Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T05:49:25.882Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

There are no Known Differences in Brain Mechanisms of Consciousness Between Humans and Other Mammals

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2023

B J Baars*
Affiliation:
The Wright Institute, California, USA
*
Current address: The Neurosciences Institute, San Diego, California, USA
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Recent scientific findings indicate that consciousness is a fundamental biological adaptation. The known brain correlates of consciousness appear to be phylogenetically ancient, going back at least to early mammals. In all mammals, alertness and sensory consciousness are required for the goal-directed behaviours that make species survival and reproduction possible. In all mammals, the anatomy, neurochemistry and electrical activity of the brain in alert states show striking similarities.

After more than seven decades of cumulative discoveries about waking and sensory consciousness, we have not yet found any fundamental differences between humans and other mammals. Species differences such as the size of neocortex seem to be irrelevant to the existence of alertness and sensory consciousness, though different mammals obviously specialize in different kinds of sensory, cognitive and motor abilities.

Sceptics sometimes claim that objective evidence for consciousness tells us little about subjective experience, such as the experience of conscious pain. Scientifically, however, plausible inferences are routinely based on reliable and consistent patterns of evidence. In other humans, we invariably infer subjective experiences from objective behavioural and brain evidence - if someone yells ‘Ouch!’ after striking a finger with a hammer, we infer that they feel pain. The brain and behavioural evidence for subjective consciousness is essentially identical in humans and other mammals. On the weight of the objective evidence, therefore, subjective experience would seem to be equally plausible in all species with human-like brains and behaviour. Either we deny this experience to other humans (which is rarely done) or, to be consistent, we must also attribute it to other species that meet the same objective standards. It seems that the burden of proof for the absence of subjectivity in mammals should be placed on the sceptics.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2001 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

References

Alkire, M T, Haier, R J and Fallon, J H Toward the unified theory of narcosis: brain imaging evidence for a thalamocortical switch as the neurophysiological basis of anesthetic-induced unconsciousness. Consciousness and Cognition (in press)Google Scholar
Baars, B J 1986 The Cognitive Revolution in Psychology. Guilford Press: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Baars, B J 1987 Biological implications of a Global Workspace theory of consciousness: evidence, theory, and some phylogenetic speculations. In: Greenberg, G and Tobach, E (eds) Cognition, Language, and Consciousness: Integrative Levels pp 209236. Erlbaum: Hillsdale, USAGoogle Scholar
Baars, B J 1988 A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness. Cambridge University Press: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Baars, B J 1993 How does a serial, integrated and very limited stream of consciousness emerge from a nervous system that is mostly unconscious, distributed, parallel and of enormous capacity? Theoretical and Experimental Studies of Consciousness, Ciba Foundation Symposium 174: 282–90; 291303Google ScholarPubMed
Baars, B J 1998 Metaphors of consciousness and attention in the brain. Trends in Neurosciences 21: 5862CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baars, B J and Newman, J (eds) Essential Sources in the Scientific Study of Consciousness. MIT Press/Bradford Books: Cambridge, USA (in press)Google Scholar
Block, N 1995 On a confusion about the function of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18: 227287CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blumenthal, A L 1979 Wilhelm Wundt - the founding father we never knew. Contemporary Psychology 24: 547550CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogen, J E 1995 On the neurophysiology of consciousness: I. An overview. Consciousness and Cognition 4: 5262CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bressler, S L and Freeman, W J 1980 Frequency analysis of olfactory system EEG in cat, rabbit, and rat. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 50: 1924CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cirelli, C, Pompeiano, M and Tononi, G 1996 Neuronal gene expression in the waking state: a role for the locus ceruleus. Science 274: 12111215CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowey, A and Stoerig, P 1995 Blindsight in monkeys. Nature 373: 247249CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darwin, C 1838-1840 Old and useless notes. In: Barrett, P H, Gautrey, P J, Herbert, S, Kohn, D and Smith, S (eds) Charles Darwin's Notebooks, 1836-1844: Geology, Transmutation of Species, Metaphysical Enquiries. Cornell University Press: Ithaca, USAGoogle Scholar
Destexhe, A, Contreras, D and Steriade, M 1999 Spatiotemporal analysis of local field potentials and unit discharges in cat cerebral cortex during natural wake and sleep states. Journal of Neuroscience 19: 45954608CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edelman, G 1989 The Remembered Present: a Biological Theory of Consciousness. Basic Books: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Freeman, W J 1991 The physiology of perception. Scientific American 264: 7885CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geschwind, N 1979 Specializations of the human brain. Scientific American 241: 180201CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hilgard, E R 1987 Psychology in America: a Historical Survey. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Hobson, J A 1997 Consciousness as a state-dependent phenomenon. In: Cohen, J D and Schooler, J W (eds) Scientific Approaches to Consciousness: the XXVth Carnegie Symposium on Cognition. L Erlbaum Ass: Mahwah, USAGoogle Scholar
Hobson, J A and Brazier, MAB (eds) 1980 The Reticular Formation Revisited: Specifying Function for a Non-specific System. Raven Press: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
James, W 1890/1983 The Principles of Psychology. Holt: New York, USA/Harvard: Cambridge, USAGoogle Scholar
Livingstone, M S and Hubel, D H 1981 Effects of sleep and arousal on the processing of visual information in the cat. Nature 291: 554561CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logothetis, N K and Schall, J D 1989 Neuronal correlates of subjective visual perception. Science 245: 761763CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magoun, H W 1962 The Waking Brain, 2nd edition. Thomas: Springfield, USAGoogle Scholar
Morgan, C L 1896 Habit and Instinct. Arnold: London, UKGoogle Scholar
Moruzzi, G and Magoun, H W 1949 Brain stem reticular formation and activation of the EEG. Electroencephalopathy and Clinical Neurophysiology 1: 455473CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rechtschaffen, A and Siegel, J M 2000 Sleep and dreaming. In: Kandel, E R, Schwartz, J H and Jessel, T M (eds) Principles of Neuroscience pp 936947. McGraw Hill: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Scheibel, A B 1980 Anatomical and physiological substrates of arousal. In: Hobson, J A and Brazier, MAB (eds) The Reticular Formation Revisited: Specifying Function for a Non-specific System pp 5566. Raven Press: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Sheinberg, D L and Logothetis, N K 1997 The role of temporal cortical areas in perceptual organization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 94: 34083413CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner B Γ 1976 Particulars of My Life. Knopf: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Steriade, M 1995 Brain activation then (1949) and now: coherent fast rhythms in corticothalamic networks. Archives Italiennes de Biologie 134: 520Google ScholarPubMed
Tononi, G and Edelman, G M 1999 Consciousness and complexity. Science 282: 18461851CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Essen, D C and Anderson, C H 1995 Information processing strategies and pathways in the visual system. In: An Introduction to Neural and Electronic Networks, 2nd edition. Academic Press: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Watson, J B 1925 Behaviorism. Norton: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Weiskrantz, L 1991 Disconnected awareness for detecting, processing, and remembering in neurological patients. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine 84: 466470CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeki, S 1993 A Vision of the Brain. Blackwell Scientific: London, UKGoogle Scholar