Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T06:11:47.010Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

What we (don’t) know about parrot welfare: Finding welfare indicators through a systematic literature review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 December 2024

Andrea Piseddu*
Affiliation:
Centre for Animal Nutrition and Welfare, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, Veterinaerplatz 1, 1210 Vienna, Austria
Yvonne RA van Zeeland
Affiliation:
Division of Zoological Medicine, Department of Clinical Sciences, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Utrecht University, Yalelaan 108, 3584 CM Utrecht, The Netherlands
Jean-Loup Rault
Affiliation:
Centre for Animal Nutrition and Welfare, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, Veterinaerplatz 1, 1210 Vienna, Austria
*
Corresponding author: Andrea Piseddu; Email: andrea.piseddu@vetmeduni.ac.at
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Parrots are popular companion animals but show prevalent and at times severe welfare issues. Nonetheless, there are no scientific tools available to assess parrot welfare. The aim of this systematic review was to identify valid and feasible outcome measures that could be used as welfare indicators for companion parrots. From 1,848 peer-reviewed studies retrieved, 98 met our inclusion and exclusion criteria (e.g. experimental studies, captive parrots). For each outcome collected, validity was assessed based on the statistical significance reported by the authors, as other validity parameters were rarely provided for evaluation. Feasibility was assigned by considering the need for specific instruments, veterinary-level expertise or handling the parrot. A total of 1,512 outcomes were evaluated, of which 572 had a significant P-value and were considered feasible. These included changes in behaviour (e.g. activity level, social interactions, exploration), body measurements (e.g. body weight, plumage condition) and abnormal behaviours, amongst others. Many physical and physiological parameters were identified that either require experimental validation, or veterinary-level skills and expertise, limiting their potential use by parrot owners themselves. Moreover, a high risk of bias undermined the internal validity of these outcomes, while a strong taxonomic bias, a predominance of studies on parrots in laboratories, and an underrepresentation of companion parrots jeopardised their external validity. These results provide a promising starting point for validating a set of welfare indicators in parrots.

Type
Systematic Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

Parrots have always fascinated human beings, influencing art, literature, and religion across centuries and continents (Boehrer Reference Boehrer2010). Today, these birds, belonging to the order Psittaciformes (Gill et al. Reference Gill, Donsker and Rasmussen2022), are one of the most popular companion animals after dogs and cats (Kidd & Kidd Reference Kidd and Kidd1998; Meyers Reference Meyers1998; Anderson Reference Anderson2003; Engebretson Reference Engebretson2006). Such popularity can be attributed to their bright and colourful plumages, and to their learning abilities which are comparable to those of human toddlers (Pepperberg & Funk Reference Pepperberg and Funk1990; Pepperberg Reference Pepperberg2009; Spierings & ten Cate Reference Spierings and ten Cate2016; Eggleston et al. Reference Eggleston, Viloria, Delgado, Mata, Guerrero, Kline, Beissinger and Berg2022). Parrots, supported by large and neuron-rich forebrains (Emery Reference Emery2006; Olkowicz et al. Reference Olkowicz, Kocourek, Lučan, Porteš, Fitch, Herculano-Houzel and Němec2016), can use and even manufacture tools (Auersperg et al. Reference Auersperg, Von Bayern, Gajdon, Huber and Kacelnik2011, Reference Auersperg, Szabo, von Bayern and Kacelnik2012, Reference Auersperg, Köck, O’Hara and Huber2018; Lambert et al. Reference Lambert, Seed and Slocombe2015), think economically (Laumer et al. Reference Laumer, Bugnyar and Auersperg2016; Krasheninnikova et al. Reference Krasheninnikova, Höner, O’Neill, Penna and von Bayern2018), succeed in problem-solving, reasoning and planning tasks (Rössler & Auersperg Reference Rössler and Auersperg2022), and even remember their own past actions (Torres Ortiz et al. Reference Torres Ortiz, Smeele, Champenois and von Bayern2022), an important prerequisite for self-awareness. Parrots also show different kinds of social competences: they can co-operate during problem-solving tasks (Tebbich et al. Reference Tebbich, Taborsky and Winkler1996; Péron et al. Reference Péron, Rat-Fischer, Lalot, Nagle and Bovet2011; Schwing et al. Reference Schwing, Jocteur, Wein, Noë and Massen2016, Reference Schwing, Meaux, Piseddu, Huber and Noë2021), learn from conspecifics (Auersperg et al. Reference Auersperg, von Bayern, Weber, Szabadvari, Bugnyar and Kacelnik2014; Klump et al. Reference Klump, Martin, Wild, Hörsch, Major and Aplin2021) and exhibit prosocial behaviours (Krasheninnikova et al. Reference Krasheninnikova, Brucks, Blanc and von Bayern2019; Brucks & von Bayern Reference Brucks and von Bayern2020; Laumer et al. Reference Laumer, Massen, Boehm, Boehm, Geisler and Auersperg2021). Similar to humans and a few other species, they can learn and imitate sounds (Pepperberg Reference Pepperberg2009; Vernes et al. Reference Vernes, Kriengwatana, Beeck, Fischer, Tyack, ten Cate and Janik2021), and synchronise their motor output on incoming rhythmic acoustic or visual information (Patel et al. Reference Patel, Iversen, Bregman and Schulz2009; Schachner et al. Reference Schachner, Brady, Pepperberg and Hauser2009; Hasegawa et al. Reference Hasegawa, Okanoya, Hasegawa and Seki2011).

These characteristics render parrots valuable and desirable companion animals (Kidd & Kidd Reference Kidd and Kidd1998; Anderson Reference Anderson2014). However, their high social needs and cognitive abilities, along with specific dietary and husbandry requirements, also render companion parrots prone to developing serious health and welfare issues in captivity. Some common welfare issues are nutritional deficiencies (e.g. hypocalcaemia, hypovitaminosis A), and associated pathologies (e.g. metabolic bone disease, egg binding, secondary infections), other (non-infectious) diseases (e.g. atherosclerosis, obesity), and development of fear-related, aggressive, stereotypic and/or self-injurious behaviours such as feather-damaging behaviour (Koski Reference Koski2002; Engebretson Reference Engebretson2006; Kalmar et al. Reference Kalmar, Janssens and Moons2010; Speer et al. Reference Speer, Olsen, Doneley, Monks, Verstappen, Yarto-Jaramillo, Elliott, Weston, Rivero, McDonald and Speer2016; Seibert Reference Seibert2020).

Experts from 51 different countries predict an increase in the trading of parrots due to their popularity (Ribeiro et al. Reference Ribeiro, Reino, Schindler, Strubbe, Vall-llosera, Araújo, Capinha, Carrete, Mazzoni, Monteiro, Moreira, Rocha, Tella, Vaz, Vicente and Nuno2019). This will likely have detrimental consequences for the conservation of this highly threatened taxon (Olah et al. Reference Olah, Butchart, Symes, Guzmán, Cunningham, Brightsmith and Heinsohn2016; IUCN 2024), but it also implies an increase of companion parrots population. Despite this prediction and the well-known welfare challenges of keeping captive parrots, there are currently no standardised guidelines for evaluating companion parrot welfare. Identifying welfare indicators would enable significant advancements to be made in companion parrot welfare, as they could be used for instantaneous assessments and in a repeated manner to monitor and evaluate changes in the parrots’ welfare. Additionally, it could increase a caregiver’s understanding of parrots’ needs, ultimately improving parrots’ overall quality of life. Suitable welfare indicators can be identified by following five key steps.

The first step is to find and collect information from peer-reviewed scientific studies. Integrating scientific information represents the most appropriate strategy, as it allows welfare assessors to employ standardised and objective methods, lowering the risk of making assessments biased by personal experience, mood, and emotional subjective states (Tuyttens et al. Reference Tuyttens, de Graaf, Heerkens, Jacobs, Nalon, Ott, Stadig, Van Laer and Ampe2014; Mota-Rojas et al. Reference Mota-Rojas, Mariti, Zdeinert, Riggio, Mora-Medina, del Mar Reyes, Gazzano, Domínguez-Oliva, Lezama-García, José-Pérez and Hernández-Ávalos2021).

However, systematic reviews and simulations have shown that, due to weak experimental designs and settings, single research studies carry a high risk of bias, which is defined as “a systematic error or deviation from the truth, in results or inferences” (Boutron et al. Reference Boutron, Page, Higgins, Altman, Lundh and Hróbjartsson2019). For this reason, the second step to identify welfare indicators is to verify the internal validity of the scientific findings collected. Internal validity is defined as “the extent to which the design and conduct of a study are likely to have prevented bias” , and it is typically divided in four sub-categories: construct validity (i.e. the extent to which a test measures what it is intended to measure) (Cronbach & Meehl Reference Cronbach and Meehl1955), face validity (i.e. appropriateness of the test and its parameters) (Gravetter & Forzano Reference Gravetter and Forzano2012), content validity (i.e. the extent to which the test covers the entire construct) (Lawshe Reference Lawshe1975), and criterion validity (i.e. the extent to which the outcomes of the test aligns with those previously obtained with validated instruments or “gold standards”) (Bellamy Reference Bellamy, Hochberg, Silman, Smolen, Weinblatt and Weisman2015). “Internal validity” and “risk of bias” are closely associated (Viswanathan et al. Reference Viswanathan, Ansari, Berkman, Chang, Hartling, McPheeters, Santaguida, Shamliyan, Singh, Tsertsvadze and Treadwell2008); in fact, when a test presents high risk of bias, its results cannot be considered internally valid. Similarly, reliability, which is defined as production of consistent results within the same subject (“test-retest reliability”) (Gravetter & Forzano Reference Gravetter and Forzano2012), or between (“inter-observer reliability”) and within (“intra-observer reliability”) observers (Martin & Bateson Reference Martin and Bateson2007), is an important prerequisite for internal validity. A reliable measure is not necessarily valid; however, when the measure is not reliable, it cannot be valid (Gravetter & Forzano Reference Gravetter and Forzano2012). As such, it is important to screen scientific studies according to the aforementioned parameters to assess their internal validity.

The third step is to verify the studies’ external validity, i.e. the extent to which the findings of a study can be generalised and applied to other species, environmental conditions, or experimental settings (Lehner Reference Lehner1998; Bailoo et al. Reference Bailoo, Reichlin and Würbel2014). This is especially important in the case of parrots as the Psittaciformes order comprises a vast diversity of species. Unlike ‘dog’, ‘cat’ or ‘rabbit’, ‘parrot’ is a general term grouping more than 350 species that are adapted to different ecological niches and have distinct environmental, dietary, and behavioural needs (Bright-Smith Reference Bright-Smith1999). This raises the question whether conclusions drawn from studies on a single species can be applied to other species (Hill & Broom Reference Hill and Broom2009). For our purposes, it is necessary to determine whether and to what extent the species of interest, i.e. those commonly kept as companion animals, have been studied. Similarly, the setting in which the study results have been obtained should be considered as living conditions in a zoo, shelter or laboratory differ markedly from those in a private household, thereby implying that results obtained under these circumstances are not necessarily applicable to parrots kept as companions.

The fourth step is to identify feasible measurements. As suggested by Yon and colleagues (Reference Yon, Williams, Harvey and Asher2019), animal welfare assessments should ideally be “rapid, non-invasive and should not require any specialist equipment, facilities or specific training”. This considerably reduces the risk of errors due to, for instance, assessors’ tiredness, instruments’ accuracy and sensitivity, or the animal’s reaction in response to handling.

According to Fraser (Reference Fraser2008), animal welfare should be assessed by employing measurements that reflect three distinct inextricable conceptual frameworks: the animal’s affective state, its biological functioning, and natural living. As such, the fifth and final step requires capturing the various welfare dimensions through different indicators. Although behavioural indicators are considered the best reflection of animals’ ability to cope with their environment (Hill & Broom Reference Hill and Broom2009), a more accurate welfare assessment can be obtained by combining behaviour with other measurements, including physical condition, physiological parameters, presence of disease and pathologies, husbandry, nutrition, and management considerations.

Given the current lack of science-based welfare indicators to evaluate the welfare of captive parrots, we conducted a systematic literature review in which we reframed the five key steps according to the following research questions:

  1. (i) Which, if any, scientific results related to the welfare of captive parrots can be considered valid and feasible welfare indicators?

  2. (ii) How many and which types of welfare indicators have been identified?

  3. (iii) From how many and which parrot taxa have these indicators been collected? and

  4. (iv) How much and what type of information is available specifically regarding companion parrots?

Although our main target were companion parrots, we also collected information gathered from studies focused on other types of captive parrots, with the objective of identifying welfare indicators still applicable to our category of interest.

Materials and methods

All phases of this study were conducted following the PRISMA 2020 statement for reporting systematic reviews (Page et al. Reference Page MJ, McKenzie, Bossuyt, Boutron, Hoffmann, Mulrow, Shamseer, Tetzlaff, Akl, Brennan, Chou, Glanville, Grimshaw, Hróbjartsson, Lalu, Li, Loder, Mayo-Wilson, McDonald, McGuinness, Stewart, Thomas, Tricco, Welch, Whiting and Moher2021).

Systematic search

A systematic search was conducted to find all scientific, peer-reviewed studies relevant to the research questions. The population, intervention, control, outcomes (‘PICO’) strategy (Nishikawa-Pacher Reference Nishikawa-Pacher2022) was followed as much as possible and allowed to identify key terms related to the population of interest, to the type of intervention, and to the outcomes collected (note that the ‘control’ was not included as a search term as we did not restrict our search to case-control studies focusing on comparison of two interventions or comparison of the intervention with a control). We used terms such as ‘parrot’, ‘parakeet’, ‘psittacids’ and the specific type of parrot (e.g. macaw, grey parrot) for the population; terms related to nutrition, husbandry and management (e.g. ‘foraging enrichment’, ‘diet’, ‘hand-rearing’) for the intervention; and terms such as ‘abnormal behaviour’, ‘disease’, ‘life span’, ‘emotional state’, or specific behaviour problems, diseases or pathologies (e.g. ‘feather picking’, ‘atherosclerosis’, ‘obesity’) for the outcome (for the complete list of the 86 search key terms, see Table S1 in the Supplementary material). These key terms were then combined to create a search query using the Boolean operators AND, OR and NOT, and the queries were uploaded on the advanced search tool of the databases, PubMed, CAB Direct and Web of Science (on May 16th, 2022).

Paper selection: Title and abstract screen, full-text screen

Following the literature search, the scientific studies found were uploaded into a reference manager (Endnote X7; The EndNoteTeam 2013). After removal of duplicates and triplicates, all remaining texts were screened twice using exclusion and inclusion criteria (Table S2; Supplementary material). Studies could involve parrots from any species, gender or age, and a variety of different interventions and outcomes (as specified in Table S2) and were included for further evaluation as long as the study was conducted in a captive population and was not focused on reproductive parameters (egg hatchability, number of eggs) or chick development as we considered these irrelevant for companion parrots. Further restrictions were related to language (English only), methodology (no studies involving < 5 subjects or without statistical analysis), publication type (full papers describing original research only) and retrievability of the paper. The first screening, based on the title and abstract, was conducted by one reviewer (AP), and aimed to exclude all studies that were considered irrelevant to address the research questions. All studies that passed this initial screening subsequently underwent a second screening, in which the full-text was read by two independent reviewers (AP, J-LR, see Supplementary material). Additional studies found through external sources (e.g. cited references) were also considered for their eligibility to ensure to encompass the latest literature available (updated until January 27th, 2023).

Data collection

Collection of the outcome measures and corresponding risk factors

All behavioural, physiological, physical and health parameters that could potentially be linked to parrot welfare were collected from studies that passed the exclusion and inclusion criteria. Only parameters that were included as “outcome measures” (Percie du Sert et al. Reference Percie du Sert, Ahluwalia, Alam, Avey, Baker, Browne, Clark, Cuthill, Dirnagl, Emerson, Garner, Holgate, Howells, Hurst, Karp, Lazic, Lidster, MacCallum, Macleod, Pearl, Petersen, Rawle, Reynolds, Rooney, Sena, Silberberg, Steckler and Würbel2020), i.e. as part of the experimental design, were considered for further evaluation. For instance, outcome measures such as feeding behaviour or body weight were collected and considered only when measured specifically in relation to examined environmental conditions (e.g. social isolation, unbalanced diet, cage size or use of enrichment items) but not when correlated with natural biological phenomena (e.g. breeding season changes). To consider an outcome measure as a potential welfare indicator, a significant statistical link has to exist between this measure and the examined environmental conditions. A significant P-value in fact reflects that this outcome measure (e.g. behaviours, body measurement) is sensitive to a specific environmental condition that can therefore be considered a risk factor for parrot welfare (e.g. being housed alone vs in group, enrichment provided vs not provided). For this reason, we collected the P-value associated with each outcome measure and its corresponding risk factor. If not explicitly written by the authors, the potential risk factors were interpreted from the experimental conditions. For instance, in studies that compared behaviours of enriched and non-enriched parrots, the main risk factor for welfare was the lack of enrichment, in studies comparing the behaviours of hand-reared vs parent-reared subjects, the risk factor was being hand-reared, whereas for studies where parrots displayed a preference for a specific object or food item, the risk factor was a lack of offered choices. All risk factors associated with outcome measures with a P-value < 0.05 (both feasible and non-feasible) that were easy to define and identify (e.g. social isolation, diet, cage size etc) were also collected.

Internal validity and feasibility of outcome measures

Aside from evaluating presence of a significant P-value, hence significant correlation between environmental parameter and outcome measure, outcome measures were also assessed for internal validity or risk of bias, which was a second criterion for assessing overall validity of welfare parameters. As multiple outcome measures could be identified from a single study, and because our focus was on identifying all outcome measures that could be used as parrot welfare indicators, we assessed the internal validity of each outcome measure separately rather than for the study as a whole, as is commonplace for most systematic reviews. For this purpose, we employed selected domains of the SYRCLE’s RoB tool (Hooijmans et al. Reference Hooijmans, Rovers, de Vries, Leenaars, Ritskes-Hoitinga and Langendam2014): from the main text, we extrapolated whether data were measured and collected by a blinded assessor, whether studied population was randomised (where applicable) and, in case of experimental set-ups with multiple conditions, whether the design was balanced between subjects and/or groups. In addition, we checked for tests of intra- and inter-observer reliability (see Table S3 in the Supplementary material for definitions of each of the evaluated validity parameters).

Given that we aimed to find welfare indicators that could be used to assess parrot welfare in practice, we also evaluated the outcome measures for their feasibility. They were classified as feasible if the behavioural assessment or measurements could be readily performed, requiring only the use of commonly available equipment (e.g. weight scale) or the use of minimally invasive routine handling techniques, or as non-feasible if the use of specific instrumentation, calculation or veterinarian-level skills or expertise were required.

Grouping of outcome measures in welfare categories and welfare dimensions

To facilitate interpretability of the results and determine how many outcome measures of a certain type could be identified, these were first grouped in categories according to commonalities in their underlying biological construct (e.g. stereotypic behaviour, body condition, foraging behaviour). These categories were then classified into one of eight distinct welfare dimensions, which were created by grouping the welfare categories according to their shared characteristics. (i.e. physical or physiological measures, abnormal and fear-related behaviours, maintenance behaviours, locomotory behaviours, exploratory and foraging behaviours, social behaviours, and diseases and pathologic conditions, see Table S4; Supplementary material).

Extrapolability of outcome measures across species and settings

To determine the extent to which outcome measures would be applicable or extrapolable to other species or settings, we collected taxonomic data (parrot species and genus), where available. Data acquired from multiple genera were classified under the category ‘multiple’. Following the definition of ‘pet’ or ‘companion’ animal proposed by Farnworth (Reference Farnworth, Vonk and Shackelford2018), the studied subjects were identified as companion parrots when they “lived with humans or within human social structures where they were provided with some, or all, of their needs” and “played a primarily social role within a household or community”. Alternatively, we identified the parrots based on their living conditions as parrots kept in laboratories, zoos, shelters or breeding centres (see Table S5 in the Supplementary material for the complete list and definitions of subjects’ living conditions).

Data analysis

Outcomes with a significant P-value (alpha threshold fixed at 0.05) were identified and subsequently screened for their feasibility. The outcome measures with a significant P-value and considered feasible were then grouped according to the welfare categories and dimensions they belonged, and according to subjects’ characteristics (genus and living condition). Data were analysed using the R statistical software (R CoreTeam 2022) and the R package ‘dplyr’ (Wickham Reference Wickham, François, Henry, Müller and Vaughan2023). Figures were created using the R package ‘ggplot2’ (Wickham Reference Wickham2016).

Results

Result of the systematic search and paper selection

The systematic search led to the collection of 1,946 scientific studies: 697 from CAB direct, 657 from Web of Science and 592 from PubMed. A total of 189 studies were found to be duplicates and triplicates and, after removing these, the total amount of hits dropped to 1,848. The first screening, based on title and abstract reading, led to the selection of 140 studies. The second screening, based on full-text reading, led to the retention of 83 studies. An additional 15 studies were found from external sources. This screening step led to a final amount of 98 studies from which data were collected (Figure 1).

Figure 1. PRISMA flowchart identifying the number of studies reporting on parrot welfare parameters retrieved during the literature search from each database (PubMed, Web of Science, CAB Direct) and via other methods, the number of studies subjected to a first screening based on title and abstract reading and a second based on eligibility criteria, studies excluded during both screening phases, and the number of studies included in the final review.

General results

The year of publication of eligible studies ranged from 1993 to 2023, with 76 studies (77.6%) published between 2010 and 2023. Of the studies that were eligible for full evaluation, 95 (96.9%) reported significant results and, of these, 72 (73.5%) reported outcome measures that were considered feasible to be carried out by owners (see Table S8; Supplementary material). Only 13 of these 72 studies with significant and feasible outcomes (13.3%) specifically related to companion parrots (see Table S8). The number of outcomes collected in a study ranged from 1 to 100 (median: 11). Of the total of 1,512 outcomes collected, 720 (47.6%) had a significant P-value, and of those 572 (37.8%) were also considered feasible. Of these 572 outcomes, 68 (4.49%) were obtained from companion parrots.

Risk of bias

Intra- and inter-observer reliability and assessor blindness were reported for less than 5% of the outcome measures and were not specified for more than 77% of the outcomes (Table 1). In addition, a high number of outcome measures were from studies that, due to their experimental set-up, did not allow to control for the presence of biases or prevent it. For instance, outcomes collected from questionnaires and retrospective studies (17.5%) could not be tested for intra- and inter-observer reliability, and random assignment of subjects to groups, assessor blindness and balancing of experimental conditions could not be applied to this type of studies. Another example came from studies where subjects were assigned to control and enriched groups: in this case, it may not have been possible to blind the assessors and therefore to control for this specific bias. Due to these circumstances, we could not establish with certainty the internal validity of the findings collected from the studies.

Table 1. Assessment of the risk of bias for the outcome measures (n = 1,512) related to parrot welfare identified in the systematic literature search by using five validity parameters. The percentages refer to outcome measures for which the validity parameters, as indicated in the table, were reported by the authors (‘Yes’), were not considered by the author (‘No’), were not executable (‘Not possible’), or data regarding the validity parameter were not reported in the main text (‘Not specified’)

Representation of welfare dimensions and categories

Outcomes classified according to welfare dimensions

Out of the eight welfare dimensions, the welfare dimension with the highest number of feasible and significant outcomes was ‘social behaviours’ (n = 141), whereas for most other dimensions the number of significant and feasible outcomes ranged between 80 and 93 (Figure 2, Table S9; Supplementary material). The welfare dimension ‘physiological parameters’ included a high number of significant outcomes (n = 97); however, all of these were considered not feasible as they required invasive sampling techniques (e.g. venipuncture) and laboratory equipment. A similar trend was noted for the welfare dimension ‘diseases and pathologic conditions’, albeit the number of outcomes reported was lower to start with (Figure 2).

Figure 2. Number of outcomes related to parrot welfare as identified in the systematic literature search, grouped by welfare dimensions according to the biological construct that they represent. For each welfare dimension, the overlapped bar plot indicates, from darkest to lightest colour, the total number of outcome parameters collected, the number of significant outcomes (i.e. P-value < 0.05), the number of significant outcomes that are considered feasible for owners to assess (i.e. not requiring specific skills, expertise or equipment), and the number of significant and feasible outcomes collected from companion parrots (smallest bar).

Outcome measures classified according to categories

The significant and feasible outcome measures were grouped in 26 different welfare categories (Table S10; Supplementary material). ‘Stereotypies’ covered the highest number of significant and feasible outcomes measures (n = 76) and captured oral stereotypies (e.g. wire or sham chewing), head stereotypies (e.g. spot pecking), and locomotor stereotypies (e.g. pacing, route tracing). ‘Indirect measures of feather-damaging behaviour’ included multiple scoring methods related to feather condition or feather improvement and was the category with the second highest number of significant and feasible outcome measures (n = 69), followed by ‘self-care’ (n = 42) with behaviours such as preening and stretching (Table S10). ‘Human-animal interaction’ was the category with the highest variety and included measures such as response to unfamiliar and familiar handlers, human-direct aggressiveness, and food acceptance (Table S10). Outcome measures, such as body weight and body mass, were grouped in the category ‘body condition’; walking, climbing, and flying in ‘locomotion’; food intake and feeding bout in ‘feeding’; crown, nape, cheek feather ruffling, crest erection and beak grinding in ‘facial and body displays’. Of the 68 significant and feasible outcomes measures collected from companion parrots, 51 referred to feather-damaging behaviour, five to human-animal interactions, five to stereotypies, three to fear-related behaviour, one to foraging behaviour, and one to sexual behaviour (Table S12; Supplementary material).

Association between risk factors and outcomes measures

Significant and not feasible outcome measures were grouped in eight welfare categories and were related to various risk factors (Table S11; Supplementary material). For instance, human (neonatal) handling was linked to changes in the immune system (Collette et al. Reference Collette, Millam, Klasing and Wakenell2000), increased respiration rate (Aengus & Millam Reference Aengus and Millam1999) and increased serum corticosterone concentrations (Collette et al. Reference Collette, Millam, Klasing and Wakenell2000); social isolation negatively affected telomere length which has been associated with shorter lifespan (Aydinonat et al. Reference Aydinonat, Penn, Smith, Moodley, Hoelzl, Knauer and Schwarzenberger2014); and indoor housing and lack of UV-B lighting increased the risk for vitamin D deficiency (West et al. Reference West, Tully, Nevarez and Stout2019; Nightengale et al. Reference Nightengale, Stout and Tully2022). An unbalanced diet was correlated with changes in several parameters, including feather colour (crown feathers luminance) (Berg et al. Reference Berg, Knott, Ribot, Buchanan and Bennett2019); immune system responses (increased haemoglobin, lymphocyte, monocyte and leucocyte counts, increased heterophil/lymphocyte ratio) (Berg et al. Reference Berg, Knott, Ribot, Buchanan and Bennett2019; Di Santo et al. Reference Di Santo, Braos, Kawanami, Oliveira, Cruz, Mendonça, Peixoto and Carciofi2019); increased plasma, aortic, arterial and hepatic cholesterol levels (Beaufrere et al. Reference Beaufrere, Nevarez, Wakamatsu, Clubb, Cray and Tully2013); changes in echocardiographic parameters associated with cardiovascular dysfunctions (Dos Santos et al. Reference Dos Santos, Aleixo, Hippólito, Ferro, Okamoto, Lourenço, de Vasconcelos Machado, Ramos, Rahal, Teixeira and Melchert2022); and increased incidence of atherosclerosis (Di Santo et al. Reference Di Santo, Braos, Kawanami, Oliveira, Cruz, Mendonça, Peixoto and Carciofi2019) (Table S11; Supplementary material).

Risk factors were also identified for significant and feasible outcome measures. For instance, the lack of environmental enrichment was associated with 18 welfare categories, including emergence of stereotypies and feather-damaging behaviours, decrease of physical activity and preening (Table S10; Supplementary material). Social isolation represented a risk factor for developing stereotypies (Williams et al. Reference Williams, Hoppitt and Grant2017), and was associated with reduced preening (Williams et al. Reference Williams, Hoppitt and Grant2017), flying (Nicol & Pope Reference Nicol and Pope1993) and locomotor activities (Meehan et al. Reference Meehan, Garner and Mench2003), and increased vocalisations (Nicol & Pope Reference Nicol and Pope1993) and avoidance behaviour towards humans (Meehan et al. Reference Meehan, Garner and Mench2003). Being hand-reared was correlated with the development of feather-damaging behaviour (Schmid et al. Reference Schmid, Doherr and Steiger2006; Costa et al. Reference Costa, Macchi, Tomassone, Ricceri, Bollo, Scaglione, Tarantola, Md, Prola, Bergero and Schiavone2016), stereotypies (Williams et al. Reference Williams, Hoppitt and Grant2017) and preferential interactions with humans (Schmid et al. Reference Schmid, Doherr and Steiger2006) (Table S10). Living in a small cage was associated with the emergence of phobic behaviours (Schmid et al. Reference Schmid, Doherr and Steiger2006); abnormal behaviours such as incessant screaming, oral and locomotor stereotypies, increase of courtship behaviours and singing towards conspecifics (Polverino et al. Reference Polverino, Manciocco, Vitale and Alleva2015); and increase of locomotor activities and preening (Polverino et al. Reference Polverino, Manciocco, Vitale and Alleva2015; Phillips et al. Reference Phillips, Farrugia, Lin, Mancera and Doneley2018) (Table S10). Regarding the significant and feasible outcome measures collected from companion parrots, risk factors related to human-animal interactions (e.g. little time spent interacting each day, being hand-reared or wild caught, being acquired before the end of weaning or from a pet shop) were the most common investigated (Table S12; Supplementary material).

Representation of the various genera and living conditions

Living conditions represented in the studies

The majority of studies (n = 48) were conducted on parrots kept in laboratories, followed by 22 on companion parrots, ten on parrots kept in zoos, six on parrots kept in breeding facilities, three on parrots kept in rehabilitation centres and two on parrots kept in shelters (Table S6; Supplementary material). Two studies focused on comparing parrots kept in different settings (i.e. wild versus companion parrots vs parrots kept in zoos or breeding facilities, and companion vs parrots kept in laboratories; Table S6). For seven studies, it was not possible to define the living condition of the parrots (Table S6).

Outcome measures in relation to genera

Outcome measures were collected from 13 genera, of which ten belonged to the superfamily Psittaccoidea and three to the superfamily Cacatuoidea. Melopsittacus (budgerigars) and Amazona (Amazon parrots) were the genera with the highest number of significant and feasible outcomes (n = 150 and n = 128, respectively), followed by the Ara (macaws; n = 72), Nymphicus (cockatiels; n = 65), and Psittacus (grey parrots; n = 54) genera (Figure 3, Table S8; Supplementary material). Fifty-four (9.44%) significant and feasible outcome measures were available from studies that included multiple genera (see Table S7; Supplementary material). For other genera, the number of significant and feasible outcomes collected ranged between 1 (Guaruba; golden conures) and 17 (Pyrrhura; conures). For the genus Platycercus (rosellas) no significant and feasible outcome measures were identified (Figure 3, Table S8; Supplementary material). A total of 68 feasible and significant outcome measures were collected from companion parrots, with 32 (47%) outcomes related to multiple genera, 24 (35.4%) to the genus Psittacus, nine (13.2%) to the genus Cacatua (cockatoos), and three (4.4%) to the genus Agapornis (lovebirds) (Figure 3, Table S12; Supplementary material).

Figure 3. Number of welfare outcomes identified in the systematic literature search, grouped by parrot genera. For each genus, the overlapped bar plot shows, from darkest to lightest colour, the total number of welfare-related outcomes collected, the number of significant outcomes (P < 0.05), the number of significant outcomes that are considered feasible for evaluation by owners (i.e. not requiring any particular skill, expertise or equipment; next-to-smallest bar plot), and the number of significant and feasible outcomes that were obtained from companion parrots (smallest bar plot). The genus ‘Other’ refers to the pooled genera Calyptorhynchus (black cockatoos), Guaruba (golden conures) and Loriculus (hanging parrots).

Relationship between genera and welfare dimensions

Of the welfare dimensions for which we identified significant and feasible outcomes, ‘social behaviours’ was the one investigated in the highest number of genera (nine out of 13). All other welfare dimensions covered eight genera, except the dimensions ‘abnormal and fear-related behaviour’ and ‘locomotor behaviour’ which both were covered by six genera (Figure 4). The genera-welfare dimension association with the highest number of feasible and significant outcomes was the combination Melopsittacus – ‘abnormal and fear-related behaviours’ (n = 61), followed by Nymphicus – ‘social behaviours’ (n = 41), Amazona – ‘exploratory and foraging behaviours’ (n = 38), Melopsittacus – ‘maintenance behaviours’ (n = 35), and ‘multiple genera’ – ‘body measurements’ (n = 29) (Figure 4). Three genera were covered by only one welfare dimension: Agapornis and Guaruba with ‘body measurements’, and Loriculus with ‘exploratory and foraging behaviours’ (Figure 4).

Figure 4. Bubble plot showing the number of significant and feasible outcome measures related to parrot welfare identified during the systematic literature search (n = 572). The numbers inside the bubbles correspond to the number of outcome measures identified for each welfare dimension (x-axis) – parrot genus (y-axis), while size and colour of the bubbles reflect the relative proportion of outcomes covered by each combination; hence, larger and darker bubbles represent a combination for which a higher number of significant and feasible outcomes were reported.

Discussion

Internal validity

The main aim of this systematic study was to identify potential welfare indicators for companion parrots by first assessing the internal validity of outcome measures from published scientific studies. We found a high risk of bias associated with the outcome measures in the scientific literature. For instance, intra- and inter-observer reliabilities and assessor blindness were almost never reported. We identified 572 outcomes measures that presented a significant P-value and that we classified as feasible, but these need to be thoroughly validated before being used as welfare indicators in practical assessments (European Food Safety Authority [EFSA] 2012).

Welfare dimensions

The significant and feasible outcome measures linked to parrot welfare were well distributed across six of the eight welfare dimensions, except for physiological parameters, and disease and pathological conditions. Behaviours represented the most common type of outcomes measures, covering five out of eight welfare dimensions, and 24 out of 34 categories.

‘Social behaviour’ was the welfare dimension with the greatest variety of significant and feasible outcome measures and welfare categories. Within this dimension, we found several behaviours such as vocalisation, mate-related behaviours, aggressiveness, allopreening, or behaviours related to human-animal interactions. The latter presented a remarkable heterogeneity of outcome measures, including several outcomes linked to inappropriate handling and physical contact. Human-animal relationship plays a fundamental role in guaranteeing companion animals’ positive welfare (Rault et al. Reference Rault, Waiblinger, Boivin and Hemsworth2020); for this reason, these results represent a good starting point for the development of an assessment tool tailored to companion parrots. Outcome measures of the welfare category, ‘facial and body display’ were the only results retrieved that proposed the use of facial expressions (feather ruffling, blushing) as indicators of calmness and positive human-parrot interaction (Bertin et al. Reference Bertin, Beraud, Lansade, Blache, Diot, Mulot and Arnould2018, Reference Bertin, Beraud, Lansade, Mulot and Arnould2020, Reference Bertin, Mulot, Nowak, Blache, Love, Arnold, Pinateau, Arnould and Lansade2023) and the display of erected crests as a sign of high arousal (Lievin-Bazin et al. Reference Lievin-Bazin, Pineaux, Clerc, Gahr, von Bayern and Bovet2018). Many other parrots’ displays, such as body postures, have been interpreted as ways parrots communicate their level of arousal or signal an imminent aggressive response (Wilson Reference Wilson2022), however this information is not supported by experimental studies. Observing facial and body displays can be useful to assess welfare, for example, in preventing negative interactions with caretakers or other animals that live in the same environment; however, further investigations are needed to validate such indicators.

Three welfare dimensions concentrated almost half of the significant and feasible outcomes: ‘locomotor behaviours’, in which we grouped behaviours such as flying and climbing; ‘exploratory and foraging behaviours’, reflecting the way in which parrots explore new environments and novel objects and interact with enrichment; and ‘maintenance behaviours’, in which we clustered behaviours such as feeding and resting. All these behaviours are used while foraging, an important activity for wild parrots as it occupies between 40 and 70% of their daily active time (Magrath & Lill Reference Magrath and Lill1983; Westcott & Cockburn Reference Westcott and Cockburn1988). Therefore, indicators linked to foraging are suggested to be relevant to monitor captive parrot welfare.

For the welfare dimension ‘abnormal and fear-related behaviours’ we retrieved several outcomes that could be used to assess welfare: incessant screaming, phobic behaviours, and many types of stereotypies, such as locomotor (e.g. route trace, pacing), oral (e.g. wire chewing) and whole body (e.g. rocking, bobbing) stereotypies. Feather-damaging behaviour, a common abnormal behaviour in companion parrots with a wide range of underlying causes, including medical issues, socio-environmental factors and neurochemical changes (van Zeeland et al. Reference van Zeeland, Spruit, Rodenburg, Riedstra, van Hierden, Buitenhuis, Korte and Lumeij2009), affects between 11.7 and 25.4% of the overall parrot population, according to surveys conducted in different countries (Kinkaid et al. Reference Kinkaid, Mills, Nichols, Meagher and Mason2013; Costa et al. Reference Costa, Macchi, Tomassone, Ricceri, Bollo, Scaglione, Tarantola, Md, Prola, Bergero and Schiavone2016; Ebisawa et al. Reference Ebisawa, Nakayama, Pai, Kinoshita and Koie2021; Mellor et al. Reference Mellor, McDonald Kinkaid, Mendl, Cuthill, van Zeeland and Mason2021; Mahdavi et al. Reference Mahdavi, Abdi-Hachesoo, Ansari-Lari and Haddad-Marandi2023). Grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) and cockatoos are reported to be predisposed to develop this problem (Seibert Reference Seibert2006a; Kinkaid et al. Reference Kinkaid, Mills, Nichols, Meagher and Mason2013), with surveys indicating a prevalence between 22.5 and 39.4% in grey parrots (Jayson et al. Reference Jayson, Williams and Wood2014; Costa et al. Reference Costa, Macchi, Tomassone, Ricceri, Bollo, Scaglione, Tarantola, Md, Prola, Bergero and Schiavone2016; Ebisawa et al. Reference Ebisawa, Nakayama, Pai, Kinoshita and Koie2021; Mahdavi et al. Reference Mahdavi, Abdi-Hachesoo, Ansari-Lari and Haddad-Marandi2023) and between 30.6 and 42.4% in cockatoos (Kinkaid et al. Reference Kinkaid, Mills, Nichols, Meagher and Mason2013; Jayson et al. Reference Jayson, Williams and Wood2014; Ebisawa et al. Reference Ebisawa, Nakayama, Pai, Kinoshita and Koie2021). Nevertheless, other species, including lovebirds, pacific parrotlets (Forpus coelestis), red-shouldered macaws (Diopsittaca nobilis), conures (Aratinga spp, Pyrrhura spp), and eclectus parrots (Eclectus roratus) have also emerged as species that are seemingly prone to develop this behaviour (Kinkaid et al. Reference Kinkaid, Mills, Nichols, Meagher and Mason2013; Costa et al. Reference Costa, Macchi, Tomassone, Ricceri, Bollo, Scaglione, Tarantola, Md, Prola, Bergero and Schiavone2016; Ebisawa et al. Reference Ebisawa, Nakayama, Pai, Kinoshita and Koie2021). Due to its high prevalence and association with several medical problems (van Zeeland et al. Reference van Zeeland, Spruit, Rodenburg, Riedstra, van Hierden, Buitenhuis, Korte and Lumeij2009), feather-damaging behaviour has been the subject of many studies. However, feather-damaging behaviour is difficult to observe directly, which may explain why we retrieved only one study where authors recorded duration and frequency of this self-injuring behaviour (Seibert et al. Reference Seibert, Crowell-Davis, Wilson and Ritchie2004). Stereotypies and feather-damaging behaviour, however, do not always reflect the current welfare state of the subject as they may also manifest themselves as “behavioural scars” and can remain even after the stressful stimulus or situation that triggered them is no longer present (Mason Reference Mason1991). For this reason, they should be included in a parrot welfare assessment scheme but accompanied by the corresponding risk factors identified, such as being hand-reared or single-housed, lack of enrichment, or living in a small cage.

‘Body measurements’ was the only welfare dimension to include significant and feasible outcome measures that were not behaviours and consisted of two categories. One was ‘indirect measures of feather-damaging behaviours’ and included the outcomes ‘presence of feather damage (yes/no)’ and plumage scores. These two outcomes can be used to detect the presence of feather-damaging behaviour. Additionally, plumage scores allow caretakers to monitor improvement or deterioration of plumage condition over time. Moreover, previous studies showed good to excellent agreements within and between observers for this type of measurement (van Zeeland et al. Reference van Zeeland, Bergers, van der Valk, Schoemaker and Lumeij2013b; Mellor et al. Reference Mellor, Mendl, Mason, Davison, van Zeeland and Cuthill2023). It is important to highlight that damage to the plumage is not specific to behavioural disorders, as it can also be caused by other factors that are still relevant to parrot welfare such as malnutrition, virus infections, parasitic infestation or inappropriate husbandry or management (e.g. small or overcrowded cages) (van Zeeland & Schoemaker Reference van Zeeland and Schoemaker2014). Due to their high feasibility and their well-established link to the welfare of captive parrots, ‘indirect measures of feather-damaging behaviours’ can be considered the most promising welfare indicators among all types of outcomes collected. The second welfare category in the dimension ‘body measurements’ was ‘body condition’, which contained measures indirectly linked to body fat composition, such as body weight, chest girth, and body mass. Despite the lack of data on the prevalence of obesity in the companion parrot population, this condition is regarded as a common welfare problem in companion parrots, especially in budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus), cockatiels (Nymphicus hollandicus), Amazon parrots and galahs (Eolophus roseicapilla) (Speer et al. Reference Speer, Olsen, Doneley, Monks, Verstappen, Yarto-Jaramillo, Elliott, Weston, Rivero, McDonald and Speer2016; Chitty Reference Chitty2023); its emergence is believed to be linked to a combination of unbalanced diets, selective eating, and lack of exercise (Harrison et al. Reference Harrison, Lightfoot and Harrison2006; Chitty Reference Chitty2023). Several studies included in this systematic review tested the effect of an unbalanced diet on parrots’ body condition; however, most of the results were not significant. Caloric deficit and surplus are responsible for changes in body fat composition in most animal species and, arguably, this also likely applies to parrots. However, some studies found that body mass decreases in association with regular physical activity (Schnegg et al. Reference Schnegg, Gebhardt-Henrich, Keller, Visser and Steiger2007; Gustavsen et al. Reference Gustavsen, Stanhope, Lin, Graham, Havel and Paul-Murphy2016) and increases with a prolonged exposure to artificial light at night (Malek et al. Reference Malek, Haim and Izhaki2020). Changes in body composition and the factors influencing these changes appear to be understudied, yet they could be highly relevant to the welfare of companion parrots.

We could not retrieve any feasible outcomes for the welfare dimension ‘physiological parameters’, mostly due to these requiring specialised techniques or equipment for collection or analysis. However, we identified several risk factors that can be associated with changes in these physiological parameters. Advancements in new technologies or methodologies could enhance welfare assessments by enabling caregivers to collect non-invasive physiological measurements. However, until such advancements are achieved, and widely available, veterinarian input may be necessary to obtain a more complete picture of parrot welfare. Husbandry and management conditions were recurrent risk factors that influenced parrots’ physiology, especially stress-related parameters. For the welfare dimension ‘diseases and pathologic conditions’, which also lacked feasible outcome measures, risk factors mostly included demographic characteristics like parrot age, sex, or species. None of the studies retrieved looked for physical measurements as a clinical sign for existing disease or pathology. This finding was unexpected considering that these types of measurements are heavily influenced by health problems; for instance, upper beak and nail overgrowth, increased weight, and changes in feather colour and quality are anecdotally reported as signs indicative of (fatty) liver disease (Grunkemeyer Reference Grunkemeyer2010). Some of these parameters, commonly used by veterinarians based on expert knowledge or experience, were not reflected in the scientific literature or may have been missed in our search as it is difficult to comprehensively capture all possible health problems. Further experimental validation of some of these commonly used clinical diagnostic signs would be valuable.

Most relevant risk factors for companion parrots

Among all risk factors associated with poor welfare, four emerged as especially important for the welfare of companion parrots. As suggested by several authors, captive parrots need to be mentally stimulated with different types of enrichment in order to prevent boredom, frustration, and other conditions associated with poor welfare (Livingstone Reference Livingstone2018; Seibert Reference Seibert2020). In support of this, we found that a lack of physical and foraging enrichment was the most recurrent risk factor and was associated with changes in maintenance, locomotor, exploratory and social behaviours, and with expression of stereotypies and feather-damaging behaviour. A recent study on grey parrots, not included in our results, demonstrated that combining two different enrichment devices stimulated both the appetitive and consummatory phases of foraging behaviour, resulting in increased daily foraging time (Beekmans et al. Reference Beekmans, Vinke, Maijer, de Haan, Schoemaker, Rodenburg, Kooistra and van Zeeland2023); hence not just the provision but also the design of enrichment devices is important. Moreover, other forms of enrichment that have been less investigated (e.g. cognitive and auditory) warrant research.

Social deprivation and social isolation also appeared to be recurrent risk factors in our results and were associated with outcome measures belonging to seven out of the eight welfare dimensions. Parrots are highly social species (Seibert Reference Seibert2006b) but are often housed alone as companion animal, a living condition that we found being linked to poor welfare. A recent study showed that parrots living without other parrots were more likely to show problematic behaviours such as biting humans and stealing human food, and parrots left alone for more than 6 h daily tended to be more prone to show feather-damaging behaviour (Tygesen & Forkman Reference Tygesen and Forkman2023).

Personality also influences how parrots interact with their environment and cope with challenging situations. Several studies included in this systematic review showed that specific personality traits or coping styles were linked to the emergence of feather-damaging behaviour (van Zeeland et al. Reference van Zeeland, Van der Aa, Vinke, Lumeij and Schoemaker2013a), to the exhibition of attention bias (Cussen & Mench Reference Cussen and Mench2014), to the time spent feeding and interacting with the enrichment (Ramos et al. Reference Ramos, Azevedo, Jardim and Sant’Anna2021), and to fearful responses towards humans (Franzone et al. Reference Franzone, GdAP, de Lima Kascher, de Azevedo and Sant’Anna2022). Assessing personality may be an effective strategy for improving the welfare of captive animals (Wilson et al. Reference Wilson, Guenther, Øverli, Seltmann and Altschul2019) although the best methods of determining personality remain debated (Richter & Hintze Reference Richter and Hintze2019).

Rearing methods also emerged as a crucial risk (developmental) factor that may impact parrots’ quality of life and welfare. Neonatal handling of parent-reared chicks can result in reduced aggressiveness and fear-related and feather-damaging behaviours in later life (Collette et al. Reference Collette, Millam, Klasing and Wakenell2000; Fox & Millam Reference Fox and Millam2004), whereas hand-rearing has been linked to these problematic behaviours (Schmid et al. Reference Schmid, Doherr and Steiger2006; Costa et al. Reference Costa, Macchi, Tomassone, Ricceri, Bollo, Scaglione, Tarantola, Md, Prola, Bergero and Schiavone2016; Ebisawa et al. Reference Ebisawa, Kusuda, Nakayama, Pai, Kinoshita and Koie2022), and to issues related to sexual imprinting, resulting in social and sexual preference for humans and impaired social bonds with conspecifics (Fox Reference Fox2006). However, given that hand-rearing might induce irreversible changes, the results observed from the studies should be used with informative and preventive purpose, as these cannot be changed after weaning.

Overall, our data point to a lack of enrichment, social isolation, personality, and rearing method as important aspects for companion parrots that should be taken into account in parrot welfare assessment.

External validity

The second aim of this systematic review was to assess the external validity of the outcomes by establishing from which species data were obtained and in which settings the studied subjects lived. We found two important factors that potentially compromise external validity of the outcomes collected: the presence of a strong taxonomic bias, and an overrepresentation of results from studies of parrots kept in laboratories. The term ‘taxonomic bias’ refers to differences in our knowledge of certain species and the degree to which they are the subject of scientific investigation across a wide variety of biological fields (Troudet et al. Reference Troudet, Grandcolas, Blin, Vignes-Lebbe and Legendre2017). The fact that amazon parrots, budgerigars, and cockatiels received more research attention compared to other species such as cockatoos, monk parakeets (Myiopsitta monachus), or lovebirds clearly demonstrates a bias in the scientific literature. Several factors might have contributed to this discrepancy. For instance, parrots like budgerigars and cockatiels are easily found, possess lower economic value, are easy to handle and tend to have a short generation interval as they become sexually mature before one year (Kavanau Reference Kavanau1987). All these characteristics, typical of commonly used animal models, make these species good candidates to conduct scientific studies under laboratory conditions. Although lovebirds possess similar characteristics, only one study on this genus met our inclusion criteria. Amazon parrots do not possess any of these characteristics, yet were the most studied species, with the second highest number of significant and feasible outcome measures. This is explained not by the widespread study of these species, but rather by the large amount of information gathered from one laboratory at the University of California, Davis, which published several studies on this taxon. We retrieved scarce data for black cockatoos (Calyptorhynchus spp), golden conures (Guaruba guarouba), and hanging parrots (Loriculus spp), but this is not surprising as these genera are mostly kept in zoos and breeding facilities and are rarely used in laboratory settings or kept as companion parrots. Nonetheless, no studies could be found for certain species that are commonly kept in captivity, including ring-necked parrots (Psittacula spp), caiques (Pionites spp), galahs and eclectus.

Studies conducted on multiple genera included these taxa along with many others, however the information obtained from these studies should be evaluated carefully before being used for individual species assessments. Some taxonomic groups have in fact specific needs and show different sensitivities when exposed to similar environmental stimuli, making generalisation of findings to other genera sometimes difficult or irrelevant. It should be emphasised that data extrapolation should be performed with caution even within the same genus, as for example Amazona, Ara and Cacatua each contain several species adapted to different natural habitats and showing distinct behaviours within the same genus (Parr & Juniper Reference Parr and Juniper2010). As such, extrapolating findings to other species, even within the same genus, may be improper and counterproductive, emphasising the need for further research to bridge these knowledge gaps for understudied species.

Our results show that certain welfare dimensions and categories were investigated only in a limited number of genera. For instance, we identified outcomes such as abnormal, locomotor, exploratory, and foraging behaviours for Amazon parrots, macaws, and budgerigars, but these parameters were not described in lovebirds, cockatoos, and monk parakeets. It is important to underline that the absence of information related to some taxa in our findings was not necessarily due to a lack of scientific studies but rather to a lack of significant results. For instance, we found that the provision of physical enrichment was linked to changes of preening in Amazon parrots, macaws and conures (Van Hoek & King Reference Van Hoek and King1997; Cussen & Mench Reference Cussen and Mench2015; Reimer et al. Reference Reimer, Maia and Santos2016; Almeida et al. Reference Almeida, Palme and Moreira2018), but not in cockatiels (Carvalho et al. Reference Carvalho, Saad, Alvarenga, Ferreira, Assis, Pereira, Scalon, Silva and Zangeronimo2017; Stevens et al. Reference Stevens, Doneley, Cogny and CJC2021). In fact, none of the results obtained from this genus presented a significant P-value. This could mean that cockatiels, unlike other parrot species, do not show changes in preening behaviour in these situations or, alternatively, that experimental set-ups and methods applied in the studies were not able to detect such changes.

As mentioned above, more than half of the outcomes were found from studies conducted in laboratory settings. Laboratories are highly controlled environments where daily routines related to animal care and testing are highly standardised. Such settings theoretically ensure results are more reproducible, but only in the case that characteristics are similar to those from which original data were extrapolated. This condition, defined as “standardization fallacy”, can lead to a decrease of external validity (Würbel Reference Würbel2000). In our case, external validity is important as parrot welfare indicators should ideally be applicable across parrots of different species and existing in various living conditions. Although zoos, shelters, rehabilitation centres, and breeding facilities offer settings that are not as standardised as in laboratories, they still differ greatly from domestic environments, which could be a cause for concern when extrapolating these findings to companion parrots.

Companion parrots

We found limited information related to companion parrots in terms of the number and variety of outcomes measures. In fact, the number of feasible and significant outcomes retrieved from companion parrots was less than 5% of the total outcome measures collected, and 75% of those were related to feather-damaging behaviour. Moreover, half of these outcomes for companion parrots were obtained from only three genera: Psittacus, Cacatua and Agapornis, with Psittacus being the only genus that covered the entire breadth of welfare categories, and outcomes for the other two genera being restricted to those related to feather-damaging behaviour. While these three genera are among the most commonly kept companion parrot species, several other species, such as macaws, Amazon parrots, conures, caiques, parrotlets, budgerigars, and other parakeets are also popular as companions, for which no significant and feasible outcomes were identified. An additional problematic aspect is that almost all outcome measures for companion parrots were obtained through questionnaires. Prospective, case-control studies might be challenging to perform with companion parrots under experimental circumstances as parrots may be harder to recruit, and possibly be less adaptable to new environments and/or unfamiliar humans compared to dogs and cats, which could lead to altered behavioural responses. Questionnaires may represent the best way to study this specific cohort and gather large amounts of data while preventing potential discomfort in the studied parrots. However, questionnaires are also highly sensitive to bias (Choi & Pak Reference Choi and Pak2005), and therefore results obtained using this method should be applied cautiously and possibly require complementary experimental testing. Overall, these results emphasise the need for more research on companion parrots, especially on species and welfare dimensions that have been underrepresented in this cohort. Some welfare indicators from other companion animal species could also be relevant and applicable to parrots, but we decided to focus on the literature on parrots because it offers greater external validity, and considering the already large number of parrot taxa and species with heterogenous needs.

Study limitations

This systematic review presents some limitations. The first is a lack of terms used to create the search queries, especially those related to medical conditions. This would certainly have allowed to increase the final number of studies retrieved; however, due to the high number of diseases and pathologic conditions, we decided to select terms related to specific medical conditions. The second important limiting factor was the fact that data collection was carried out by only one person (AP), which might have led to missing information or to systematic errors that reduced the accuracy of the results presented. The third and final limitation is related to the low internal validity and the selection of significant outcomes. The P-value significance is a necessary condition to reflect the sensitivity of an outcome measure as a potential welfare indicator. However, when studies present high risk of bias, the P-value can be influenced by confounding factors, leading to false negative or false positive results (Ioannidis Reference Ioannidis2005). Further studies should include the effect size as a parameter to assess internal validity with higher confidence, but this was not possible in this study based on the heterogeneity of the outcomes collected and the information provided by authors (e.g. lack of reporting of effect size).

Animal welfare implications and conclusion

The purpose of this systematic review was to identify valid and feasible welfare indicators for companion parrots. Despite the large amount of information collected, we could only identify plumage condition as a previously validated and feasible parrot welfare indicator. In fact, the lack of information contained in the publications made it difficult to assess the internal validity of the outcome measures. Moreover, given the lack of experimental studies focused on parrot health, future research should aim to validate physical parameters commonly used by veterinarians as clinical indicators of disease, metabolic disorders, or nutritional deficiencies. We also noticed potentially low external validity due to taxonomic bias and an overrepresentation of studies on parrots kept in laboratories. These challenges to ascertain validity prevented us from establishing a definitive list of reliable and useful welfare indicators for companion parrots. Nevertheless, this systematic review helps to summarise the current state of scientific knowledge on aspects relevant to parrot welfare (a dataset containing all data collected from the studies is available as Supplementary material for further reference) and identifies a list of potential welfare indicators for use in parrots. Future directions should focus on validating the identified welfare indicators. This is essential to create a comprehensive and reliable welfare assessment that accurately reflects the overall well-being of companion parrots.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/awf.2024.61.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Marta Trogu and Lewis Urquhart for proof-reading the manuscript, Suzanne Truong for proof-reading and for offering valuable comments on figures and writing style, and Patrizia Piotti for providing advice and constructive feedback about assessment of internal validity.

Competing interest

None.

Footnotes

Author contributions: Conceptualisation: AP, J-LR; Data curation: AP; Formal analysis: AP; Supervision: YvZ, J-LR; Methodology: AP, YvZ, J-LR; Writing – original draft: AP; Writing – review & editing: AP, YvZ, J-LR

References

Aengus, WL and Millam, JR 1999 Taming parent-reared orange-winged Amazon parrots by neonatal handling. Zoo Biology 18: 177187. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2361(1999)18:3<177::AID-ZOO2>3.0.CO;2-D3.0.CO;2-D>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Almeida, AC, Palme, R and Moreira, N 2018 How environmental enrichment affects behavioral and glucocorticoid responses in captive blue-and-yellow macaws (Ara ararauna). Applied Animal Behaviour Science 201: 125135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2017.12.019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, P 2003 A bird in the house: An anthropological perspective on companion parrots. Society & Animals 11: 393418. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853003322796109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, PK 2014 Social dimensions of the human–avian bond: Parrots and their persons. Anthrozoös 27: 371387. https://doi.org/10.2752/175303714X13903827488006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auersperg, A, Köck, C, O’Hara, M and Huber, L 2018 Tool making cockatoos adjust the lengths but not the widths of their tools to function. PLoS One 13: e0205429. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0205429CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auersperg, AA, Von Bayern, AM, Gajdon, GK, Huber, L and Kacelnik, A 2011 Flexibility in problem solving and tool use of kea and New Caledonian crows in a multi access box paradigm. PLoS One 6: e20231. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0020231CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auersperg, AMI, Szabo, B, von Bayern, AMP and Kacelnik, A 2012 Spontaneous innovation in tool manufacture and use in a Goffin’s cockatoo. Current Biology 22: R903R904. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auersperg, AMI, von Bayern, AMI, Weber, S, Szabadvari, A, Bugnyar, T and Kacelnik, A 2014 Social transmission of tool use and tool manufacture in Goffin cockatoos (Cacatua goffini). Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 281: 20140972. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.0972CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aydinonat, D, Penn, DJ, Smith, S, Moodley, Y, Hoelzl, F, Knauer, F and Schwarzenberger, F 2014 Social isolation shortens telomeres in African Grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus erithacus). PLoS One 9: e93839. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093839CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bailoo, JD, Reichlin, TS and Würbel, H 2014 Refinement of experimental design and conduct in laboratory animal research. Ilar Journal 55: 383391. https://doi.org/10.1093/ilar/ilu037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaufrere, H, Nevarez, JG, Wakamatsu, N, Clubb, S, Cray, C and Tully, TN 2013 Experimental diet-induced atherosclerosis in quaker parrots (Myiopsitta monachus). Veterinary Pathology 50: 11161126. https://doi.org/10.1177/0300985813488958CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beekmans, MHC, Vinke, CM, Maijer, A, de Haan, I, Schoemaker, NJ, Rodenburg, TB, Kooistra, HS, and van Zeeland, YRA 2023 Increasing foraging times with appetitive and consummatory foraging enrichment in grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus). Applied Animal Behaviour Science 265: 105986. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2023.105986CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bellamy, N 2015 Principles of clinical outcome assessment In: Hochberg, MC, Silman, AJ, Smolen, JS, Weinblatt, ME and Weisman, MH (eds) Rheumatology, Sixth Edition pp 919. Mosby: Philadelphia, USA. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-323-09138-1.00002-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berg, ML, Knott, B, Ribot, RFH, Buchanan, KL and Bennett, ATD 2019 Do glucocorticoids or carotenoids mediate plumage coloration in parrots? An experiment in Platycercus elegans. General Comparative Endocrinology 280: 8290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ygcen.2019.04.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bertin, A, Beraud, A, Lansade, L, Blache, MC, Diot, A, Mulot, B and Arnould, C 2018 Facial display and blushing: Means of visual communication in blue-and-yellow macaws (Ara ararauna)? PLoS One 13: e0201762. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201762CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bertin, A, Beraud, A, Lansade, L, Mulot, B and Arnould, C 2020 Bill covering and nape feather ruffling as indicators of calm states in the sulphur-crested cockatoo (Cacatua galerita). Behavioural Processes 178: 104188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2020.104188CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bertin, A, Mulot, B, Nowak, R, Blache, M-C, Love, S, Arnold, M, Pinateau, A, Arnould, C and Lansade, L 2023 Captive blue-and-yellow macaws (Ara ararauna) show facial indicators of positive affect when reunited with their caregiver. Behavioural Processes 206: 104833. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2023.104833CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boehrer, BT 2010 Parrot Culture: Our 2500-Year-Long Fascination with the World’s Most Talkative Bird. University of Pennsylvania Press: USA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boutron, I, Page, MJ, Higgins, JP, Altman, DG, Lundh, A, Hróbjartsson, A and on behalf of the Cochrane Bias Methods Group 2019 Considering bias and conflicts of interest among the included studies Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions pp 177204. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119536604.ch7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bright-Smith, DJ 1999 Parrots: A guide to parrots of the world. The Auk 116: 868870. https://doi.org/10.2307/4089356Google Scholar
Brucks, D and von Bayern, AMP 2020 Parrots voluntarily help each other to obtain food rewards. Current Biology 30: 292297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2019.11.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carvalho, TSG, Saad, CEP, Alvarenga, RR, Ferreira, WM, Assis, VDL, Pereira, VM, Scalon, JD, Silva, JP and Zangeronimo, MG 2017 Use of collard green stalks as environmental enrichment for cockatiels (Nymphicus hollandicus) kept in captivity. Arquivo Brasileiro de Medicina Veterinária e Zootecnia 69: 718724. https://doi.org/10.1590/1678-4162-8988CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chitty, J 2023 Approach to managing obesity in parrots. In Practice 45: 461474. https://doi.org/10.1002/inpr.360CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, BC and Pak, AW 2005 A catalog of biases in questionnaires. Preventing Chronic Disease 2: A13.Google ScholarPubMed
Collette, JC, Millam, JR, Klasing, KC and Wakenell, PS 2000 Neonatal handling of Amazon parrots alters the stress response and immune function. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 66: 335349. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00098-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, P, Macchi, E, Tomassone, L, Ricceri, F, Bollo, E, Scaglione, FE, Tarantola, M, Md, Marco, Prola, L, Bergero, D and Schiavone, A 2016 Feather picking in pet parrots: sensitive species, risk factor and ethological evidence. Italian Journal of Animal Science 15: 473480. https://doi.org/10.1080/1828051X.2016.1195711CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cronbach, LJ and Meehl, PE 1955 Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychological Bulletin 52: 281302. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0040957CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cussen, VA and Mench, JA 2014 Personality predicts cognitive bias in captive psittacines, Amazona amazonica. Animal Behaviour 89: 123130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.12.022CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cussen, VA and Mench, JA 2015 The relationship between personality dimensions and resiliency to environmental stress in orange-winged Amazon parrots (Amazona amazonica), as indicated by the development of abnormal behaviors. PLoS One 10: e0126170. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0126170CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Di Santo, LG, Braos, LB, Kawanami, AE, Oliveira, JP, Cruz, NRN, Mendonça, FS, Peixoto, MC and Carciofi, AC 2019 Feed processing effects on digestibility, palatability, excreta fermentation products and blood parameters in blue-fronted amazon parrots (Amazona aestiva). Journal of Animal Physiology and Animal Nutrition (Berlin) 103: 339353. https://doi.org/10.1111/jpn.13011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dos Santos, GJ, Aleixo, ASC, Hippólito, AG, Ferro, BS, Okamoto, P, Lourenço, MLG, de Vasconcelos Machado, VM, Ramos, PRR, Rahal, SC, Teixeira, CR and Melchert, A 2022 Are echocardiographic parameters affected by body condition scores in blue-fronted Amazon parrots (Amazona aestiva, Linnaeus, 1758)? Veterinary Research Communication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11259-022-09894-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ebisawa, K, Kusuda, S, Nakayama, S, Pai, C, Kinoshita, R and Koie, H 2022 Effects of rearing methods on feather-damaging behavior and corticosterone metabolite excretion in the peach-faced lovebird (Agapornis roseicollis Vieillot). Journal of Veterinary Behavior 54: 2835. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2022.07.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ebisawa, K, Nakayama, S, Pai, C, Kinoshita, R and Koie, H 2021 Prevalence and risk factors for feather-damaging behavior in psittacine birds: Analysis of a Japanese nationwide survey. PLoS One 16: e0254610. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254610CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
European Food Safety Authority [EFSA] 2012 Statement on the use of animal-based measures to assess the welfare of animals. EFSA Journal 10: 2767. https://doi.org/10.2903/j.efsa.2012.2767Google Scholar
Eggleston, R, Viloria, N, Delgado, S, Mata, A, Guerrero, HY, Kline, RJ, Beissinger, SR and Berg, KS 2022 Vocal babbling in a wild parrot shows life history and endocrine affinities with human infants. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 289: 20220592. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2022.0592CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emery, NJ 2006 Cognitive ornithology: the evolution of avian intelligence. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences 361: 2343. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2005.1736CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engebretson, M 2006 The welfare and suitability of parrots as companion animals: a review. Animal Welfare 15: 263276. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600030475CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farnworth, MJ 2018 Pets. In: Vonk, J and Shackelford, T (eds) Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior pp 113. Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland. https://10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_367-1Google Scholar
Fox, R 2006 Hand-Rearing: Behavioral Impacts and Implications for Captive Parrot Welfare; Manual of Parrot Behavior pp 8391. Wiley: London, UK. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470344651.ch10Google Scholar
Fox, RA and Millam, JR 2004 The effect of early environment on neophobia in orange-winged Amazon parrots (Amazona amazonica). Applied Animal Behaviour Science 89: 117129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2004.05.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franzone, V, GdAP, Ramos, de Lima Kascher, LK, de Azevedo, CS and Sant’Anna, AC 2022 Flight capacity and human aversion in captive Amazon parrots: Related factors and the effects of pre-releasing training. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 256: 105772. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2022.105772CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraser, D 2008 Understanding animal welfare. Acta Veterinaria Scandinavica 50: S1. https://doi.org/10.1186/1751-0147-50-S1-S1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gill, F, Donsker, D and Rasmussen, P 2022 IOC World Bird List (v11.1). https://doi.org/10.14344/IOC.ML.11.1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gravetter, FJ and Forzano, LAB 2012 Research Methods for the Behavioral Sciences. Wadsworth: London, UK.Google Scholar
Grunkemeyer, VL 2010 Advanced diagnostic approaches and current management of Avian Hepatic Disorders. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Exotic Animal Practice 13: 413427. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cvex.2010.05.005Google ScholarPubMed
Gustavsen, KA, Stanhope, KL, Lin, AS, Graham, JL, Havel, PJ and Paul-Murphy, JR 2016 Effects of exercise on the plasma lipid profile in Hispaniolan Amazon parrots (Amazona ventralis) with naturally occurring hypercholesterolemia. Journal of Zoo Wildlife Medicine 47: 760769. https://doi.org/10.1638/2015-0192.1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, GJ, Lightfoot, TL, and Harrison, LR 2006 Clinical Avian Medicine. Spix Publishing: Palm Beach, FL,USA.Google Scholar
Hasegawa, A, Okanoya, K, Hasegawa, T and Seki, Y 2011 Rhythmic synchronization tapping to an audio–visual metronome in budgerigars. Scientific Reports 1: 120. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep00120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, SP and Broom, DM 2009 Measuring zoo animal welfare: theory and practice. Zoo Biology 28: 531544. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.20276CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hooijmans, CR, Rovers, MM, de Vries, RBM, Leenaars, M, Ritskes-Hoitinga, M and Langendam, MW 2014 SYRCLE’s risk of bias tool for animal studies. BMC Medical Research Methodology 14: 43. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2288-14-43CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ioannidis, JPA 2005 Why most published research findings are false. PLOS Medicine 2: e124. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
IUCN 2024 The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. Version 2022-2. https://www.iucnredlist.org. (Accessed 24 January 2024).Google Scholar
Jayson, SL, Williams, DL and Wood, JLN 2014 Prevalence and risk factors of feather plucking in African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus erithacus and Psittacus erithacus timneh) and cockatoos (Cacatua spp). Journal of Exotic Pet Medicine 23: 250257. https://doi.org/10.1053/j.jepm.2014.06.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kalmar, I, Janssens, G and Moons, C 2010 Guidelines and ethical considerations for housing and management of psittacine birds used in research. ILAR Journal/National Research Council, Institute of Laboratory Animal Resources 51: 409423. https://doi.org/10.1093/ilar.51.4.409CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kavanau, JL 1987 Behavior and evolution: lovebirds, cockatiels, budgerigars. Science Software Systems: Los Angeles, CA, USA.Google Scholar
Kidd, AH and Kidd, RM 1998 Problems and benefits of bird ownership. Psychological Reports 83: 131138. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1998.83.1.131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinkaid, HYM, Mills, DS, Nichols, SG, Meagher, RK and Mason, GJ 2013 Feather-damaging behaviour in companion parrots: An initial analysis of potential demographic risk factors. Avian Biology Research 6: 289296. https://doi.org/10.3184/175815513X13803574144572CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klump, BC, Martin, JM, Wild, S, Hörsch, JK, Major, RE and Aplin, LM 2021 Innovation and geographic spread of a complex foraging culture in an urban parrot. Science 373: 456460. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.abe7808CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koski, MA 2002 Dermatologic diseases in psittacine birds: An investigational approach. Seminars in Avian and Exotic Pet Medicine 11: 105124. https://doi.org/10.1053/saep.2002.123981CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krasheninnikova, A, Brucks, D, Blanc, S, and von Bayern, AMP 2019 Assessing African grey parrots’ prosocial tendencies in a token choice paradigm. Royal Society Open Science 6: 190696. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.190696CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krasheninnikova, A, Höner, F, O’Neill, L, Penna, E and von Bayern, AMP 2018 Economic decision-making in parrots. Scientific Reports 8: 12537. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-30933-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lambert, ML, Seed, AM and Slocombe, KE 2015 A novel form of spontaneous tool use displayed by several captive greater vasa parrots (Coracopsis vasa). Biology Letters 11: 20150861. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2015.0861CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laumer, IB, Bugnyar, T and Auersperg, AMI 2016 Flexible decision-making relative to reward quality and tool functionality in Goffin cockatoos (Cacatua goffiniana). Scientific Reports 6: 28380. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep28380CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laumer, IB, Massen, JJM, Boehm, PM, Boehm, A, Geisler, A and Auersperg, AMI 2021 Individual Goffin´s cockatoos (Cacatua goffiniana) show flexible targeted helping in a tool transfer task. PLoS One 16: e0253416. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253416CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawshe, CH 1975 A quantitative approach to content validity. Personnel Psychology 28: 563575. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.1975.tb01393.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehner, PN 1998 Handbook of Ethological Methods. Cambridge University Press Book: Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
Lievin-Bazin, A, Pineaux, M, Clerc, O, Gahr, M, von Bayern, AMP and Bovet, D 2018 Emotional responses to conspecific distress calls are modulated by affiliation in cockatiels (Nymphicus hollandicus). PLoS One 13. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0205314CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingstone, M 2018 Foraging toys and environmental enrichment for parrots. Companion Animal 23: 462469. https://doi.org/10.12968/coan.2018.23.8.462CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magrath, R and Lill, A 1983 The use of time and energy by the crimson rosella in a temperate wet forest in winter. Australian Journal of Zoology 31: 903912. https://doi.org/10.1071/ZO9830903CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahdavi, I, Abdi-Hachesoo, B, Ansari-Lari, M and Haddad-Marandi, MR 2023 Prevalence and risk factors of feather damaging behavior in companion parrots: A cross-sectional study in Iran. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 266: 106028. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2023.106028CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malek, I, Haim, A and Izhaki, I 2020 Melatonin mends adverse temporal effects of bright light at night partially independent of its effect on stress responses in captive birds. Chronobiology International 37: 189208. https://doi.org/10.1080/07420528.2019.1698590CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, P and Bateson, P 2007 Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511810893CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, GJ 1991 Stereotypies and suffering. Behavioural Processes 25: 103115. https://doi.org/10.1016/0376-6357(91)90013-PCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meehan, CL, Garner, JP and Mench, JA 2003 Isosexual pair housing improves the welfare of young Amazon parrots. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 81: 7388. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00238-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellor, EL, McDonald Kinkaid, HK, Mendl, MT, Cuthill, IC, van Zeeland, YRA and Mason, GJ 2021 Nature calls: intelligence and natural foraging style predict poor welfare in captive parrots. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 288: 20211952. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2021.1952CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mellor, EL, Mendl, M, Mason, G, Davison, C, van Zeeland, Y and Cuthill, IC 2023 Validating owner-reporting of feather condition of pet Psittaciformes using photographs. Animal Welfare 31: 163173. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.31.2.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyers, N 1998 Perspectives on pet bird welfare from the pet industry. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association (USA). https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9569161/Google Scholar
Mota-Rojas, D, Mariti, C, Zdeinert, A, Riggio, G, Mora-Medina, P, del Mar Reyes, A, Gazzano, A, Domínguez-Oliva, A, Lezama-García, K, José-Pérez, N and Hernández-Ávalos, I 2021 Anthropomorphism and its adverse effects on the distress and welfare of companion animals. Animals 11: 3263. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11113263CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicol, CJ and Pope, SJ 1993 A comparison of the behaviour of solitary and group-housed budgerigars. Animal Welfare 2: 269277. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600015918CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nightengale, M, Stout, RW and Tully, TN 2022 Plasma vitamin D (25-Hydroxyvitamin D) levels in Hispaniolan Amazon parrots (Amazona ventralis) housed indoors over time. Avian Diseases. https://doi.org/10.1637/aviandiseases-D-21-00117CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishikawa-Pacher, A 2022 Research Questions with PICO: A Universal Mnemonic. Publications 10: 21. https://doi.org/10.3390/publications10030021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olah, G, Butchart, SHM, Symes, A, Guzmán, IM, Cunningham, R, Brightsmith, DJ and Heinsohn, R 2016 Ecological and socio-economic factors affecting extinction risk in parrots. Biodiversity and Conservation 25: 205223. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10531-015-1036-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olkowicz, S, Kocourek, M, Lučan, RK, Porteš, M, Fitch, WT, Herculano-Houzel, S and Němec, P 2016 Birds have primate-like numbers of neurons in the forebrain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113: 72557260. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1517131113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Page MJ, McKenzie, JE, Bossuyt, PM, Boutron, I, Hoffmann, TC, Mulrow, CD, Shamseer, L, Tetzlaff, JM, Akl, EA, Brennan, SE, Chou, R, Glanville, J, Grimshaw, JM, Hróbjartsson, A, Lalu, MM, Li, T, Loder, EW, Mayo-Wilson, E, McDonald, S, McGuinness, LA, Stewart, LA, Thomas, J, Tricco, AC, Welch, VA, Whiting, P and Moher, D 2021 The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. PLoS Medicine 18: e1003583. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1003583CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parr, M and Juniper, T 2010 Parrots: A Guide to Parrots of the World. Bloomsbury Publishing: London, UK.Google Scholar
Patel, AD, Iversen, JR, Bregman, MR and Schulz, I 2009 Experimental evidence for synchronization to a musical beat in a non-human animal. Current Biology 19: 827830. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.038CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, IM 2009 The Alex Studies: Cognitive and Communicative Abilities of Grey Parrots. Harvard University Press Book: USA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, IM, and Funk, MS 1990 Object permanence in four species of psittacine birds: An African grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus), an Illiger mini macaw (Ara maracana), a parakeet (Melopsittacus undulatus), and a cockatiel (Nymphicus hollandicus). Animal Learning & Behavior 18: 97108. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03205244CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Percie du Sert, N, Ahluwalia, A, Alam, S, Avey, MT, Baker, M, Browne, WJ, Clark, A, Cuthill, IC, Dirnagl, U, Emerson, M, Garner, P, Holgate, ST, Howells, DW, Hurst, V, Karp, NA, Lazic, SE, Lidster, K, MacCallum, CJ, Macleod, M, Pearl, EJ, Petersen, OH, Rawle, F, Reynolds, P, Rooney, K, Sena, ES, Silberberg, SD, Steckler, T and Würbel, H 2020 Reporting animal research: Explanation and elaboration for the ARRIVE guidelines 2.0. PLoS Biology 18: e3000411. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000411CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Péron, F, Rat-Fischer, L, Lalot, M, Nagle, L and Bovet, D 2011 Cooperative problem solving in African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus). Animal Cognition 14: 545553. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-011-0389-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, CJC, Farrugia, C, Lin, C, Mancera, K and Doneley, B 2018 The effect providing space in excess of standards on the behaviour of budgerigars in aviaries. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 199: 8993. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2017.10.015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polverino, G, Manciocco, A, Vitale, A and Alleva, E 2015 Stereotypic behaviours in Melopsittacus undulatus: behavioural consequences of social and spatial limitations. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 165: 143155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2015.02.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramos, GAP, Azevedo, CS, Jardim, THA and Sant’Anna, AC 2021 Temperament in captivity, environmental enrichment, flight ability, and response to humans in an endangered parrot species. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 24: 379391. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2020.1765367CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rault, J-L, Waiblinger, S, Boivin, X and Hemsworth, P 2020 The power of a positive human–animal relationship for animal welfare. Frontiers in Veterinary Science 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2020.590867CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reimer, J, Maia, CM and Santos, EF 2016 Environmental enrichments for a group of captive macaws: Low interaction does not mean low behavioral changes. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 19: 385395. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2016.1175944CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ribeiro, J, Reino, L, Schindler, S, Strubbe, D, Vall-llosera, M, Araújo, MB, Capinha, C, Carrete, M, Mazzoni, S, Monteiro, M, Moreira, F, Rocha, R, Tella, JL, Vaz, AS, Vicente, J and Nuno, A 2019 Trends in legal and illegal trade of wild birds: a global assessment based on expert knowledge. Biodiversity and Conservation 28: 33433369. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10531-019-01825-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richter, SH and Hintze, S 2019 From the individual to the population – and back again? Emphasising the role of the individual in animal welfare science. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 212: 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.12.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rössler, T and Auersperg, AM 2022 Recent developments in parrot cognition: a quadrennial update. Animal Cognition. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-022-01733-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schachner, A, Brady, TF, Pepperberg, IM and Hauser, MD 2009 Spontaneous motor entrainment to music in multiple vocal mimicking species. Current Biology 19: 831836. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.061CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmid, R, Doherr, MG and Steiger, A 2006 The influence of the breeding method on the behaviour of adult African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus). Applied Animal Behaviour Science 98: 293307. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2005.09.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnegg, A, Gebhardt-Henrich, SG, Keller, P, Visser, GH and Steiger, A 2007 Feeding behaviour and daily energy expenditure of domesticated budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus): Influence of type of housing and vertical position of the feeder. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 108: 302312. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2007.01.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwing, R, Jocteur, E, Wein, A, Noë, R and Massen, JJ 2016 Kea cooperate better with sharing affiliates. Animal Cognition 19: 10931102. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-016-1017-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwing, R, Meaux, E, Piseddu, A, Huber, L and Noë, R 2021 Kea, Nestor notabilis, achieve cooperation in dyads, triads, and tetrads when dominants show restraint. Learning Behaviour 49: 3653. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-021-00462-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seibert, L 2020 Mental Health Issues in Captive Birds. CABI International: Wallingford, UK. https://doi.org/10.1079/9781786393401.0291CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seibert, LM 2006a Feather-Picking Disorder in Pet Birds; Manual of Parrot Behavior pp 255265. Wiley Blackwell: UK. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470344651.ch23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seibert, LM 2006b Social Behavior of Psittacine Birds; Manual of Parrot Behavior pp 4348. Wiley Blackwell: UK. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470344651.ch5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seibert, LM, Crowell-Davis, SL, Wilson, GH, and Ritchie, BW 2004 Placebo-controlled clomipramine trial for the treatment of feather picking disorder in cockatoos. Journal of the American Animal Hospital Association 40: 261269. https://doi.org/10.5326/0400261CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Speer, BL, Olsen, GP, Doneley, R, Monks, D, Verstappen, F, Yarto-Jaramillo, E, Elliott, D, Weston, M, Rivero, J and McDonald, A 2016 Common conditions of commonly held companion birds in multiple parts of the world, In: Speer, BL (ed) Current Therapy in Avian Medicine and Surgery pp 777794. WB. Saunders: London, UK. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-1-4557-4671-2.00034-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spierings, MJ and ten Cate, C 2016 Budgerigars and zebra finches differ in how they generalize in an artificial grammar learning experiment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113: E3977E3984. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1600483113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, A, Doneley, R, Cogny, A and CJC, Phillips 2021 The effects of environmental enrichment on the behaviour of cockatiels (Nymphicus hollandicus) in aviaries. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2020.105154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team 2022 R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria.Google Scholar
The EndNoteTeam 2013 EndNote, EndNote X7 Edition. Clarivate: Philadelphia, PA, USA.Google Scholar
Tebbich, S, Taborsky, M and Winkler, H 1996 Social manipulation causes cooperation in keas. Animal Behaviour 52: 110. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1996.0147CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torres Ortiz, S, Smeele, SQ, Champenois, J and von Bayern, AMP 2022 Memory for own actions in parrots. Scientific Reports 12: 20561. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-25199-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Troudet, J, Grandcolas, P, Blin, A, Vignes-Lebbe, R, and Legendre, F 2017 Taxonomic bias in biodiversity data and societal preferences. Scientific Reports 7: 9132. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-09084-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuyttens, FAM, de Graaf, S, Heerkens, JLT, Jacobs, L, Nalon, E, Ott, S, Stadig, L, Van Laer, E and Ampe, B 2014 Observer bias in animal behaviour research: can we believe what we score, if we score what we believe? Animal Behaviour 90: 273280. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.02.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tygesen, A and Forkman, B 2023 The parrot–owner relationship and problem behaviors in parrots. Anthrozoös: 113. https://doi.org/10.1080/08927936.2023.2238434CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Hoek, CS and King, CE 1997 Causation and influence of environmental enrichment on feather picking of the crimson-bellied conure (Pyrrhura perlata perlata). Zoo Biology 16: 161172. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2361(1997)16:2<161::AID-ZOO6>3.0.CO;2-83.0.CO;2-8>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Zeeland, YRA, Van der Aa, MMJA, Vinke, CM, Lumeij, JT and Schoemaker, NJ 2013a Behavioural testing to determine differences between coping styles in Grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus erithacus) with and without feather damaging behaviour. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 148: 218231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2013.08.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Zeeland, YRA, Bergers, MJ, van der Valk, L, Schoemaker, NJ and Lumeij, JT 2013b Evaluation of a novel feather scoring system for monitoring feather damaging behaviour in parrots. Veterinary Journal 196: 247252. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2012.08.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Zeeland, YRA and Schoemaker, NJ 2014 Plumage disorders in psittacine birds-part 1: feather abnormalities. European Journal of Companion Animal Practice 24: 3447.Google Scholar
van Zeeland, YRA, Spruit, BM, Rodenburg, TB, Riedstra, B, van Hierden, YM, Buitenhuis, B, Korte, SM and Lumeij, JT 2009 Feather damaging behaviour in parrots: A review with consideration of comparative aspects. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 121: 7595. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2009.09.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickham, H, François, R, Henry, L, Müller, K, Vaughan, D 2023 dplyr: A Grammar of Data Manipulation. R package version 1.1.4. https://github.com/tidyverse/dplyr, https://dplyr.tidyverse.orgGoogle Scholar
Vernes, SC, Kriengwatana, BP, Beeck, VC, Fischer, J, Tyack, PL, ten Cate, C and Janik, VM 2021 The multi-dimensional nature of vocal learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 376: 20200236. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0236CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viswanathan, M, Ansari, MT, Berkman, ND, Chang, S, Hartling, L, McPheeters, M, Santaguida, PL, Shamliyan, T, Singh, K, Tsertsvadze, A and Treadwell, JR 2008 Assessing the Risk of Bias of Individual Studies in Systematic Reviews of Health Care Interventions; Methods Guide for Effectiveness and Comparative Effectiveness Reviews pp. Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (US): Rockville, MD, USA. MISSING PAGE NUMBERSGoogle Scholar
West, JA, Tully, TN, Nevarez, JG and Stout, RW 2019 Effects of fluorescent lighting versus sunlight exposure on calcium, magnesium, vitamin D, and feather destructive behavior in Hispaniolan Amazon parrots (Amazona ventralis). Journal of Avian Medicine and Surgery 33: 235244. https://doi.org/10.1647/2018-378CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westcott, D and Cockburn, A 1988 Flock size and vigilance in parrots. Australian Journal of Zoology 36: 335349. https://doi.org/10.1071/ZO9880335CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickham, H 2016 ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. Springer-Verlag: New York, NY, USA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, I, Hoppitt, W and Grant, R 2017 The effect of auditory enrichment, rearing method and social environment on the behavior of zoo-housed psittacines (Aves: Psittaciformes); implications for welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 186: 8592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.10.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, C 2022 Companion Animal Behaviour Problems: Prevention and Management of Behaviour Problems in Veterinary Practice; What Every Parrot Owner Should Know pp 136153. CABI International: Wallingford, UK. https://doi.org/10.1079/9781800621312.0011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, V, Guenther, A, Øverli, Ø, Seltmann, MW and Altschul, D 2019 Future directions for personality research: Contributing new insights to the understanding of animal behavior. Animals 9: 240. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9050240CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Würbel, H 2000 Behaviour and the standardization fallacy. Nature Genetics 26: 263263. https://doi.org/10.1038/81541CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yon, L, Williams, E, Harvey, ND and Asher, L 2019 Development of a behavioural welfare assessment tool for routine use with captive elephants. PLoS One 14: e0210783. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210783CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. PRISMA flowchart identifying the number of studies reporting on parrot welfare parameters retrieved during the literature search from each database (PubMed, Web of Science, CAB Direct) and via other methods, the number of studies subjected to a first screening based on title and abstract reading and a second based on eligibility criteria, studies excluded during both screening phases, and the number of studies included in the final review.

Figure 1

Table 1. Assessment of the risk of bias for the outcome measures (n = 1,512) related to parrot welfare identified in the systematic literature search by using five validity parameters. The percentages refer to outcome measures for which the validity parameters, as indicated in the table, were reported by the authors (‘Yes’), were not considered by the author (‘No’), were not executable (‘Not possible’), or data regarding the validity parameter were not reported in the main text (‘Not specified’)

Figure 2

Figure 2. Number of outcomes related to parrot welfare as identified in the systematic literature search, grouped by welfare dimensions according to the biological construct that they represent. For each welfare dimension, the overlapped bar plot indicates, from darkest to lightest colour, the total number of outcome parameters collected, the number of significant outcomes (i.e. P-value < 0.05), the number of significant outcomes that are considered feasible for owners to assess (i.e. not requiring specific skills, expertise or equipment), and the number of significant and feasible outcomes collected from companion parrots (smallest bar).

Figure 3

Figure 3. Number of welfare outcomes identified in the systematic literature search, grouped by parrot genera. For each genus, the overlapped bar plot shows, from darkest to lightest colour, the total number of welfare-related outcomes collected, the number of significant outcomes (P < 0.05), the number of significant outcomes that are considered feasible for evaluation by owners (i.e. not requiring any particular skill, expertise or equipment; next-to-smallest bar plot), and the number of significant and feasible outcomes that were obtained from companion parrots (smallest bar plot). The genus ‘Other’ refers to the pooled genera Calyptorhynchus (black cockatoos), Guaruba (golden conures) and Loriculus (hanging parrots).

Figure 4

Figure 4. Bubble plot showing the number of significant and feasible outcome measures related to parrot welfare identified during the systematic literature search (n = 572). The numbers inside the bubbles correspond to the number of outcome measures identified for each welfare dimension (x-axis) – parrot genus (y-axis), while size and colour of the bubbles reflect the relative proportion of outcomes covered by each combination; hence, larger and darker bubbles represent a combination for which a higher number of significant and feasible outcomes were reported.

Supplementary material: File

Piseddu et al. supplementary material 1

Piseddu et al. supplementary material
Download Piseddu et al. supplementary material 1(File)
File 264.4 KB
Supplementary material: File

Piseddu et al. supplementary material 2

Piseddu et al. supplementary material
Download Piseddu et al. supplementary material 2(File)
File 750.9 KB