple2005); thus, the hermit crabs’ abdomen is unlikely to touch the inner wall of the last coils of the shell’s apex where we measured the temperature. This fact, and the complete recovery of the righting response and equilibrium, indicate that C. californiensis abandons its shell at temperatures below its thermal limit.

A potential inconvenience to removing the crabs from their shell via heating is that individuals could place different values on different shells, and therefore be willing to endure a more noxious stimulus to retain it (Appel & Elwood Reference Appel and Elwood2009b). For instance, the hermit crab (Pagurus bernhardus) shows a motivational trade-off associated with the shell quality; individuals in the preferred shell abandoned it at a higher shock intensity than those in poor quality shells (Appel & Elwood Reference Appel and Elwood2009b; Elwood & Appel Reference Elwood and Appel2009). However, in C. californiensis the temperature endured by the individuals was not related to the shell quality, at least in terms of shell fit.

Cracking the shell using a bench press is the other most frequently used procedure to remove hermit crabs from their shell. Fracturing the shell did not affect the crabs’ respiratory rate. Instead, the crabs removed from their shells through cracking spent more time retracted inside their new shell after the shell was cracked relative to untreated individuals; the ones manipulated out of the water, and those forced to leave the shell by heating. Long-lasting motivational changes enable the animal to avoid similar potentially dangerous or harmful situations in the future (Bateson Reference Bateson1991; Elwood et al. Reference Elwood, Dalton and Riddell2017); the greater the noxious stimulus, the more long-lasting the future impact. For instance, individuals suffering injuries during a contest show a loser effect for longer (Hsu et al. Reference Hsu, Earley and Wolf2006; Briffa & Sneddon Reference Briffa and Sneddon2007; Okada et al. Reference Okada, Okada, Dall and Hosken2019). The experience of shell fracture could resemble a life-threatening predator attack, since once their shell is broken the hermit crabs have no other defence to survive a predation attempt. If so, it should not be surprising that, after their shells were cracked, the hermit crabs increased the time spent hiding in their new refuge and that this response lasted for more than 24 h. Alternatively, the hermit crabs could have remained hidden for longer as a camouflage response to reduce the risk of being exposed to another predation attempt in an area where they perceive the risk to be high.

An interesting response was the decrease in metabolic rate during the time the hermit crabs remained in the laboratory. After moving from the field to laboratory conditions, hermit crabs have shown variation in several functional and behavioural responses. For instance, C. californiensis pay a high metabolic cost for using heavy shells in the wild, but after several days under more amenable controlled laboratory conditions, their metabolism recovers and no longer differs from those occupying light shells (Alcaraz & Kruesi Reference Alcaraz and Kruesi2012). Meanwhile, the hermit crab (Clibanarius vittatus) hides longer in its shell after being startled under laboratory conditions compared to individuals in the wild (Gorman et al. Reference Gorman, Ragagnin, McCarthy and Turra2018). Our results highlight the relevance of considering the motivational and physiological changes in captivity versus in the field, and caution is required when extrapolating the results of laboratory experiments to wild animals.

Although several animals present different behavioural or physiological responses to noxious stimuli depending on their sex (e.g. humans: Riley et al. Reference Riley, Robinson, Wise, Myers and Fillingim1998; rats: Cairns et al. Reference Cairns, Hu, Arendt-Nielsen, Sessle and Svensson2001; Craft et al. Reference Craft, Mogil and Aloisi2004; and crustaceans: Appel & Elwood Reference Appel and Elwood2009a), we found no differences between males’ and females’ responses to any of the experimental treatments. This is similar to findings in P. bernhardus, in which there are no sex differences in the probability of leaving the shell due to electroshocks (Magee & Elwood Reference Magee and Elwood2013).

Here, we show that the metabolic and behavioural effects of evicting hermit crabs from their shells by heating and cracking dissipate in 24 and 48 h, respectively. Thus, neither shell heating nor cracking have prolonged effects. It is important to consider these times when designing experimental procedures or releasing hermit crabs after temporary captivity; before these times have elapsed, further experimental manipulations could be subject to biases, and releasing hermit crabs into the wild could increase their vulnerability to natural challenges.

Both procedures can be used to remove hermit crabs from Nerita or Littorina shells (with relatively thin walls). Forcing hermit crabs to leave their N. scabricosta shells by cracking is a relatively fast and straightforward procedure, in part because Nerita gastropods are considered to have the thinnest shell walls among those most frequently used by C. californiensis (Arce & Alcaraz Reference Arce and Alcaraz2011; Chávez-Solís & Alcaraz Reference Chávez-Solís and Alcaraz2015). However, tropical gastropods can have thick shells due to increasing calcium carbonate saturation with increasing water temperature and as an adaptation to the higher predator pressure relative to the lower latitudes (e.g. Littorina obtusata: Trussell Reference Trussell2000; Trussell & Smith Reference Trussell and Smith2000; Watson et al. Reference Watson, Peck, Tyler, Southgate, Tan, Day and Morley2012). In our experience, the cracking procedure can frequently fail when applied to thick shells because some sharp portions can detach and readily damage the hermit crabs’ soft abdomen. Nevertheless, temperate regions are exposed to colder water and lower daily and seasonal temperature variations, so they could also have narrower thermal windows (Vinagre et al. Reference Vinagre, Mendonça, Cereja, Abreu-Afonso, Dias, Mizrahi and Flores2018; Ángeles-González et al. Reference Ángeles-González, Martínez-Meyer, Yañez-Arenas, Velázquez-Abunader, Garcia-Rueda, Díaz, Tremblay, Flores-Rivero, Gebauer and Rosas2020) which could lead to different effects of shell heating. A similar study comparing the shell-heating and cracking procedures could shed light on the consequence of each of these procedures as a function of the hermit crabs’ habitat. Tropical tide pools can reach extremely high temperatures during periods of low tide that prevail until the next tide. Indeed, Vinagre et al. (Reference Vinagre, Mendonça, Cereja, Abreu-Afonso, Dias, Mizrahi and Flores2018) found that tropical tide pools can even function as refuges for intertidal species that are able to tolerate temperatures above the upper thermal limits of their predators. Given this fact and the shorter duration of the consequences, we prefer the shell-heating method to remove hermit crabs from their shells.

Animal welfare implications

The level of stressful experiences determines the strength and retention of their consequences (e.g. level of predatory risk; Brown et al. Reference Brown, Demers, Joyce, Ferrari and Chivers2015). Recent investigations have shown that invertebrates, like other animals, can experience unpleasant sensations and fear-associated behaviours that can be prolonged in memory (Adolphs Reference Adolphs2013; Appel & Elwood Reference Appel and Elwood2009a,Reference Appel and Elwoodb). If the more prolonged physiological and behavioural alterations after the stimulus indicate higher levels of pain, negative, or adverse sensations, heating to remove the hermit crabs from their shell should be used, at least when they are in thin-walled shells in tropical areas. The protection and welfare of the animals used for experimentation or any other purpose (e.g. aquaculture, food, industry) must be thoughtfully considered in the decision-making related to their management. Shell heating appears to be less stressful than shell cracking but still causes stress to the animals. Since hermit crabs demonstrate signs of stress in response to both methods of removing them from their shells careful consideration should be given to using these procedures. Harm-benefit studies should be carried out to consider whether the research’s benefits outweigh the harmful effects of shell removal.

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by the Programa de Apoyo a Proyectos de Investigación e Innovación Tecnológica (PAPIIT-IN218321). We thank the Posgrado en Ciencias del Mar y Limnología and the Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnología (CONACyT) for scholarship no 709354. We thank Carolina Rodríguez-Bobadilla and María Fernanda Pacheco-Pérez for their contributions and Karla Kruesi for their technical assistance. We especially thank Dr Lynna Kiere for reviewing this article and for helping with the English editing.

Competing interests

None.

Footnotes

Author contributions: Conceptualisation: LMB, GA; Data curation: LMB; Formal analysis: LMB; Funding acquisition: GA; Investigation: LMB, GA; Methodology: LMB, GA; Resources: GA; Writing – original draft: LMB, GA; Writing – review and editing: LMB, GA.

References

Adolphs, R 2013 The biology of fear. Current Biology 23: R79R93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.11.055CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alcaraz, G and Arce, E 2017 Predator discrimination in the hermit crab Calcinus californiensis: tight for shell breakers, loose for shell peelers. Oikos 126: 12991307. https://doi.org/10.1111/oik.03742CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alcaraz, G, Chávez-Solís, CE and Kruesi, K 2015 Mismatch between body growth and shell preference in hermit crabs is explained by protection from predators. Hydrobiologia 743: 151156. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10750-014-2029-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alcaraz, G and García-Cabello, KN 2017 Feeding and metabolic compensations in response to different foraging costs. Hydrobiologia 787: 217227. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10750-016-2965-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alcaraz, G and Jofre, GI 2017 Aggressiveness compensates for low muscle strength and metabolic disadvantages in shell fighting: an outcome of the individual’s past. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 71: 111. http://10.1007/s00265-017-2311-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alcaraz, G and Kruesi, K 2009 The role of previous shell occupancy in the wild on laboratory shell choice by the hermit crab Calcinus californiensis. Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology 42: 5562. https://doi.org/10.1080/10236240802663564CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alcaraz, G and Kruesi, K 2012 Exploring the phenotypic plasticity of standard metabolic rate and its inter-individual consistency in the hermit crab Calcinus californiensis. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 412: 2026. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2011.10.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alcaraz, G and Kruesi, K 2019 Niche overlap and resource partitioning between two intertidal hermit crab species. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 99: 135142. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0025315417001850CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alcaraz, G, Toledo, B and Burciaga, LM 2020 The energetic costs of living in the surf and impacts on zonation of shells occupied by hermit crabs. The Journal of Experimental Biology 223: jeb.222703. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.222703CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angel, JE 2000 Effects of shell fit on the biology of the hermit crab Pagurus longicarpus (Say). Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 243: 169184. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-0981(99)00119-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ángeles-González, LE, Martínez-Meyer, E, Yañez-Arenas, C, Velázquez-Abunader, I, Garcia-Rueda, A, Díaz, F, Tremblay, N, Flores-Rivero, MA, Gebauer, P and Rosas, C 2020 Using realized thermal niche to validate thermal preferences from laboratory studies. How do they stand? Ecological Indicators 118: 106741. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolind.2020.106741CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Appel, M and Elwood, RW 2009a Gender differences, responsiveness, and memory of a potentially painful event in hermit crabs. Animal Behaviour 78: 13731379. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Appel, M and Elwood, RW 2009b Motivational trade-offs and potential pain experience in hermit crabs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 119: 120124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2009.03.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arce, E and Alcaraz, G 2011 Shell use by the hermit crab Calcinus californiensis at different levels of the intertidal zone. Scientia Marina 75: 121128. https://doi.org/10.3989/scimar.2011.75n1121Google Scholar
Arce, E and Alcaraz, G 2012 Shell preference in a hermit crab: Comparison between a matrix of paired comparisons and a multiple-alternative experiment. Marine Biology 159: 853862. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00227-011-1861-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arce, E and Alcaraz, G 2013 Plasticity of shell preference and its antipredatory advantages in the hermit crab Calcinus californiensis. Canadian Journal of Zoology 91: 321327. https://doi.org/10.1139/cjz-2012-0310CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Argüelles-Ticó, A, Álvarez, F and Alcaraz, G 2010. Shell utilization by the hermit crab Clibanarius antillensis Stimpson 1862 (Crustacea Anomura) in intertidal rocky pools at Montepio, Veracruz, Mexico. Tropical Zoology 23: 6373.Google Scholar
Arnott, G and Elwood, RW 2007 Fighting for shells: how private information about resource value changes hermit crab pre-fight displays and escalated fight behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 274: 30113017. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2007.1196CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnott, G and Elwood, RW 2010 Signal residuals and hermit crab displays: flaunt it if you have it!. Animal Behaviour 79: 137143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.10.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asakura, A 1992 Population ecology of the sand-dwelling hermit crab Diogenes nitidimanus Terao. 5. Ecological implications in the pattern of molting. Journal of Crustacean Biology 12: 537545. https://doi.org/10.1163/193724092X00021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asakura, A 1995 Sexual differences in life history and resource utilization by the hermit crab. Ecology 76: 22952313. https://doi.org/10.2307/1941703CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, P 1991 Assessment of pain in animals. Animal Behavior 42: 827839. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-60761-880-5_1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, CD and Genoway, RG 1979 Evaluation of the critical thermal maximum for determining thermal tolerance of freshwater fish. Environmental Biology of Fishes 4: 245256. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00005481CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beitinger, TL and Lutterschmidt, WI 2011 Temperature | measures of thermal tolerance. In: Farrell, A (ed) Encyclopedia of Fish Physiology, First Edition pp 16951702. Elsevier: London, UK. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-374553-8.00200-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertness, MD 1980 Shell preference and utilization patterns in littoral hermit crabs of the Bay of Panama. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 48: 116. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0981(80)90002-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertness, MD 1981a The influence of shell-type on hermit crab growth rate and clutch size (Decapoda, Anomura). Crustaceana 197205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertness, MD 1981b Competitive dynamics of a tropical hermit crab assemblage. Ecology 62: 751761. https://doi.org/10.2307/1937743CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertness, MD 1981c Interference, exploitation, and sexual components of competition in a tropical hermit crab assemblage. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 49: 189202. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0981(81)90070-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Billock, WL and Dunbar, SG 2009 Influence of motivation on behaviour in the hermit crab, Pagurus samuelis. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 89: 775779. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0025315408002543CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouvier, EL 1898 Sur quelques Crustacés anomures et brachyures recueillis par M. Diguet en Basse-Californie. Bulletin du Museum d’Histoire Naturelle 4: 371384.Google Scholar
Bridger, D, Bonner, SJ and Briffa, M 2015 Individual quality and personality: bolder males are less fecund in the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 282: 20142492. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.2492CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briffa, M and Austin, M 2009 Effects of predation threat on the structure and benefits from vacancy chains in the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus. Ethology 115: 10291035. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01687.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briffa, M, Bridger, D and Biro, PA 2013 How does temperature affect behaviour? Multilevel analysis of plasticity, personality and predictability in hermit crabs. Animal Behaviour 86: 4754. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.04.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briffa, M and Dallaway, D 2007 Inter-sexual contests in the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus: females fight harder but males win more encounters. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 61: 17811787. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-007-0411-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briffa, M and Elwood, RW 2001 Motivational change during shell fights in the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus. Animal Behaviour 62: 505510. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2001.1764CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briffa, M and Elwood, RW 2002 Power of shell–rapping signals influences physiological costs and subsequent decisions during hermit crab fights. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 269: 23312336. https://doi.org/10.1098%2Frspb.2002.2158CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briffa, M and Elwood, RW 2004 Use of energy reserves in fighting hermit crabs. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 271: 373379. http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2003.2633CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briffa, M and Fortescue, KJ 2017 Motor pattern during fights in the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus: evidence for the role of skill in animal contests. Animal Behaviour 128: 1320. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.03.031CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briffa, M, Rundle, SD and Fryer, A 2008 Comparing the strength of behavioural plasticity and consistency across situations: animal personalities in the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 275: 13051311. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.0025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briffa, M and Sneddon, LU 2007 Physiological constraints on contest behaviour. Functional Ecology 21: 627637. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2435.2006.01188.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briffa, M and Twyman, C 2011 Do I stand out or blend in? Conspicuousness awareness and consistent behavioural differences in hermit crabs. Biology letters 7: 330332. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2010.0761CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briffa, M and Williams, R 2006 Use of chemical cues during shell fights in the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus. Behaviour 143: 12811290. http://doi.org/10.1163/156853906778691577CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broom, DM 2016 Considering animals’ feelings animal sentience: Précis of sentience and animal welfare. Animal Sentience 1: 1. https://doi.org/10.51291/2377-7478.1015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, GE, Demers, EE, Joyce, BJ, Ferrari, MCO and Chivers, DP 2015 Retention of neophobic predator recognition in juvenile convict cichlids: effects of background risk and recent experience. Animal Cognition 18: 13311338. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0902-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, GE, Demers, EEM, Goldman, JA, Singh, A, Chivers, DP and Ferrari, MCO 2020 Unpredictable risk enhances induced neophobia in northern red-bellied dace. Animal Behaviour 168: 121127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2020.08.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bulinski, KV 2007 Shell-selection behavior of the hermit crab Pagurus granosimanus in relation to isolation, competition, and predation. Journal of Shellfish Research 26: 233239. https://doi.org/10.2983/0730-8000(2007)26[233:SBOTHC]2.0.CO;2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burciaga, LM, Alvarez, A and Alcaraz, G 2021 Same resource, different benefits: hermit crab shell structure advantages owners, but not intruders in agonistic interactions. Hydrobiologia 848: 25392550. http://10.1007/s10750-021-04573-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cairns, BE, Hu, JW, Arendt-Nielsen, L, Sessle, BJ and Svensson, P 2001 Sex-related differences in human pain and rat afferent discharge evoked by injection of glutamate into the masseter muscle. Journal of Neurophysiology 86: 782791. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.2001.86.2.782CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cech, JJ and Brauner, CJ 2011 Ventilation and animal respiration. In: Farrell, AP (ed) Techniques in Whole Animal Respiratory Physiology. Encyclopedia of Fish Physiology: From Genome to Environment, First Edition pp 846863. Academic Press: London UK. https://10.1016/B978-0-12-374553-8.00128-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chabot, D, Steffensen, JF and Farrell, AP 2016 The determination of standard metabolic rate in fishes. Journal of Fish Biology 88: 81121. https://doi.org/10.1111/jfb.12845CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chase, ID, Weissburg, M and Dewitt, TH 1988 The vacancy chain process: a new mechanism of resource distribution in animals with application to hermit crabs. Animal Behaviour 36: 12651274. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(88)80195-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chávez-Solís, CE and Alcaraz, G 2015 Hiding time of the hermit crab Calcinus californiensis: Observations from different shell types. Canadian Journal of Zoology 93: 559565. https://doi.org/10.1139/cjz-2014-0312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clinchy, M, Sheriff, MJ and Zanette, LY 2013 Predator-induced stress and the ecology of fear. Functional Ecology 27: 5665. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2435.12007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conte, F, Voslarova, E, Vecerek, V, Elwood, RW, Coluccio, P, Pugliese, M and Passantino, A 2021 Humane slaughter of edible decapod crustaceans. Animals 11: 113. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11041089CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Courtene-Jones, W and Briffa, M 2014 Boldness and asymmetric contests: role-and outcome-dependent effects of fighting in hermit crabs. Behavioral Ecology 25: 10731082. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/aru085CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowles, RB and Bogert, CM 1944 A preliminary study of the thermal requirements of desert reptiles. Bulletin of American Museum of Natural History 83: 261296Google Scholar
Craft, RM, Mogil, JS and Aloisi, AM 2004 Sex differences in pain and analgesia: the role of gonadal hormones. European Journal of Pain 8: 397411. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2004.01.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crump, A, Mullens, C, Bethell, EJ, Cunningham, EM and Arnott, G 2020 Microplastics disrupt hermit crab shell selection. Biology Letters 16: 20200030. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2020.0030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cunningham, EM, Mundye, A, Kregting, L, Dick, JT, Crump, A, Riddell, G and Arnott, G 2021 Animal contests and microplastics: evidence of disrupted behaviour in hermit crabs Pagurus bernhardus. Royal Society Open Science 8: 211089. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.211089CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daws, A, Grills, J, Konzen, K and Moore, P 2002 Previous experiences alter the outcome of aggressive interactions between males in the crayfish, Procambarus Clarkii. Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology 35: 139148. https://doi.org/10.1080/1023624021000014725CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De la Haye, KL, Spicer, JI, Widdicombe, S and Briffa, M 2011 Reduced sea water pH disrupts resource assessment and decision making in the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus. Animal Behaviour 82: 495501. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.05.030CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denti, A, Dimant, B and Maldonado, H 1988 Passive avoidance learning in the crab Chasmagnathus granulatus. Physiology and Behavior 43: 317320. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(88)90194-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diggles, BK 2019 Review of some scientific issues related to crustacean welfare. ICES Journal of Marine Science 76: 6681. https://doi.org/10.1093/icesjms/fsy058CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dominciano, LCC and Mantelatto, FLM 2004 The influence of shell species and size on the shell selection pattern of Paguristes tortugae (Decapoda, Diogenidae) from Anchieta Island (Ubatuba, Brazil). Iheringia. Série Zoologia 94: 425428. https://doi.org/10.1590/s0073-47212004000400012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elwood, RW 2019 Discrimination between nociceptive reflexes and more complex responses consistent with pain in crustaceans Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 374: 20190368. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0368CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elwood, RW 2022 Hermit crabs, shells, and sentience. Animal Cognition. 117. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-022-01607-7Google ScholarPubMed
Elwood, RW and Appel, M 2009 Pain experience in hermit crabs? Animal Behaviour 77: 12431246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.01.028CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elwood, RW, Barr, S and Patterson, L 2009 Pain and stress in crustaceans? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 118: 128136. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elwood, RW, Dalton, N and Riddell, G 2017 Aversive responses by shore crabs to acetic acid but not to capsaicin. Behavioural Processes 140: 15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2017.03.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elwood, RW and Glass, CW 1981 Negotiation or aggression during shell fights of the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus?. Animal behaviour 29: 12391244. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(81)80075-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elwood, R and Kennedy, H 1988 Sex differences in shell preferences of the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus L. The Irish Naturalists’ Journal 22: 436440Google Scholar
Elwood, RW, Marks, N and Dick, JTA 1995 Consequences of shell-species preferences for female reproductive success in the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus. Marine Biology 123: 431434. http://doi.org/10.1007/BF00349221CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elwood, RW, McClean, A and Webb, L 1979 The development of shell preferences by the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus. Animal Behaviour 27: 940946. http://doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(79)90032-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elwood, RW, Pothanikat, RME and Briffa, M 2006 Honest and dishonest displays, motivational state and subsequent decisions in hermit crab shell fights. Animal Behaviour 72: 853859. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.01.025CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elwood, RW and Stewart, A 1985 The timing of decisions during shell investigation by the hermit crab, Pagurus bernhardus. Animal Behaviour 33: 620627. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(85)80086-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fotheringham, N 1976a Effects of shell stress on the growth of hermit crabs. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 23: 299305. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0981(76)90027-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fotheringham, N 1976b Population consequences of shell utilization by hermit crabs. Ecology 57: 570578. https://doi.org/10.2307/1936441CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia, RB and Mantelatto, FLM 2001 Shell selection by the tropical hermit crab Calcinus tibicen (Herbst, 1791) (anomura, diogenidae) from southern Brazil. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 265: 114. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-0981(01)00321-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gherardi, F 2006 Fighting behaviour in hermit crabs: the combined effect of resource-holding potential and resource value in Pagurus longicarpus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 59: 500510. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-005-0074-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorman, D, Ragagnin, MN, McCarthy, ID and Turra, A 2018 Risk-taking and risk-avoiding behaviors by hermit crabs across multiple environmental contexts. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 506: 2529. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2018.05.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grant, WC Jr and Ulmer, KM 1974 Shell selection and aggressive behavior in two sympatric species of hermit crabs. The Biological Bulletin 146: 3243. http://doi.org/10.2307/1540395CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hahn, DR 1998 Hermit crab shell use patterns: response to previous shell experience and to water flow. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 228: 3551. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-0981(98)00002-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hazlett, BA 1981 The behavioral ecology of hermit crabs. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 12: 122. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.es.12.110181.000245CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hazlett, BA 1996 Assessments during shell exchanges by the hermit crab Clibanarius vittatus: the complete negotiator. Animal Behaviour 51: 567573. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1996.0060CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hazlett, BA and Bach, CE 2010 Microhabitat distribution of the hermit crabs Calcinus haigae and Calcinus hazletti (Decapoda: Anomura: Diogenidae). Pacific Science 64: 441447. https://doi.org/10.2984/64.3.441CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, Y, Earley, RL and Wolf, LL 2006 Modulation of aggressive behaviour by fighting experience: Mechanisms and contest outcomes. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 81: 3374. https://doi.org/10.1017/S146479310500686XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsu, Y and Wolf, LL 1999 The winner and loser effect: Integrating multiple experiences. Animal Behaviour 57: 903910. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1998.1049CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Junaid, J 2015 Animal suffering: From science to law: International Symposium. Canadian Veterinary Journal 56: 830Google Scholar
Kasuya, N, Sanda, T, Hamasaki, K, Dan, S and Kitada, S 2020 Upper thermal tolerance of early juveniles of six terrestrial hermit crab species. Animal Biology 70: 3953. https://doi.org/10.1163/15707563-20191119CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krans, JL and Chapple, WD 2005 Variability of motoneuron activation and the modulation of force production in a postural reflex of the hermit crab abdomen. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 191: 761775. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-005-0631-yCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lagerspetz, KYH 2003 Thermal acclimation without heat shock, and motor responses to a sudden temperature change in Asellus aquaticus. Journal of Thermal Biology 28: 421427. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4565(03)00027-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lagerspetz, KYH and Vainio, LA 2006 Thermal behaviour of crustaceans. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 81: 237258. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1464793105006998CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamarck, JB 1822 Histoire naturelle des animaux sans vertébres pp 194. Paris, FranceGoogle Scholar
Lane, SM and Briffa, M 2020 The role of spatial accuracy and precision in hermit crab contests. Animal Behaviour 167: 111118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2020.07.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lane, SM, Cornwell, TO and Briffa, M 2022 The angle of attack: rapping technique predicts skill in hermit crab contests. Animal Behaviour 187: 5561. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2022.02.017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Latreille, PA 1802 Histoire Naturelle, Générale et Particuliere des Crustaces et des Insectes pp 468. Dufart:Paris, France.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liszka, D and Underwood, AJ 1990 An experimental design to determine preferences for gastropod shells by a hermit-crab. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 137: 4762. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0981(90)90059-LCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lutterschmidt, WI and Hutchison, VH 1997 The critical thermal maximum: History and critique. Canadian Journal of Zoology 75: 15611574. https://doi.org/10.1139/z97-783CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madeira, C, Leal, MC, Diniz, MS, Cabral, HN and Vinagre, C 2018 Thermal stress and energy metabolism in two circumtropical decapod crustaceans: Responses to acute temperature events. Marine Environmental Research 141: 148158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.marenvres.2018.08.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magee, B and Elwood, RW 2013 Shock avoidance by discrimination learning in the shore crab (Carcinus maenas) is consistent with a key criterion for pain. Journal of Experimental Biology 216: 353358. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.072041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClintock, TS 1985 Effects of shell condition and size upon the shell choice behavior of a hermit crab. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 88: 271285. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0981(85)90235-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mowles, SL, Cotton, PA and Briffa, M 2010 Whole-organism performance capacity predicts resource-holding potential in the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus. Animal Behaviour 80: 277282. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.05.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mowles, SL, Cotton, PA and Briffa, M 2012 Consistent crustaceans: the identification of stable behavioural syndromes in hermit crabs. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 66: 10871094. http://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-012-1359-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagabhushanam, R and Sarojini, R 1969 Effect of temperature and salinity on the heat tolerance in the hermit crab, Diogenes bicristimanus. Hydrobiologia 34: 126134. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00040328CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neil, SJ 1985 Size assessment and cues: studies of hermit crab contests. Behaviour 92: 2237. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853985X00361CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Okada, K, Okada, Y, Dall, SRX and Hosken, DJ 2019 Loser-effect duration evolves independently of fighting ability. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 286: 20190582. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.0582CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paschke, K, Agüero, J, Gebauer, P, Díaz, F, Mascaró, M, López-Ripoll, E and Rosas, C 2018 Comparison of aerobic scope for metabolic activity in aquatic ectotherms with temperature related metabolic stimulation: a novel approach for aerobic power budget. Frontiers in Physiology 9: 1438. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2018.01438CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pechenik, JA, Hsieh, J, Owara, S, Wong, P, Marshall, D, Untersee, S and Li, W 2001 Factors selecting for avoidance of drilled shells by the hermit crab Pagurus longicarpus. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 262: 7589. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-0981(01)00284-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pörtner, HO 2010 Oxygen- and capacity-limitation of thermal tolerance: A matrix for integrating climate-related stressor effects in marine ecosystems. Journal of Experimental Biology 213: 881893. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.037523CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team 2020 R Core Team. A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria.Google Scholar
Ramsay, DS and Woods, SC 2016 Physiological regulation: how it really works. Cell Metabolism 24: 361364. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cmet.2016.08.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riley, JL III, Robinson, ME, Wise, EA, Myers, CD and Fillingim, RB 1998 Sex differences in the perception of noxious experimental stimuli: a meta-analysis. Pain 74: 181187. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(97)00199-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rimmer, JE, Todd, CD and Shuker, DM 2021 Context-dependent use of visual cues in the shell selection behaviour of the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus. Behavioural Processes 188: 104414. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2021.104414CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rittschof, D and Hazlett, BA 1997 Behavioural responses of hermit crabs to shell cues, predator haemolymph and body odour. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 77: 737751. https://doi.org/10.1017/S002531540003616XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotjan, RD, Blum, J and Lewis, SM 2004 Shell choice in Pagurus longicarpus hermit crabs: does predation threat influence shell selection behavior? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 56: 171176. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-004-0770-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scully, EP 1979 The effects of gastropod shell availability and habitat characteristics on shell utilization by the intertidal hermit crab Pagurus longicarpus Say. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 37: 139152. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0981(79)90091-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suárez-Rodríguez, M, Kruesi, K and Alcaraz, G 2019 The shadow of the shell: A cue for a new home. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 99: 11651169. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0025315419000122CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tattersall, GJ, Sinclair, BJ, Withers, PC, Fields, PA, Seebacher, F, Cooper, CE and Maloney, SK 2012 Coping with thermal challenges: physiological adaptations to environmental temperatures. Comprehensive Physiology 2: 21512202. https://doi.org/10.1002/cphy.c110055CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, PR 1981 Hermit crab fitness: The effect of shell condition and behavioral adaptations on environmental resistance. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 52: 205218. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0981(81)90037-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, PR 1982 Environmental resistance and the ecology of coexisting hermit crabs: Thermal tolerance. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 57: 229236. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0981(82)90194-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tidau, S and Briffa, M 2019 Distracted decision makers: ship noise and predation risk change shell choice in hermit crabs. Behavioral Ecology 30: 11571167. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arz064CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomanek, L and Somero, GN 1999 Evolutionary and acclimation-induced variation in the heat-shock responses of congeneric marine snails (genus Tegula) from different thermal habitats: Implications for limits of thermotolerance and biogeography. Journal of Experimental Biology 202: 29252936. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.202.21.2925CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tricarico, E and Gherardi, F 2006 Shell acquisition by hermit crabs: which tactic is more efficient? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 60: 492500. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-006-0191-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tricarico, E and Gherardi, F 2007 Resource assessment in hermit crabs: the worth of their own shell. Behavioral Ecology 18: 615620. http://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arm019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trussell, GC 2000 Phenotypic clines, plasticity, and morphological trade‐offs in an intertidal snail. Evolution 54: 151166. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0014-3820.2000.tb00016.xGoogle Scholar
Trussell, CG and Smith, LD 2000 Induced defenses in response to an invading crab predator: An explanation of historical and geographic phenotypic change. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 97: 21232127. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.040423397CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turra, A and Denadai, MR 2001 Desiccation tolerance of four sympatric tropical intertidal hermit crabs (Decapoda, anomura). Marine and Freshwater Behaviour and Physiology 34: 227238. https://doi.org/10.1080/10236240109379076CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turra, A and Gorman, D 2014 Subjective resource value and shell abandoning behavior in hermit crabs. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 452: 137142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2013.12.018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turra, A and Leite, FPP 2004 Shell-size selection by intertidal sympatric hermit crabs. Marine Biology 145: 251257. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00227-004-1323-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valère-Rivet, MG, Juma, D and Dunbar, SG 2017 Thermal tolerance of the hermit crab Pagurus samuelis subjected to shallow burial events. Crustacean Research 46: 6582. https://doi.org/10.18353/crustacea.46.0_65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vance, RR 1972 The role of shell adequacy in behavioral interactions involving hermit crabs. Ecology 53: 10751083. https://doi.org/10.2307/1935419CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velasque, M and Briffa, M 2016 The opposite effects of routine metabolic rate and metabolic rate during startle responses on variation in the predictability of behaviour in hermit crabs. Behaviour 153: 15451566. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003371CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verberk, WCEP, Overgaard, J, Ern, R, Bayley, M, Wang, T, Boardman, L and Terblanche, JS 2016 Does oxygen limit thermal tolerance in arthropods? A critical review of current evidence. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology -Part A: Molecular and Integrative Physiology 192: 6478. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpa.2015.10.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vieira, MC and Peixoto, PECC 2013 Winners and losers: A meta-analysis of functional determinants of fighting ability in arthropod contests. Functional Ecology 27: 305313. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2435.12051CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vinagre, C, Mendonça, V, Cereja, R, Abreu-Afonso, F, Dias, M, Mizrahi, D and Flores, AAV 2018 Ecological traps in shallow coastal waters-potential effect of heat-waves in tropical and temperate organisms. PLoS ONE 13: 117. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0192700CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wada, S, Arashiro, Y, Takeshita, F and Shibata, Y 2010 Male mate choice in hermit crabs: prudence by inferior males and simple preference by superior males. Behavioral Ecology 22: 114119. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arq1834CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watling, S 2013 Feeding and digestive system. In: Watling, L and Thiel, M (eds) Functional Morphology and Diversity. The Natural History of the Crustacea, First Edition pp 237260. Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, SA, Peck, LS, Tyler, PA, Southgate, PC, Tan, KS, Day, RW and Morley, SA 2012 Marine invertebrate skeleton size varies with latitude, temperature and carbonate saturation: Implications for global change and ocean acidification. Global Change Biology 18: 30263038. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2486.2012.02755.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilber, TP 1989 Associations between gastropod shell characteristics and egg production in the hermit crab Pagurus longicarpus. Oecologia 81: 615. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00377002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilber, TP 1990 Influence of size, species and damage on shell selection by the hermit crab Pagurus longicarpus. Marine Biology 104: 3139. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01313154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoshino, K, Goshima, S and Nakao, S 1999 The interaction between shell size and shell species preferences of the hermit crab Pagurus filholi. Benthos research 54: 3744. https://doi.org/10.5179/benthos1996.54.1_37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoshino, K, Goshima, S and Nakao, S 2002 Temporal reproductive patterns within a breeding season of the hermit crab Pagurus filholi: effects of crab size and shell species. Marine Biology 141: 10691075. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00227-002-0876-8Google Scholar
Zhang, Z 2018 Statistical Power Analysis for One-Way ANOVA. In: Zhang, Z and Yuan, KH (eds) Practical statistical power analysis using Webpower and R pp 6983. ISDSA Press: Indiana, USA.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Main procedures reported in the literature to evict hermit crabs from their shells

Figure 1

Figure 1. Effect of treatments on the respiratory rate of hermit crabs. Oxygen consumption before applying any treatment (1 h; Pre-treatment), and after one, 24, and 48 h of the treatment application (Post-treatment; Post-1, Post-24, and Post-48, respectively). The hermit crabs were forced to abandon their shell by heating (squares), removed from their shell by cracking it (triangles), handled by being air-exposed (circles), and unmanipulated control (diamonds). Bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Significant differences are shown with asterisks (P < 0.05).

Figure 2

Table 2. Results of Dunnett’s tests comparing the respiratory rate and the hiding time of unmanipulated (control; n = 15) hermit crabs to those exposed to three manipulation treatments: (i) forced to abandon their shell by heating (n = 15); (ii) removed from their shell by cracking it (n = 15); and (iii) handled by being air-exposed (n = 15)

Figure 3

Figure 2. Effect of treatments on hiding time. Hiding time before applying any treatment (1 h; Pre-treatment), and after 1, 24, and 48 h of the treatment application (Post-treatment; Post-1, Post-24, and Post-48, respectively). The hermit crabs were forced to abandon their shell by heating (squares), removed from their shell by cracking it (triangles), handled by being air-exposed (circles), and control (diamonds). Bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Significant differences are shown with asterisks (P < 0.05).