Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T07:05:02.734Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Predictors of successful diversion of cats and dogs away from animal shelter intake: Analysis of data from a self-rehoming website

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 February 2023

Lexis H Ly*
Affiliation:
Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, University of British Columbia, 2357 Main Mall, Vancouver, BC, Canada, V6T 1Z4
Alexandra Protopopova
Affiliation:
Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, University of British Columbia, 2357 Main Mall, Vancouver, BC, Canada, V6T 1Z4
*
Author for correspondence: Lexis H Ly, Email: lexisly@mail.ubc.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

As animals experience distress in animal shelters, leaders call for increased efforts to divert intake of companion animals away from shelters. One novel intake diversion strategy is supported self-rehoming, where owners find new homes for their animals without surrendering to a physical shelter. This study aimed to identify predictors of successful diversion of animals through the AdoptaPet.com ‘Rehome’ online platform. Data for dogs (n = 100,342) and cats (n = 48,484) were analysed through logistic regression to assess the association of animal- and owner-related factors and outcome. Overall, 87.1% of dogs and 85.7% of cats were successfully diverted from animal shelters, out of which, 37.8% of dogs and 35.3% of cats were kept by their original owner. Multiple animal-related factors predicted increased odds of diversion (e.g. younger, smaller). Dog and cat owners who set a longer rehoming deadline (i.e. > 8 weeks) were over twice as likely to keep or adopt out their animal. Dog owners who surrendered for owner-related reasons had increased odds of diversion in comparison to animal behaviour issues. We conclude that online-supported, self-rehoming platforms provide pet owners with an alternative to relinquishment that may reduce the intake of animals to shelters; however, owners with animals that are not preferred by adopters may have to decide whether to keep their animal or relinquish their animal to a shelter or rescue. These results provide guidance for animal shelter professionals on the likelihood of successful diversion programmes given certain animal and owner characteristics.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

Animal shelters serve their communities through protecting animals and people from each other, supporting the human-animal relationship, and providing animal care (Zawistowski et al. Reference Zawistowski, Morris, Salman and Ruch-Gallie1998; Hurley Reference Hurley2022). However, animal shelters must remain at a reasonable number of animals inside its facility (i.e. capacity) to preserve resources, provide sufficient care for animals within the facility, and respond to community needs (Hobson et al. Reference Hobson, Bateman, Coe, Oblak and Veit2021). Capacity remains a critical issue in animal shelters (Horecka & Neal Reference Horecka and Neal2022), and many shelters are overcapacity due to a higher number of intakes compared to outcomes of the animals in their care. One way that shelters intake animals to facilities is through owner surrender, which makes up approximately 25–35% of shelter intake in North America (Humane Canada 2017; Shelter Animals Count 2022). While the circumstances leading to surrender are often multifaceted, recent research indicates that owner-related reasons (e.g. financial stress, difficulties finding pet-friendly housing) are more common primary reasons for surrender in comparison to animal behaviour issues (Alberthsen et al. Reference Alberthsen, Rand, Morton, Bennett, Paterson and Vankan2016; Jensen et al. Reference Jensen, Sandøe and Nielsen2020; Eagan et al. Reference Eagan, Gordon and Protopopova2022).

In response to concerns about capacity issues, animal shelters often make management decisions to reduce intake and increase adoption of animals in shelter facilities. For example, Capacity for Care (C4C) is a shelter management programme that recommends various procedures that aim to increase the speed at which animals can be adopted from the shelter, such as holding adoption specials and reducing the time it takes for an animal to be moved to the adoption floor (Hobson et al. Reference Hobson, Bateman, Coe, Oblak and Veit2021). As an additional measure to reduce capacity, many animal shelters have recently shifted toward managed intake, where animals are admitted on an appointment-basis to regulate the flow of animals (Hurley Reference Hurley2022). For animals that are not admitted to facilities immediately, animal shelters may use ‘intake diversion’ programmes, which are strategies that provide alternatives to intake, thereby removing the need for animals to enter the shelter altogether (Hurley Reference Hurley2022). Often, intake diversion interventions are based upon common reasons for surrender (Best Friends Animal Society 2019). For example, one common reason for which owners surrender animals to shelters is due to having ‘too many’ animals, supposedly the result of unwanted litters (Lambert et al. Reference Lambert, Coe, Young, Dysart, Nogueira Borden and Rajić2014; Eagan et al. Reference Eagan, Gordon and Protopopova2022). To address this reason directly, animal shelters and veterinary clinics may offer low-cost or free spaying/neutering services in order to reduce the number of unwanted litters in the community (Frank & Carlisle-Frank Reference Frank and Carlisle-Frank2007; White et al. Reference Jefferson and Levy2010).

Intake diversion programmes can also provide an alternative for pet owners whose intake is not deemed urgent through shelter triage. One method to divert intake is through supported self-rehoming, where owners relinquish their animal directly to a new owner without surrendering their animal first to a shelter or rescue. The American Pet Products Association (2021) reported that 57% of pet owners said they would give their dog to a friend or relative if they could not care for the dog anymore. While bringing the animal to a shelter or rescue was the second most popular option, it was only reported by 16% of pet owners (American Pet Products Association 2021). Weiss and colleagues (Reference Weiss, Slater, Garrison, Drain, Dolan, Scarlett and Zawistowsk2014) found that 85% of dog owners who had brought their animal to a shelter to surrender had tried to explore other options for rehoming before bringing the animal to the facility. Common methods of personal rehoming included giving the animal to family or friends, contacting a help line, and social media or online public advertisements (Weiss et al. Reference Weiss, Slater, Garrison, Drain, Dolan, Scarlett and Zawistowsk2014). Similarly, Digiacomo and colleagues (Reference Digiacomo, Arluke and Patronek1998) interviewed people who previously relinquished animals, and found that 45% attempted to rehome their animals to friends, family, or through an online advertisement (Digiacomo et al. Reference Digiacomo, Arluke and Patronek1998), although owners were often unsuccessful in self-rehoming without additional support. This indicates that informal rehoming of animals does already occur, although support may be needed to improve the success of this diversion tactic.

In 2017, the website AdoptaPet.com (hereby referred to as Adopt a Pet) launched a supported self-rehoming programme called ‘Rehome.’ Adopt a Pet also functions as an online marketing tool for animal shelters and rescues to post available animals (like Petfinder.com). Adopt a Pet collects data similar to typical animal shelter data, including physical characteristics of the animal (e.g. breed, size), behaviour (e.g. good with dogs, good with children), and reason for rehoming. The platform also collects data on the outcome of the animal, including whether the animal was adopted or kept (i.e. diverted from animal shelters) or relinquished to a shelter/rescue after being posted for adoption on ‘Rehome’. As no previous research has evaluated dedicated supported self-rehoming programmes, our primary research aim was to understand which animal and owner characteristics predict increased odds of diversion from versus relinquishment to animal shelters, similar to previous research conducted within animal shelters. For example, previous studies found that adopted dogs were likely to be small (Brown et al. Reference Brown, Davidson and Zuefle2013; Siettou et al. Reference Siettou, Fraser and Fraser2014), light coloured (Posage et al. Reference Posage, Bartlett and Thomas1998; Lepper et al. Reference Lepper, Kass and Hart2002), and younger (Clevenger & Kass Reference Clevenger and Kass2003; Normando et al. Reference Normando, Stefanini, Meerst, Adamelli, Coultis and Bono2006). Dog behaviours during interactions, such as engaging in play and lying down next to the adopter, increased likelihood of adoption (Protopopova et al. Reference Protopopova, Mehrkam, Boggess and Wynne2014). Dogs and cats that are surrendered for owner-related reasons were more likely to be adopted than stray animals, while those surrendered for behavioural reasons were less likely to be adopted (Lepper et al. Reference Lepper, Kass and Hart2002). Based on evidence from previous research conducted in animal shelters, we hypothesised that younger, purebred, and smaller animals those that were labelled as having desirable behaviours (e.g. good with children, good with other dogs), and those that were surrendered for owner-related reasons would have greater odds of diversion from animal shelters.

Our additional interest was to understand the decisions that take place during the diversion process. Diverted animals were either adopted to new owners or kept by their original owner. To further understand the process of diversion from animal shelters, our secondary aim was to explore which animal and owner characteristics were associated with a change in the odds of the pet being kept by the original owner versus it being adopted to a new home. We did not have specific hypotheses regarding the impact of animal and owner characteristics on odds of adoption versus keeping the pet as the aim was exploratory in nature, due to a lack of research in the field of intake diversion.

Materials and methods

Data

All study protocols were reviewed and approved by the University of British Columbia’s Behavioural Research Ethics Board (H21-01729). The full data and R script used for analysis can be found at https://github.com/lexisly/rehome_diversion. The datasets used for both research questions originated from data of animals that were listed on Adopt a Pet Rehome website (‘Rehome’) from January 1, 2017, until May 21, 2021. The raw data file of animal listings contained 202,163 observations. From the original dataset, 3,435 animals were removed as they were uploaded by Adopt a Pet to detect bugs on the website. An additional eight animals were removed for having no species and no age. Listings that had multiple animals because they were a bonded pair (n = 4,086) or a litter of animals (n = 109) were removed, as postings only had fields to enter information about a single animal. Animals with duplicate listings which were posted on the same day and by the same owner were removed (n = 2,189), as the true listing could not be identified. An additional 8,297 animals were removed because they were uploaded and removed on the same day. The data were also restricted to those with a final outcome, meaning that the animal was adopted, kept by the owner, or relinquished to an animal shelter/rescue.

The final dataset used for descriptive analysis included 148,826 observations (Dog: n = 100,342; Cat: n = 48,484). The data were separated by species for the statistical models. For both dogs and cats, the data collected from pet owners through Rehome included basic information such as sex (female = 0; male = 1), age (puppy/kitten, young, adult, senior), spay/neuter status (intact = 0, spayed/neutered = 1), whether the animal was microchipped (no = 0, yes = 1), and whether the animal was purebred (no = 0, yes = 1). For dogs only, the data also included the size (small, medium, large) and the breed of the dog. The primary reported dog breeds were organised into breed groups by one author (AP) loosely based on the American Kennel Club (AKC) categories (Terrier, Herding, Hound, Sporting, Toy, Working; dogs in the non-sporting category and non-AKC breeds were reassigned into categories based on the history and/or traditional use of the breed).

The data also included behavioural information about the animal, including whether the animal was house-trained (no = 0; yes = 1), and whether the animal was good with dogs, cats, and children (no = 0; yes = 1 for each). Pet owners could also indicate whether the animal had special needs (no = 0; yes = 1) or required an experienced adopter (no = 0; yes = 1). These statements were available to the owner to select without definitions.

From the pet profile, the number of photographs of the animal were included in the model (0, 1, 2, 3, 4). Pet owners could also indicate the period that they had to rehome their animal (1 week or less = short, 2 to 4 weeks = medium, 8 weeks = long, no deadline = none) and a reason for rehoming their animal (animal behaviour issue, cost issue, abandoned or found, housing issue, human health issue, personal issue, none listed). These categories were available to the owner with no definitions. The first listed level in each variable was used as the reference level for the models. The owners also had an opportunity to write more about the animal, restrictions on adopters, and/or the circumstances behind the surrender in a textbox, but this text was not included in the present analysis.

Analysis

All analyses were performed in R Studio version 2.3.492 (RStudio Team 2022). Descriptive analyses (e.g. proportion) were used to characterise the population of dogs and cats on the website by the provided variables. Prior to analysis, correlations between independent variables were tested using Phi correlation (for pairs of binary variables), Chi-squared Cramer’s V (for pairs with at least one nominal variable), and Spearman’s rank correlation (for pairs with at least one ordinal variable). All analyses were run separately by species (dog and cat).

For the first research question, we conducted a binary logistic regression model with the outcome of whether the animal was diverted (i.e. adopted or kept = 1) or relinquished to an animal shelter (= 0). The data were split into training (80%) and testing (20%) samples. Due to the unequal proportion of outcome classes, the training datasets were down-sampled such that the proportions of diverted and relinquished pets were equal (Dog: n = 10,322; Cat: n = 5,545).

We ran an additional opportunistic post hoc model for only the purebred dogs in the sample (n = 27,241) to test whether breed group was associated with a change in odds of diversion even for purebred dogs. The training dataset was down-sampled such that the proportion of diverted and relinquished pets were equal (n = 4,000).

For the second research question, the data were subset to remove animals that were relinquished to a shelter, so only those that were diverted remained. We performed a binary logistic regression with an outcome of being kept by their original owner (= 1) versus being rehomed (= 0). These data were split into training (80%) and testing (20%) samples and then down-sampled such that the proportions of adopted and kept pets were equal (Dog: n = 52,886; Cat: n = 23,488). Final regression models were selected using backwards elimination based on the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC).

Results

Descriptive analysis

In total, 148,826 animal records were used for analysis. The majority were dogs (67.4%), while the rest were cats (32.6%). For dogs (n = 100,342), 12.9% (n = 12,902) were relinquished to a shelter or rescue and the rest diverted (out of the total dog profiles, 54.2% [n = 54,387] were adopted, 32.9% [n = 33,053] were kept). For cats (n = 48,484), 14.3% (n = 6,931) were relinquished to a shelter or rescue and the rest diverted (out of the total cat profiles, 55.4% [n = 26,874] were adopted, 30.3% [n = 14,679] were kept).

The most common reported primary dog breeds in the sample were American Pit Bull Terrier (APBT; 11.3%, n = 11,582), Labrador Retriever (11.2%, n = 11,470), German Shepherd Dog (7.3%, n = 7,487), Chihuahua (6.2%, n = 6,322), and Husky (4.6%, 4,726). APBTs comprised the majority (58.3%) of the Terrier breed group, while Labrador Retrievers comprised the majority (66.3%) of the Sporting breed group. Across all dog breeds, the mean (± SD) percentage of purebred dogs across all breeds was 35.8 (± 19.2)%. For the most commonly reported primary dog breeds, percentage of dogs reported as purebred was 25.4% (n = 2,943) for APBT, 17.9% for Labrador Retriever (n = 2,055), 32.5% for German Shepherd Dog (n = 2,435), 38.0% for Husky (n = 1,796), and 18.9% for Chihuahua (n = 1,198).

Statistical analysis

The correlation analyses revealed three pairs of independent variables that were at least moderately (i.e. coefficient > 0.3) correlated and statistically significant. As dogs’ age group increased, the proportion of spayed/neutered dogs increased (Puppy = 41.5%, Young = 72.5%, Adult = 84.7%, Senior = 87.7%, r = 0.32; P < 0.001). Dog size and breed group were moderately associated (Cramer’s V = 0.48; P < 0.001). The majority (54.4%) of small dogs belonged to a Toy breed, medium-sized dogs were relatively well-represented across breed groups other than Toy breeds (Herding = 23.6%, Hound = 11.6%, Sporting = 20.2%, Terrier = 22.0%, Toy = 4.2%, Working = 18.4%), and large dogs were mostly Working breeds (33.9%), Sporting breeds (21.0%), Herding breeds (18.4%) and Terrier breeds (15.0%). For cats, age and spay/neuter status were also moderately associated (ɸ = 0.53; P < 0.001), with the percentage of spayed/neutered cats being 30.4% for kittens, 77.0% for young cats, 93.2% for adult cats, and 96.0% for senior cats. All independent variables were included in our initial models, as the Variance Inflation Factor scores did not indicate problems of severe multicollinearity.

Which dogs were diverted?

Within the cleaned data (n = 100,342), 87.1% (n = 87,440) of the dogs were diverted from animal shelters after being posted on ‘Rehome’, while the rest were relinquished to a shelter or rescue facility. All the variables that were originally entered into the model are shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Animal- and owner-related characteristics across the total sample (n = 100,342) of dogs listed on ‘Rehome’, the diverted population, and dogs that were adopted to a new home or kept by their original owner. The data are from dogs listed on ‘Rehome’ from January 1, 2017 to May 21, 2021

The final model using the down-sampled data (n = 10,322) contained 14 variables that were associated with a change in odds of diversion from animal shelters following backwards, step-wise elimination. The final model correctly classified 60.4% of the cases (95% CI = 59.8–61.1%) in the testing dataset. Figure 1 shows the final model variables; although within each of these variables, not all variable levels were statistically significant.

Figure 1. Associations between dog and owner characteristics and whether dogs were diverted (= 1) or relinquished to animal shelters (= 0) after being posted on an online-supported, self-rehoming website (n = 10,322). Data are presented by odds ratios and their 95% confidence interval (error bars); P < 0.05 when the 95% CI does not cross the vertical dotted line.

Several physical characteristics of dogs on ‘Rehome’ predicted a change in odds of diversion from animal shelters. As dogs increased in age group, the odds of diversion decreased in comparison to puppies by 10% for young dogs (95% CI = 0.83–0.98), by 28% for adult dogs (95% CI = 0.66–0.79), and by 48% for senior dogs (95% CI = 0.48–0.60). Both medium and large dogs had decreased odds of diversion by 0.75 (95% CI = 0.68–0.82) and 0.79 times (95% CI = 0.71–0.88), respectively, in comparison to small dogs. In comparison to Terrier breed dogs, Working (OR = 1.13, CI = 1.04–1.25), Sporting (OR = 1.12, 95% CI = 1.02–1.23), Herding (OR = 1.21, 95% CI = 1.10–1.33), and Toy breed (OR = 1.53, 95% CI = 1.37–1.70) dogs had increased odds of being diverted from an animal shelter. Dogs that were a Hound breed showed 0.88 times decreased odds of being diverted (95% CI = 0.79–0.98). Purebred dogs had 1.56 times greater odds of being diverted in comparison to mixed breed dogs (95% CI = 1.45–1.67).

The post hoc model revealed that breed groups also impacted odds of diversion even for purebred dogs. However, in comparison to Terrier dogs, only Sporting (OR = 1.35, 95% CI = 1.07–1.71) and Toy breed (OR–1.52, 95% CI =1.22–1.89) dogs had increased odds of diversion.

Behaviour and health status also predicted a change in odds of diversion for dogs. Dogs that were labelled good with cats, other dogs, and children had increased odds of diversion by 1.14 (95% CI = 1.08–1.22), 1.16 (1.08–1.24), and 1.30 times (1.21–1.39), respectively. Similarly, odds of diversion for dogs that were house-trained were 1.28 times greater in comparison to non-house-trained dogs (95% CI = 1.18–1.38). Dogs that were microchipped and spayed/neutered had 0.84 (95% CI = 0.79–0.89) and 0.89 times (0.82–0.96), respectively, decreased odds of diversion from animal shelters. Dogs that were labelled as needing an experienced adopter and dogs that were labelled as special needs had decreased odds of being diverted from animal shelters by 0.92 (95% CI = 0.86–0.98) and 0.74 times (95% CI = 0.66–0.83), respectively.

Owner circumstance, such as the deadline to rehome and the reason for rehoming, affected the odds of diversion for dogs. As the rehome deadline increased, dogs had increasingly greater odds of being diverted from animal shelters. Odds of diversion increased by 1.60 times for those with medium deadlines (95% CI = 1.49–1.71), 2.6 times for those with long deadlines (95% CI = 2.38–1.87), and 2.89 times for those with no deadline (95% CI = 2.61–3.21) in comparison to dogs with short rehome deadlines. In comparison to rehoming for animal behaviour issues, dogs that were rehomed for housing (OR = 1.45, 95% CI = 1.15–1.38), human health (OR = 1.23, 95% CI = 1.09–1.38) or personal issues (OR = 1.36, 95% CI = 1.25–1.48) had greater odds of diversion from animal shelters.

Which cats were diverted?

Within the cleaned data (n = 48,484), 85.7% (n = 41,553) of the cats were diverted, while the rest were relinquished to a shelter or rescue facility. All the variables that were originally entered into the model are shown by final outcome in Table 2.

Table 2. Animal- and owner-related characteristics across the total sample (n = 48,484) of cats listed on ‘Rehome’, the diverted population, and cats that were adopted to a new home or kept by their original owner. The data are from cats listed on ‘Rehome’ from January 1, 2017 to May 21, 2021

The final model using the down-sampled data (n = 5,545) contained nine variables that were associated with a change in odds of diversion from animal shelters (Figure 2). The final model correctly classified 56.1% of the cases (95% CI = 55.1–57.1%) in the testing dataset. Figure 2 shows the final model variables; although within each of these variables, not all variable levels were statistically significant.

Figure 2. Associations between cat and owner characteristics and whether cats were diverted (= 1) or relinquished to animal shelters (= 0) after being posted on an online-supported, self-rehoming website (n = 5,545). Data are presented by odds ratios and their 95% confidence interval (error bars); P < 0.05 when the 95% CI does not cross the vertical dotted line.

Similarly to dogs, physical characteristics of cats such as age and being purebred influenced the outcome of cats posted on ‘Rehome.’ As age groups increased, odds of diversion from animal shelters decreased by 21% for young cats (95% CI = 0.70–0.87), by 38% for adult cats (95% CI = 0.55–0.69), and 49% for senior cats (95% CI = 0.43–0.62). Purebred cats had 1.87 times greater likelihood of diversion in comparison to mixed breed cats (95% CI = 1.58–2.22).

While being good with other cats did not change the outcome, cats that were good with dogs and children had 1.13 (95% CI = 1.04–1.22) and 1.23 times (95% CI = 1.14–1.34) increased odds of being adopted or kept, respectively. Cats that were microchipped had 0.81 times less chance of being diverted from animal shelters compared to non-microchipped cats (95% CI = 0.74–0.88).

While the number of photographs on dogs’ profiles was not statistically significant, the number on cats’ profiles was relevant in predicting outcome — cats who had three (OR = 1.33, 95% CI = 1.01–1.77) or four photographs (OR = 1.47, 95% CI = 1.13–1.93) had greater odds of being diverted in comparison to having none on the profile.

Similarly to dogs, as the rehome deadline increased, the odds of diversion increased by 1.40 times for those with a medium deadline (95% CI = 1.26–1.54), by 2.17 for a long deadline (1.92–2.46), and by 2.24 for those with no deadline (95% CI = 1.94–2.58) to rehome their cat. Unlike for dogs, the only reason for rehoming that was statistically significant was for cats who were abandoned or found — these individuals had decreased odds of being diverted from animal shelters (OR = 0.76, 95% CI = 0.66–0.87).

What happened to diverted dogs?

Out of dogs that were not relinquished to a shelter or rescue (n = 87,440), 37.8% (n = 33,053) were kept by their owners, while the remaining 62.2% (n = 54,387) were rehomed. The final model correctly classified 57.0% of the cases (95% CI = 56.3–57.8%) in the testing dataset. The final model using the down-sampled data (n = 52,886) contained 13 variables that were associated with a change in odds of keeping versus adopting dogs; although within each of these variables, not all variable levels were statistically significant (Figure 3).

Figure 3. Associations between dog and owner characteristics and whether dogs were kept by their original owner (= 1) or adopted to a new home (= 0) after being posted on an online-supported, self-rehoming website (n = 52,886). Data are presented by odds ratios and their 95% confidence interval (error bars); P < 0.05 when the 95% CI does not cross the vertical dotted line.

Both medium- and large-sized dogs were more likely to be kept by their owner rather than rehomed, in comparison to small (medium OR = 1.16, 95% CI = 1.11–1.21; large OR = 1.22, 95% CI = 1.15–1.29) dogs. Herding, Sporting, Toy, and Working breed dogs had decreased odds of being kept by their original owner in comparison to Terrier breeds by 0.91 (95% CI = 0.86–0.97), 0.89 (95% CI = 0.84 = 0.95), 0.87 (95% CI = 0.83–0.94), and 0.87 times (95% CI = 0.82–0.92), respectively.

Dogs that were good with cats or children had only slightly decreased odds of being kept in comparison to those that were not (good with cats OR = 0.94, 95% CI = 0.90–0.98; good with children OR = 0.92, 95% CI = 0.88–0.96). Dogs that were spayed or neutered had 7% decreased odds of being kept in comparison to intact dogs (OR = 93, 95% CI = 0.88–0.97). Dogs that needed an experienced adopter had 1.30 times greater odds of being kept by their owner (95% CI = 1.25–1.35). Similarly, special needs dogs had 1.20 times increased odds of being kept by their owner (95% CI = 1.12–1.29).

As the number of photographs on a dogs’ profile increased, odds of an owner keeping their dog decreased in comparison to having no photographs on the profile (one OR = 0.67, 95% CI = 0.60–0.76; two OR = 0.60, 95% CI = 0.53–0.68; three OR = 0.56, 95% CI = 0.49–0.63; four photographs OR = 0.55, 95% CI = 0.49–0.62).

As the owner’s rehome deadline increased from a short deadline, the odds of keeping the dog increased by 1.15 times for a medium rehoming deadline (95% CI = 1.11–1.21), by 1.47 times for a long rehoming deadline (95% CI = 1.39–1.56), and by 1.73 times for a those with no rehoming deadline (95% CI = 1.63–1.84). In comparison to animal behaviour issues, dogs that were being rehomed for numerous owner-related issues had lower odds of being kept. Odds of being kept decreased by 0.40 times for dogs whose rehoming reason was the owner’s personal issues (95% CI = 0.35–0.45). Dogs that were originally abandoned to the current owner or found had 0.60 times less chance of being kept by their owner (95% CI = 0.56–0.65). Odds of being kept decreased by 0.75 times for dogs rehomed for housing-related issues (95% CI = 0.71–0.80) and by 0.80 times for human-health issues (95% CI = 0.74–0.86). Dogs rehomed for cost-related issues had only marginally increased odds of being kept (OR = 1.10, 95% CI = 1.00–1.21).

What happened to diverted cats?

Out of cats that were not relinquished to a shelter/rescue (n = 41,553), 35.3% (n = 14,679) were kept by their original owners, while the remaining 64.4% (n = 26,874) were adopted to a new home. The final model using the down-sampled data (n = 23,488) contained eleven variables that were associated with a change in odds of the owner keeping their cat; although within each of these variables, not all variable levels were statistically significant (Figure 4). The final model correctly classified 58.3% of the cases (95% CI = 57.3–59.4%) in the testing dataset.

Figure 4. Associations between cat and owner characteristics and whether cats were kept by their original owner (= 1) or adopted to a new home (= 0) after being posted on an online-supported, self-rehoming website (n = 23,488). Data are presented by odds ratios and their 95% confidence interval (error bars); P < 0.05 when the 95% CI does not cross the vertical dotted line.

Cats in all three older age groups had increased odds of being kept by their owners in comparison to kittens (young OR = 1.33, 95% CI = 1.23–1.44; adult OR = 1.30, 95% CI = 1.20–1.42; senior OR = 1.51, 95% CI = 1.31–1.74). Purebred cats had 0.86 times decreased odds of being kept in comparison to mixed breed cats (95% CI = 0.87–0.94).

Cats that were good with children had slightly decreased odds of being kept by their original owner (OR = 0.93, 95% CI = 0.88–0.98). Cats that needed an experienced adopter had 1.39 times greater odds of being kept (95% CI = 1.29–1.51), while cats that had special needs had 1.41 times greater odds of being kept (95% CI = 1.24–1.61). Also, odds of being kept were 1.33 times greater for house-trained cats (95% CI = 1.21–1.48), but the odds of being kept were decreased by 0.90 times for spayed or neutered cats in comparison to those intact (95% CI = 0.84–0.97).

As the number of photographs on a cats’ profile increased, odds of a cat being kept decreased in comparison to having none on the profile (one OR = 0.54, 95% CI = 0.44–0.65; two OR = 0.57, 95% CI = 0.44–0.65; three OR = 0.50, 95% CI 0.48–0.69; four photographs OR = 0.50, 95% CI = 0.42–0.60).

The odds of being kept by their owner did not differ significantly from those who had a medium rehome deadline in comparison to a short deadline. However, cats with a long deadline or no deadline for rehoming were more 1.15 (95% CI = 1.05–1.24) and 1.37 times (95% CI = 1.25–1.51) more likely to be kept, respectively. Similarly to dogs, cats that were rehomed due to owner-related reasons (other than cost-related issues) were less likely to be kept in comparison to animal behaviour issues. Odds of being kept decreased by 0.70 times for cats who were rehomed due to housing-related issues (95% CI = 0.64–0.76), by 0.64 times for human health issues (95% CI =0.58–0.70), and by 0.51 times for personal issues (95% CI = 0.42–0.62). Also, cats that were originally abandoned to the owner or found were 0.75 times less likely to be kept in comparison to those rehomed due to animal behaviour issues (95% CI = 0.68–0.83).

Discussion

Approximately 85% of both cats and dogs were adopted to a new home or kept by their owners after being posted online, suggesting that supported self-rehoming could be a useful tool to reduce the intake of animals to shelter and rescue facilities. In our study, the remaining 15% of animals were still relinquished to a shelter, although the odds of relinquishment differed based on animal- and owner-related characteristics. While previous research has investigated sources of acquisition of companion animals (e.g. Freiwald et al. Reference Freiwald, Litster and Weng2014; Weiss et al. Reference Weiss, Gramann, Spain and Slater2015; Bir et al. Reference Bir, Widmar and Croney2017), adoption through supported self-rehoming has not been recognised as a separate acquisition source. However, Weiss and colleagues (Reference Weiss, Gramann, Spain and Slater2015) found that the most common method of rehoming pets was to a friend or family member (37%), while an additional 11% were given to someone that was not previously known to the pet owner. In comparison, 36% of relinquishing pet owners still brought their animal to a shelter or rescue (Weiss et al. Reference Weiss, Gramann, Spain and Slater2015). Bringing supported self-rehoming to online pet adoption platforms, such as Adopt a Pet, that potential adopters have already visited when searching for animals (Weiss et al. Reference Weiss, Miller, Mohan-Gibbons and Vela2012; Workman & Hoffman Reference Workman and Hoffman2015), may further promote the use of supported self-rehoming as a way of diverting animals from shelters.

The population of dogs and cats posted on ‘Rehome’ was generally younger, smaller, and had a higher proportion of morphologically preferred dog breeds and purebred cats in comparison to animal shelter populations described in previous literature. In our study, 5.1% of dogs and 4.8% of cats were seniors. Previous studies reported that the proportion of senior dogs in shelters ranged from 7–18%, while the senior cat population proportion ranged from 8–14% (New Jr et al. Reference New, Salman, Scarlett, Kass, Vaughn, Scherr and Kelch1999; Scarlett et al. Reference Scarlett, Salman, New and Kass1999; Hawes et al. Reference Hawes, Kerrigan, Hupe and Morris2020). Our study population had a similar proportion of purebred dogs (27.1%) in comparison to older in-shelter studies, which ranged from 24–30% (New Jr et al. Reference New, Salman, Scarlett, Kass, Vaughn, Scherr and Kelch1999; New et al. Reference New, Salman, King, Scarlett, Kass and Hutchison2000). However, while most older studies are based on visual identification and owner reports, Gunter and colleagues (Reference Gunter, Barber and Wynne2018) found that only 5% of shelter dogs had only one majority breed through genetic testing. Our study also found a slightly higher proportion of purebred cats (7.2%) in comparison to previous reports (2–6%; New Jr et al. Reference New, Salman, Scarlett, Kass, Vaughn, Scherr and Kelch1999; New et al. Reference New, Salman, King, Scarlett, Kass and Hutchison2000; Shore & Girrens Reference Shore and Girrens2001). Hawes and colleagues (Reference Hawes, Kerrigan, Hupe and Morris2020) reported a higher proportion of Terrier (27.5 versus 19.3%) and Herding (21.6 versus 17.9%) dogs, while our sample had a much higher proportion of Toy (17.8 versus 4.9%) and Hound (10.1 versus 5.9%) dogs. Similarly, our study also reported a higher proportion of small dogs (28.8 versus 15.7%; Hawes et al. Reference Hawes, Kerrigan, Hupe and Morris2020). Given that we found that animals of a young age, purebred status, certain breed types, and small size to be more likely to be diverted from animal shelters, this suggests that animal shelter data represent a particular sub-sample of all owner-surrendered pets.

Predictors of diversion and retention

In line with our hypothesis, morphological characteristics that are preferred by adopters in shelter settings were also important in predicting the type of diversion through self-rehoming (i.e. remaining with the original owner or entering a new home). Morphological features are prioritised by adopters, compared to behaviour, when potential adopters initially evaluate shelter dogs in-person (Protopopova et al. Reference Protopopova, Gilmour, Weiss, Shen and Wynne2012; Weiss et al. Reference Weiss, Miller, Mohan-Gibbons and Vela2012). Indeed, this may be even more true for adopters searching for pets on the internet since there is no possible physical interaction with the animals. Research in animal shelters as well as on online adoption websites similarly found that likelihood of adoption for dogs and cats decreased with increased age (Lepper et al. Reference Lepper, Kass and Hart2002; Workman & Hoffman Reference Workman and Hoffman2015). Similarly, previous research found that purebred dogs were 1.43 times more likely to be adopted rather than euthanased (Lepper et al. Reference Lepper, Kass and Hart2002). Lepper and colleagues did not evaluate whether purebred cats had an increased likelihood of adoption; however, rare breeds, such as Siamese, were more likely to be adopted in comparison to domestic shorthair cats. More generally, when pet owners were asked why they did not adopt their animal from a shelter, 35% cited the desire for a purebred animal (Maddalena et al. Reference Maddalena, Zeidman and Campbell2012), indicating that potential adopters value purebred animals, similar to our study.

Congruent with the results of our study, previous research found larger dogs to be less likely to be adopted from animal shelters (Lepper et al. Reference Lepper, Kass and Hart2002; Brown et al. Reference Brown, Davidson and Zuefle2013; Siettou et al. Reference Siettou, Fraser and Fraser2014). Physically, individual breeds within each group may differ greatly, particularly when the animal is mixed breed. However, we also found that, even within purebred dogs, Sporting and Toy dog breeds still had greater odds of diversion. Preference for particular breeds and smaller dogs indicates that appearance is important in the decision to adopt an animal through online-supported, self-rehoming programmes. In addition to the morphological differences seen across dog breeds, breed labels may influence potential adopters’ perceptions of behaviour or attractiveness of dogs. In our study, APBTs were the most common dog breed posted on ‘Rehome’, in addition, this breed comprised the majority of the Terrier breed group. Similarly, in animal shelters, pit bull-type dogs are often the most prevalent breed-label available for adoption (Protopopova et al. Reference Protopopova, Gilmour, Weiss, Shen and Wynne2012; Voith et al. Reference Voith, Trevejo, Dowling-Guyer, Chadik, Marder, Johnson and Irizarry2013). Breed labelling in animal shelters often relies upon owner reports or visual identification of staff, which is often inaccurate in comparison to DNA analysis, particularly for pit bull-type dogs (Voith et al. Reference Voith, Ingram, Mitsouras and Irizarry2009). While in the present study, the impact of breed on the odds of diversion was likely a mix between perception of dog breed labels and physical characteristics, breed labelling by owners may be even more important on websites like Adopt a Pet, where prospective adopters can filter their search based on dog breed.

While previous in-shelter research used measures such as interactions with adopters and in-kennel location and behaviours to evaluate whether behaviour mattered to adopters (Luescher & Tyson Medlock Reference Luescher and Tyson Medlock2009; Protopopova et al. Reference Protopopova, Gilmour, Weiss, Shen and Wynne2012; Grant & Warrior Reference Grant and Warrior2019), our study used owner-reported behavioural indicators. Animal shelters and online adoption websites may include tags such as ‘good with children’ and ‘needs experienced adopter’ when displaying available animals to inform potential adopters about the animals’ behaviour. Indeed, in agreement with our hypothesis, dogs and cats that were good with dogs and children had increased odds of diversion. For dogs, being good with cats also increased odds of diversion. In a survey of potential adopters, approximately three-quarters rated behaviour with people as an important factor in dog and cat adoption, while approximately one-quarter rated behaviour with other animals as important (Weiss et al. Reference Weiss, Miller, Mohan-Gibbons and Vela2012). Behavioural issues, particularly incompatibility with humans or other animals, are among the most common reasons for animals being returned to shelters (Mondelli et al. Reference Mondelli, Previde, Verga, Levi, Magistrelli and Valsecchi2004; Gates et al. Reference Gates, Zito, Thomas and Dale2018; Scott et al. Reference Scott, Jong, McArthur and Hazel2018; Hawes et al. Reference Hawes, Kerrigan, Hupe and Morris2020). However, the tags on ‘Rehome’ may also suggest undesirable behaviours or matching issues between the animals and a potential adopter’s new home (Protopopova & Bollen Reference Protopopova and Bollen2022). Indeed, shelter dogs labelled as ‘good with children’ had increased adoption rates in one US shelter (Luescher & Medlock Reference Luescher and Tyson Medlock2009). This is further corroborated by the present study, as dogs and cats that were labelled as needing an ‘experienced adopter’ had greater odds of being relinquished to an animal shelter after being posted on ‘Rehome.’ Also, dogs that were labelled as ‘special needs’ had lower odds of diversion, perhaps because the label suggests difficult cost or medical requirements for the pet. Previous authors have suggested that staff refrain from adding these descriptors to the profiles of animals, and instead discuss them during the adoption counselling phase (Protopopova & Bollen Reference Protopopova and Bollen2022); although this may be more difficult to implement with individual relinquishing owners in comparison to trained adoption counsellors in shelter organisations.

Dogs rehomed due to behavioural issues were more likely to end up being eventually surrendered to an animal shelter. Similar to our results, Lepper and colleagues (Reference Lepper, Kass and Hart2002) found that dogs that were relinquished for behavioural reasons were less likely to be adopted in comparison to stray dogs, although dogs relinquished for owner-related reasons were more likely to be adopted. Public members traditionally hold the belief that shelter dogs may differ behaviourally in comparison to dogs sourced elsewhere (Patronek et al. Reference Patronek, Bradley and Arps2022). A survey from the Republic of Ireland found that 68% of dog owners who adopted their animal from a shelter reported at least one behavioural issue, with the most common being fearfulness (Wells & Hepper Reference Wells and Hepper2000). However, research of owned dogs initially sourced from shelters, breeders, pet stores, and other sources shows that approximately 40–85% of the surveyed population reports that their dog has at least one behavioural issue (Voith et al. Reference Voith, Wright and Danneman1992; Kobelt et al. Reference Kobelt, Hemsworth, Barnett and Coleman2003; Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Twells, Seawright and Casey2008; Scott et al. Reference Scott, Jong, McArthur and Hazel2018). Our findings may indicate that dogs with owner-reported behavioural issues may have less success in adoption through online-supported, self-rehoming, and thus shelter facilities may serve as a safety net for such dogs. A rise in surrendered dogs with behavioural issues in shelter facilities may result in lack of adoption, increased length of stay (Normando et al. Reference Normando, Stefanini, Meerst, Adamelli, Coultis and Bono2006; McGuire et al. Reference McGuire, Chan, Jean-Baptiste, Kok and Rosenbaum2021; Raudies et al. Reference Raudies, Waiblinger and Arhant2021), increased resource use to care for animals (Bradley & Rajendran Reference Bradley and Rajendran2021), and increased euthanasia due to behavioural reasons (Caras Reference Caras1993; Pegram et al. Reference Pegram, Gray, Packer, Richards, Church, Brodbelt and O’Neill2021). However, further research is needed to understand whether dogs entering shelters have more prevalent or prominent behavioural problems that may require additional care and resources when surrendered to animal shelters. Animal shelter organisations can consider increasing the resources that are available for behavioural support of animals to accommodate dogs with behavioural concerns.

In contrast, behaviour was not as important in cat diversion. The lack of relationship between owner-related reasons and odds of diversion may indicate that cat adopters do not have preference for cats that were surrendered for particular reasons; however, our other results suggest that adopters do value certain behavioural traits (i.e. good with dogs, good with children, does not need an experienced adopter). The rehome reason selected by the owner is not displayed on the pets’ online profile, although owners may choose to disclose the reason in their pets’ profile biography. It may be possible that cat owners do not describe the behavioural issues in their pets’ biographies as often as dog owners do, or that behavioural issues that lead to surrender in cats may not be as deterring as behavioural issues in dogs. However, the online rehoming platform did not specify what behavioural issues led to relinquishment and the present study did not assess the biographies of pet profiles.

In contrast, dogs and cats labelled with ‘good’ behavioural characteristics were less likely to be kept (i.e. more likely to be rehomed). In addition, those that required an experienced adopter or had special needs had greater odds of being kept by their original owner. Relatedly, our results showed that for both species, all owner-related reasons decreased odds of being kept in comparison to rehoming due to behavioural issues, with the exception being cost-related issues for cats. Again, these results may be due to adopter preference for animals that are owner surrendered that do not have reported behavioural issues (Wells & Hepper Reference Wells and Hepper1992). Perhaps in addition, our results suggest that pet owners may feel that some behavioural issues are not strong enough to warrant the decision to relinquish or rehome the animal. Previous research has found that the majority of pet owners report that their animal engages in at least one behaviour that may be undesirable, such as leash pulling, hyperactivity, inappropriate elimination, and aggression (Patronek et al. Reference Patronek, Glickman, Beck, McCabe and Ecker1996; Blackwell et al. Reference Blackwell, Twells, Seawright and Casey2008; Casey et al. Reference Casey, Loftus, Bolster, Richards and Blackwell2014). Scott and colleagues (Reference Scott, Jong, McArthur and Hazel2018) found that, despite over 50% of dog owners reporting undesirable behaviours, almost all owners reported being satisfied with their pets’ behaviour. Indeed, many pet owners keep pets despite reporting behaviours that owners deem problematic (Voith et al. Reference Voith, Ingram, Mitsouras and Irizarry2009).

Microchipped dogs and cats and spayed/neutered dogs had decreased odds of diversion from animal shelters. On the contrary, some studies have found that dogs and cats that are spayed/neutered are preferred by adopters to intact ones (Lepper et al. Reference Lepper, Kass and Hart2002). However, this discrepancy may be due to a greater proportion of intact and non-microchipped animals being younger in our study, as potential adopters largely prefer to adopt younger pets (Lepper et al. Reference Lepper, Kass and Hart2002; Normando et al. Reference Normando, Stefanini, Meerst, Adamelli, Coultis and Bono2006; Weiss et al. Reference Weiss, Miller, Mohan-Gibbons and Vela2012). In animal shelters, spay/neuter status may not be as important to predict adoption outcomes, as most animal shelters in North America spay or neuter their animals prior to adoption in order to reduce the risk of unwanted litters (Protopopova & Gunter Reference Protopopova and Gunter2017). However, spaying/neutering prior to adoption is not necessarily the case for animals adopted through online-supported, self-rehoming programmes, which leaves the choice to adopt an intact animal up to the individual. A recent survey of the US public found that 74% agreed that spaying and neutering pets was the right thing to do; however, 52% agreed that spay/neuter surgeries are expensive, and 34% agreed that the surgery can be dangerous (Glasser Reference Glasser2021). Online-supported, self-rehoming platforms may give those who prefer to adopt an intact animal a source to adopt, although further research is needed to understand whether these methods of private rehoming increases risk of unwanted litters.

For cats, but not dogs, the number of photographs on the profile impacted odds of diversion from animal shelters, with those that had three or four photographs having a greater likelihood of diversion in comparison to those with none. Previous research using online adoption websites also found that probability of adoption increased as the number of photographs on the pets’ profile increased (Lampe & Witte Reference Lampe and Witte2015; Markowitz Reference Markowitz2020); although these studies included dogs, cats, and other species (e.g. horses, rabbits, small animals) in their analysis. The difference between dogs and cats may be due to potential adopters’ values when adopting each species. For example, while appearance was rated the most important factor when adopting dogs, behaviour with people was the most important for cats (Weiss et al. Reference Weiss, Miller, Mohan-Gibbons and Vela2012). People may value additional photographs when viewing cats, as multiple photographs may give adopters a better judgement of behaviour or personality.

Owners who do not have time to engage in the rehoming process were at greater risk of relinquishing their animal to a shelter or rescue. As the deadline for rehoming was extended, the odds of diversion increased greatly. Pet owners often struggle with the decision to relinquish their pet for a prolonged period, which may lead pet owners to visit the shelter to surrender at the last possible moment — when the decision to surrender has been solidified (Digiacomo et al. Reference Digiacomo, Arluke and Patronek1998). Also, the decision to bring an animal to a shelter facility may be influenced by the pet owners’ attitudes toward shelters or euthanasia (Digiacomo et al. Reference Digiacomo, Arluke and Patronek1998; Lund et al. Reference Lund, Eggertsson, Grøndahl and Eggertsdóttir2010; Martin et al. Reference Martin, Kelling and Mallavarapu2021). Our study similarly indicates that those who make the decision to post their animals with a short deadline to rehome are more likely to relinquish their animal to a shelter or rescue facility, indicating that relinquishment may be a last resort. Shelter staff could consider including supported self-rehoming resources on their websites or on their social media, where pet owners often look before they visit the shelter to surrender their animal (Workman & Hoffman Reference Workman and Hoffman2015). Additionally, shelter staff may consider suggesting online-supported, self-rehoming as an alternative to surrender for those who contact them if they do not have a short timeline for rehoming their animal. Further research may consider investigating the decision-making process for pet owners who are considering relinquishment to determine the most meaningful methods to communicating alternatives to surrender such as supported self-rehoming.

Indeed, increased time to rehome the animal increased odds of the owner ultimately keeping their animal. This result may indicate that those who post their animal with a longer rehome deadline may have more time to access services that enable them to keep their animal. Weiss and colleagues (Reference Weiss, Slater, Garrison, Drain, Dolan, Scarlett and Zawistowsk2014) found that over half of dog owners who brought their pet to a shelter for relinquishment considered their decision for a month or more before bringing their animal to shelter. As such, those who are in the earlier stages of the relinquishment decision-making process may be more successful candidates for pet retention programmes; however, further research is needed to understand how best to connect owners with these programmes, as research indicates that pet owners do not approach shelter and rescue organisations until later in the decision-making process (Digiacomo et al. Reference Digiacomo, Arluke and Patronek1998).

In situations where the owner did not have direct responsibility for the animal, the animal was not likely to be successfully diverted. Cats that were originally abandoned to or found by the current owner had decreased odds of diversion. One Australian shelter study reported that common reasons for cats being surrendered to shelters were that the cat was not theirs, they were concerned for the cat, or they thought the cat would be better off in the shelter (Zito et al. Reference Zito, Morton, Vankan, Paterson, Bennett, Rand and Phillips2016). Indeed, in our study, it is possible that an owner may feel less personal responsibility towards the animal in comparison to a pet that has been owned by them for some time, leading them to bring the animal to the shelter quicker.

Limitations and future directions

It is not fully clear whether the outcome of diversion is driven more by potential adopters’ desire to adopt or the original owners’ desire to keep their animal. Additionally, based on the current data, we cannot conclude whether an owner choosing to keep their pet led to eventual relinquishment or if they were truly able to retain their pet in their own family. Weiss and colleagues (Reference Weiss, Slater, Garrison, Drain, Dolan, Scarlett and Zawistowsk2014) found that over half of surveyed relinquishing dog owners stated that a form of assistance (e.g. low-cost training, veterinary care, temporary pet-friendly housing) may have helped them retain their dog. In this same study, most relinquishing pet owners demonstrated a strong attachment to their pets, indicating that had resources been available to help them keep their dog, they would have considered using them (Weiss et al. Reference Weiss, Slater, Garrison, Drain, Dolan, Scarlett and Zawistowsk2014). As such, supported self-rehoming programmes could consider directing all pet owners with resources to encourage retention of their animals before the use of rehoming services. However, resources for owner support may be community specific (Weiss et al. Reference Weiss, Slater, Garrison, Drain, Dolan, Scarlett and Zawistowsk2014), rather than more generally applicable to all pet owners.

Although animals with the less desirable morphological and behavioural characteristics had the greatest odds of being relinquished, animals surrendered to shelters only represented about 15% of the total population posted on ‘Rehome.’ Further, animals entering shelters through owner surrender only represent an estimated 25–35% of animal shelter intake (Humane Canada 2017; Shelter Animals Count 2021; Rodriguez et al. Reference Rodriguez, Davis, Hill, Wolf, Hawes and Morris2022). As our sample only represents a small portion of the total shelter population, it is likely that animals entering through other means (e.g. stray, intake, humane officer investigation) may differ physically or behaviourally. Wells and Hepper (Reference Wells and Hepper2000) investigated dogs that were adopted from shelters and found that stray animals were more likely to have owner-reported undesirable behaviours than surrendered animals. Within shelters, one study found that owner-surrendered cats showed more behavioural signs of stress in comparison to stray cats (Dybdall et al. Reference Dybdall, Strasser and Katz2007). While further research is required to understand differences in populations from various sources, the present study shows that the population of animals relinquished after being posted on supported self-rehoming websites are less preferred by adopters than those that are diverted.

The data collected by ‘Rehome’ relies on owner reports, which may lead to issues with consistency across animal characteristics. For example, the terms ‘needs experienced adopter’ and ‘special needs’ do not have any formal definition on the website. Previous studies that assessed owner-reported behaviour found that owners indicated a variety of behaviours that were considered problematic, including leash pulling, barking, and aggression to people or pets (New et al. Reference New, Salman, King, Scarlett, Kass and Hutchison2000; Guy et al. Reference Guy, Luescher, Dohoo, Spangler, Miller, Dohoo and Bate2001). While some owners feel that certain behaviours are incompatible with their lifestyle, others may not rate the same behaviours as problematic (Voith Reference Voith2009). Grouping all behavioural incompatibilities into general categories may be detrimental to the adoption success of animals (Patronek et al. Reference Patronek, Bradley and Arps2022). Further, behavioural indicators of pets posted on supported self-rehoming websites rely on subjective reports from relinquishing owners, which may not fully reflect the behaviours of animals in other environments. For example, cats who are labelled as ‘not good with dogs’ may be indoor cats that have not interacted with dogs. Stephen and Ledger (Reference Stephen and Ledger2007) surveyed owners who relinquished their dogs to shelters about their dogs’ behaviour and found that less than half of the behaviour ratings correlated to the responses of the dogs’ new owners. Similarly, owners report the singular reason for relinquishment on ‘Rehome’, although previous literature indicates that relinquishment reasons are multifaceted, often consisting of both animal- and owner-related reasons (Digiacomo et al. Reference Digiacomo, Arluke and Patronek1998). Future research should consider the consistency with which owners are reporting behavioural and health concerns and relinquishment reasons when posting their animal on supported self-rehoming websites, and whether these reports correspond to the animals’ welfare in a new environment.

As an additional benefit, online supported self-rehoming may reduce the emotional impact of surrendering an animal as it allows pet owners greater agency in the rehoming process, although this was not directly evaluated in the present study. Pet owners may undergo emotional distress when deciding to relinquish their animal, including internal conflict of whether their pets’ quality of life can be improved through rehoming (Buller & Ballantyne Reference Buller and Ballantyne2020). Pet owners who surrendered their animal to a shelter or rescue often struggle with the decision due to concerns of euthanasia in shelters (Digiacomo et al. Reference Digiacomo, Arluke and Patronek1998). Supported self-rehoming could allow pet owners to ensure that the animal is adopted to a new home and to even allow for the owner to select the adopter themselves, which may also increase agency and possibly reduce the emotional impact of relinquishment for pet owners, although further research is needed. However, in animal sheltering, one concern is that the subjectivity of traditionally restrictive adoption practices may allow for bias and discrimination against certain adopters (Best Friends Animal Society 2021; Maddie’s Fund 2021). In recent years, many animal shelters have introduced more ‘open’ adoption policies, such as the ‘Adopters Welcome’ programme (HumanePro 2020), which encourages the removal of restrictive adoption practices (e.g. home checks, age restrictions) in favour of conversation-based practices. Rehoming from one owner to another may subject potential adopters to bias or discrimination based on sociodemographic conditions, although further research would be required to support this hypothesis.

Animal welfare implications

Companion animals in shelter facilities experience a variety of stressors, including unfamiliar surroundings, loud noises, lack of space, and insufficient social and environmental enrichment (Hennessy et al. Reference Hennessy, Davis, Williams, Mellott and Douglas1997; Kry Reference Kry and Casey2007; Ellis & Wells Reference Ellis and Wells2010; Scheifele et al. Reference Scheifele, Martin, Clark, Kemper and Wells2012). Supported self-rehoming may alleviate stress associated with shelter stays by removing the need to intake animals into these unfamiliar environments. However, given that those with less desirable characteristics had the greatest odds of being relinquished to animal shelters, organisations may end up with a larger than expected population of pets with less-preferred characteristics, which may lead to increased length of stay and resource use for organisations (Bradley & Rajendran Reference Bradley and Rajendran2021; McGuire et al. Reference McGuire, Chan, Jean-Baptiste, Kok and Rosenbaum2021). Understanding the pet and owner characteristics that lead to diversion versus shelter relinquishment can inform animal shelters of the population that may still enter their facilities.

Conclusion

Our data showed that, on an online-supported, self-rehoming platform, various animal and owner characteristics influenced the odds of diversion from animal shelters. The results add to a growing body of literature that investigates methods to reduce intake of animals to shelter facilities. Identifying animal and owner characteristics that are associated with increased odds of diversion could inform animal shelter organisations of pets that may be more easily diverted through online-supported self-rehoming. Our analysis indicates that dogs and cats that are younger, purebred, are being surrendered for owner-related reasons, and do not have special needs or behavioural challenges have greater success in being diverted through supported self-rehoming. Also, owners who indicate an extended deadline for rehoming their animals should be diverted to an online-supported, self-rehoming platform. However, a small population are subsequently relinquished to shelter facilities after being posted on supported self-rehoming websites. Animals with behavioural challenges, including those who are being surrendered for behavioural issues, have greater odds of being surrendered to a shelter or rescue. As such, animal shelters may consider redistributing their resources to accommodate for behavioural challenges. In addition, understanding differences between animals that are adopted versus kept by their original owner could provide insight into demographics that may lead to retention. Overall, we conclude that online supported self-rehoming platforms provide pet owners with an alternative to relinquishment that may reduce the intake of animals to shelters.

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council (SSHRC) through the Canada Graduate Scholarships—Master’s program. We would like to foremost thank the Adopt a Pet team, particularly April Huntsman and Dr Robia Charles, for trusting us with their data and providing information about the ‘Rehome’ platform. We would also like to thank Dr Julie Levy for connecting us with the team at Adopt a Pet. Additionally, we are grateful for the UBC Animal Welfare Program companion animal team for their feedback throughout the project.

Competing interests

None.

Footnotes

Author contributions: Conceptualisation: LHL, AP; Data curation: LHL, AP; Formal analysis: LHL; Funding acquisition: AP; Investigation: LHL, AP; Methodology: LHL, AP; Resources: LHL; Software: LHL; Supervision: AP; Validation: AP; Visualisation: LHL, AP; Writing – original draft: LHL, AP; Writing – review and editing: LHL, AP.

References

Alberthsen, C, Rand, J, Morton, J, Bennett, P, Paterson, M and Vankan, D 2016 Numbers and characteristics of cats admitted to Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals (RSPCA) shelters in Australia and reasons for surrender. Animals 6: 23. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani6030023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Pet Products Association 2021 2021-2022 APPA National Pet Owners Survey. APPA: Greenwhich, CT, USA.Google Scholar
Best Friends Animal Society 2019 Humane Animal Control Manual | Best Friends Animal Society. https://resources.bestfriends.org/article/humane-animal-control-manual Google Scholar
Best Friends Animal Society 2021 Pet Adoption: Barriers and Solutions | Network Partners. https://network.bestfriends.org/tools-and-information/roadblock-free/pet-adoption-barriers-and-solutions Google Scholar
Bir, C, Widmar, NJO and Croney, CC 2017 Stated preferences for dog characteristics and sources of acquisition. Animals 7: 59. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani7080059 Google ScholarPubMed
Blackwell, EJ, Twells, C, Seawright, A and Casey, RA 2008 The relationship between training methods and the occurrence of behavior problems, as reported by owners, in a population of domestic dogs. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 3: 207217. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JVEB.2007.10.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, J and Rajendran, S 2021 Increasing adoption rates at animal shelters: a two-phase approach to predict length of stay and optimal shelter allocation. BMC Veterinary Research 17: 116. https://doi.org/10.1186/S12917-020-02728-2/TABLES/6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, WP, Davidson, JP and Zuefle, ME 2013 Effects of phenotypic characteristics on the length of stay of dogs at two no kill animal shelters. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 16: 218. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2013.740967 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buller, K and Ballantyne, KC 2020 Living with and loving a pet with behavioral problems: Pet owners’ experiences. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 37: 4147. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JVEB.2020.04.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caras, R 1993 Animal welfare forum: overpopulation of unwanted dogs and cats. One generation away from humanity. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 202: 910912.Google ScholarPubMed
Casey, RA, Loftus, B, Bolster, C, Richards, GJ and Blackwell, EJ 2014 Human directed aggression in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris): Occurrence in different contexts and risk factors. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 152: 5263. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2013.12.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clevenger, J and Kass, PH 2003 Determinants of adoption and euthanasia of shelter dogs spayed or neutered in the University of California veterinary student surgery program compared to other shelter dogs. Journal of Veterinary Medical Education 30: 372378. https://doi.org/10.3138/jvme.30.4.372CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Digiacomo, N, Arluke, A and Patronek, G 1998 Surrendering pets to shelters: The relinquisher’s perspective. Anthrozoös 11: 4151. https://doi.org/10.1080/08927936.1998.11425086CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dybdall, K, Strasser, R and Katz, T 2007 Behavioral differences between owner surrender and stray domestic cats after entering an animal shelter. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 104: 8594. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eagan, BH, Gordon, E and Protopopova, A 2022 Reasons for guardian-relinquishment of dogs to shelters: Animal and regional predictors in British Columbia, Canada. Frontiers in Veterinary Science 9: 448. https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2022.857634 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, SLH and Wells, DL 2010 The influence of olfactory stimulation on the behaviour of cats housed in a rescue shelter. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 123: 5662. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2009.12.011 Google Scholar
Frank, JM and Carlisle-Frank, PL 2007 Analysis of programs to reduce overpopulation of companion animals: Do adoption and low-cost spay/neuter programs merely cause substitution of sources? Ecological Economics 62: 740746. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.ECOLECON.2006.09.011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freiwald, A, Litster, A and Weng, HY 2014 Survey to investigate pet ownership and attitudes to pet care in metropolitan Chicago dog and/or cat owners. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 115: 198204. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PREVETMED.2014.03.025 Google ScholarPubMed
Gates, MC, Zito, S, Thomas, J and Dale, A 2018 Post-adoption problem behaviours in adolescent and adult dogs rehomed through a New Zealand animal shelter. Animals 8. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8060093 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glasser, CL 2021 Attitudes toward spay/neuter in the US population: Urban/rural, cat/dog, and demographic differences. Anthrozoos 34: 93107. https://doi.org/10.1080/08927936.2021.1874112 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grant, RA and Warrior, JR 2019 Clicker training increases exploratory behaviour and time spent at the front of the enclosure in shelter cats; Implications for welfare and adoption rates. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 211: 7783. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.APPLANIM.2018.12.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunter, LM, Barber, RT and Wynne, CDL 2018 A canine identity crisis: Genetic breed heritage testing of shelter dogs. PLoS ONE 13: e0202633. https://doi.org/10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0202633 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guy, NC, Luescher, UA, Dohoo, SE, Spangler, E, Miller, JB, Dohoo, IR and Bate, LA 2001 Demographic and aggressive characteristics of dogs in a general veterinary caseload. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 74: 1528. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(01)00153-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawes, SM, Kerrigan, JM, Hupe, T and Morris, KN 2020 Factors informing the return of adopted dogs and cats to an animal shelter. Animals 10: 115. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10091573 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hennessy, MB, Davis, HN, Williams, MT, Mellott, C and Douglas, CW 1997 Plasma cortisol levels of dogs at a county animal shelter. Physiology and Behavior 62: 485490. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(97)80328-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, SJ, Bateman, S, Coe, JB, Oblak, M and Veit, L 2021 The impact of deferred intake as part of Capacity for Care (C4C) on shelter cat outcomes. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2021.1894148Google ScholarPubMed
Horecka, K and Neal, S 2022 Critical problems for research in animal sheltering, a conceptual analysis. Frontiers in Veterinary Science 9: 292. https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2022.804154CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humane Canada 2017 Animal Shelter Statistics 2017. Humane Canada: Ottawa, Ontario, Canada.Google Scholar
HumanePro 2020 Adopters Welcome Manual. HumanePro: Ottawa, Ontario, Canada.Google Scholar
Hurley, KF 2022 The evolving role of triage and appointment-based admission to improve service, care and outcomes in animal shelters. Frontiers in Veterinary Science 9: 204. https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2022.809340CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jensen, JBH, Sandøe, P and Nielsen, SS 2020 Owner‐related reasons matter more than behavioural problems—a study of why owners relinquished dogs and cats to a Danish animal shelter from 1996 to 2017. Animals 10: 114. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10061064CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kobelt, AJ, Hemsworth, PH, Barnett, JL and Coleman, GJ 2003 A survey of dog ownership in suburban Australia—conditions and behaviour problems. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 82: 137148. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(03)00062-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kry, K and Casey, R 2007 The effect of hiding enrichment on stress levels and behaviour of domestic cats (Felis sylvestris catus) in a shelter setting and the implications for adoption potential. Animal Welfare, 16(3), 375383. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600027196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lambert, K, Coe, JB, Young, I, Dysart, L, Nogueira Borden, L and Rajić, A 2014 A scoping review of published research on the relinquishment of companion animals. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 17: 253273. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2014.899910Google Scholar
Lampe, R and Witte, TH 2015 Speed of dog adoption: Impact of online photo traits. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 18: 343354. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2014.982796CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lepper, M, Kass, PH and Hart, LA 2002 Prediction of adoption versus euthanasia among dogs and cats in a California animal shelter. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 5: 2942. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327604JAWS0501_3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luescher, AU and Tyson Medlock, R 2009 The effects of training and environmental alterations on adoption success of shelter dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 117: 6368. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.APPLANIM.2008.11.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lund, HS, Eggertsson, S, Grøndahl, AM and Eggertsdóttir, AV 2010 Views on euthanasia and the rehoming of dogs in Norway and Iceland. Veterinary Record 166: 749752. https://doi.org/10.1136/VR.B4856CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maddalena, SD, Zeidman, S and Campbell, K 2012 An empirical look at public perceptions and attitudes about pet adoption and spay/neuter. Proceedings of the Society of Animal Welfare. Petersburg, FL, USA.Google Scholar
Maddie’s Fund 2021 Removing Barriers to Adoption. https://www.maddiesfund.org/removing-barriers-to-adoption.htmGoogle Scholar
Markowitz, DM 2020 Putting your best pet forward: Language patterns of persuasion in online pet advertisements. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 50: 160173. https://doi.org/10.1111/JASP.12647CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, AL, Kelling, AS and Mallavarapu, S 2021 Attitudes toward dog relinquishment as assessed through a survey of university students. Anthrozoos 34: 201215. https://doi.org/10.1080/08927936.2021.1885142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGuire, B, Chan, J, Jean-Baptiste, K, Kok, P and Rosenbaum, E 2021 Results of behavioral evaluations predict length of stay for shelter dogs. Animals 11: 3272. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11113272CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mondelli, F, Previde, EP, Verga, M, Levi, D, Magistrelli, S and Valsecchi, P 2004 The bond that never developed: Adoption and relinquishment of dogs in a rescue shelter. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 7: 253266. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0704_3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
New, JC Jr, Salman, MD, Scarlett, JM, Kass, PH, Vaughn, JA, Scherr, S and Kelch, WJ 1999 Moving: Characteristics of dogs and cats and those relinquishing them to 12 US animal shelters. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 2: 8396. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0202_1Google ScholarPubMed
New, JC, Salman, MD, King, M, Scarlett, JM, Kass, PH and Hutchison, JM 2000 Characteristics of shelter relinquished animals and their owners compared with animals and their owners in U.S. pet-owning households. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 3: 179201. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0303_1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Normando, S, Stefanini, C, Meerst, L, Adamelli, S, Coultis, D and Bono, G 2006 Some factors influencing adoption of sheltered dogs. Anthrozoos 19: 211224. https://doi.org/10.2752/089279306785415556CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patronek, GJ, Bradley, J and Arps, E 2022 Saving normal: A new look at behavioral incompatibilities and dog relinquishment to shelters. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 49: 3645. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JVEB.2021.11.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patronek, GJ, Glickman, LT, Beck, AM, McCabe, GP and Ecker, C 1996 Risk factors for relinquishment of dogs to an animal shelter. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 209: 572581.Google Scholar
Pegram, C, Gray, C, Packer, RMA, Richards, Y, Church, DB, Brodbelt, DC and O’Neill, DG 2021 Proportion and risk factors for death by euthanasia in dogs in the UK. Scientific Reports 11: 112. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-88342-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posage, JM, Bartlett, PC and Thomas, DK 1998 Determining factors for successful adoption of dogs from an animal shelter. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 213: 478482.Google ScholarPubMed
Protopopova, A and Bollen, K 2022 Animal placement and follow‐up. Animal Behavior for Shelter Veterinarians and Staff pp 634658. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd: London, UK.Google Scholar
Protopopova, A, Gilmour, AJ, Weiss, RH, Shen, JY and Wynne, CDL 2012 The effects of social training and other factors on adoption success of shelter dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 142: 6168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2012.09.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Protopopova, A and Gunter, LM 2017 Adoption and relinquishment interventions at the animal shelter: A review. Animal Welfare 26: 3548. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.26.1.035CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Protopopova, A, Mehrkam, LR, Boggess, MM and Wynne, CDL 2014 In-kennel behavior predicts length of stay in shelter dogs. PLoS ONE 9: e114319. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0114319CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raudies, C, Waiblinger, S and Arhant, C 2021 Characteristics and welfare of long-term shelter dogs. Animals 11: 121. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11010194CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodriguez, JR, Davis, J, Hill, S, Wolf, PJ, Hawes, SM and Morris, KN 2022 Trends in intake and outcome data from US animal shelters from 2016 to 2020. Frontiers in Veterinary Science 9: 725. https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2022.863990CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
RStudio Team 2022 RStudio: Integrated Development for R. RStudio, PBC, Boston, MA, USA.Google Scholar
Scarlett, JM, Salman, MD, New, JG Jr and Kass, PH 1999 Reasons for relinquishment of companion animals in U.S. animal shelters: Selected health and personal issues. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 2: 4157. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0201_4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scheifele, P, Martin, D, Clark, JG, Kemper, D and Wells, J 2012 Effect of kennel noise on hearing in dogs. American Journal of Veterinary Research 73: 482489. https://doi.org/10.2460/AJVR.73.4.482CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, S, Jong, E, McArthur, M and Hazel, SJ 2018 Follow-up surveys of people who have adopted dogs and cats from an Australian shelter. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 201: 4045. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.APPLANIM.2017.12.021Google Scholar
Shelter Animals Count 2022 Intake and Outcome Data Comparison for Q1 of 2021 and 2022. https://www.shelteranimalscount.org/intake-and-outcome-data-comparison-for-q1-of-2021-and-2022/Google Scholar
Shore, ER and Girrens, K 2001 Characteristics of animals entering an animal control or humane society shelter in a midwestern city. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 4: 105115. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0402_2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siettou, C, Fraser, IM and Fraser, RW 2014 Investigating some of the factors that influence ‘consumer’ choice when adopting a shelter dog in the United Kingdom. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 17: 136147. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2014.883924CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephen, J and Ledger, R 2007 Relinquishing dog owners’ ability to predict behavioural problems in shelter dogs post adoption. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 107: 8899. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.APPLANIM.2006.09.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Voith, VL 2009 The impact of companion animal problems on society and the role of veterinarians. Veterinary Clinics of North America - Small Animal Practice 39: 327345. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cvsm.2008.10.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voith, VL, Ingram, E, Mitsouras, K and Irizarry, K 2009 Comparison of adoption agency breed identification and DNA breed identification of dogs. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 12: 253262. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888700902956151CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voith, VL, Trevejo, R, Dowling-Guyer, S, Chadik, C, Marder, A, Johnson, V and Irizarry, K 2013 Comparison of visual and DNA breed identification of dogs and inter-observer reliability. American Journal of Sociological Research 3: 1729. https://doi.org/10.5923/j.socioloGoogle Scholar
Voith, VL, Wright, JC and Danneman, PJ 1992 Is there a relationship between canine behavior problems and spoiling activities, anthropomorphism, and obedience training? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 34: 263272. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(05)80121-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, E, Gramann, S, Spain, C V and Slater, M 2015 Goodbye to a good friend: An exploration of the re-homing of cats and dogs in the U.S. S . Open Journal of Animal Sciences 5: 435456. https://doi.org/10.4236/ojas.2015.54046CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, E, Miller, K, Mohan-Gibbons, H and Vela, C 2012 Why did you choose this pet?: Adopters and pet selection preferences in five animal shelters in the United States. Animals 2: 144159. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani2020144CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiss, E, Slater, M, Garrison, L, Drain, N, Dolan, E, Scarlett, JM and Zawistowsk, SL 2014 Large dog relinquishment to two municipal facilities in New York city and Washington, DC: Identifying targets for intervention. Animals 4: 409433. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani4030409CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, D and Hepper, P 1992 The behaviour of dogs in a rescue shelter. Animal Welfare 1: 171186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, DL and Hepper, PG 2000 Prevalence of behaviour problems reported by owners of dogs purchased from an animal rescue shelter. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 69: 5565. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(00)00118-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White SC, Jefferson, E and Levy, JK 2010 Impact of publicly sponsored neutering programs on animal population dynamics at animal shelters: The new Hampshire and Austin experiences. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 13: 191212. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888700903579903Google Scholar
Workman, MK and Hoffman, CL 2015 An evaluation of the role the internet site Petfinder plays in cat adoptions. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 18: 388397. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2015.1043366CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zawistowski, S, Morris, J, Salman, MD and Ruch-Gallie, R 1998 Population dynamics, overpopulation, and the welfare of companion animals: New insights on old and new data. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1: 193206. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0103_1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zito, S, Morton, J, Vankan, D, Paterson, M, Bennett, PC, Rand, J and Phillips, CJC 2016 Reasons people surrender unowned and owned cats to Australian animal shelters and barriers to assuming ownership of unowned cats. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 19: 303319. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2016.1141682CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Animal- and owner-related characteristics across the total sample (n = 100,342) of dogs listed on ‘Rehome’, the diverted population, and dogs that were adopted to a new home or kept by their original owner. The data are from dogs listed on ‘Rehome’ from January 1, 2017 to May 21, 2021

Figure 1

Figure 1. Associations between dog and owner characteristics and whether dogs were diverted (= 1) or relinquished to animal shelters (= 0) after being posted on an online-supported, self-rehoming website (n = 10,322). Data are presented by odds ratios and their 95% confidence interval (error bars); P < 0.05 when the 95% CI does not cross the vertical dotted line.

Figure 2

Table 2. Animal- and owner-related characteristics across the total sample (n = 48,484) of cats listed on ‘Rehome’, the diverted population, and cats that were adopted to a new home or kept by their original owner. The data are from cats listed on ‘Rehome’ from January 1, 2017 to May 21, 2021

Figure 3

Figure 2. Associations between cat and owner characteristics and whether cats were diverted (= 1) or relinquished to animal shelters (= 0) after being posted on an online-supported, self-rehoming website (n = 5,545). Data are presented by odds ratios and their 95% confidence interval (error bars); P < 0.05 when the 95% CI does not cross the vertical dotted line.

Figure 4

Figure 3. Associations between dog and owner characteristics and whether dogs were kept by their original owner (= 1) or adopted to a new home (= 0) after being posted on an online-supported, self-rehoming website (n = 52,886). Data are presented by odds ratios and their 95% confidence interval (error bars); P < 0.05 when the 95% CI does not cross the vertical dotted line.

Figure 5

Figure 4. Associations between cat and owner characteristics and whether cats were kept by their original owner (= 1) or adopted to a new home (= 0) after being posted on an online-supported, self-rehoming website (n = 23,488). Data are presented by odds ratios and their 95% confidence interval (error bars); P < 0.05 when the 95% CI does not cross the vertical dotted line.