Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T22:19:19.276Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Development of morphophonemic segments in children’s mental representations of words

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Noel K. Jones*
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina, Wilmington
*
Department of Curricular Studies, University of North Carolina at Wilmington, 601 South College Road, Wilmington, NC 28403-3297

Abstract

This study explores the development in children of dual-level phonological processing. Evidence suggesting that 6-year-olds form underlying representations composed of morphophonemic segments was obtained by asking children to imitate complex words, omit specified portions, and discuss the meaning of the resulting word parts. Trial items represent a variety of instances in which phonetic forms differ from underlying representations. Although language-advanced first graders produced stronger evidence suggesting morphophonemic segments than language-delayed age-mates, and young adults supplied stronger evidence than either first-grade group; strength of evidence leads to the interpretation that even language-delayed 6-year-olds form morphophonemic segments. Differences in performance between groups probably derive from differences in metalinguistic abilities and linguistic experience rather than from differences in units of phonological processing.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1991

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Berko, J. (1958). The child’s learning of English morphology. Word, 14, 150177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read – A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419421.Google Scholar
Bruce, D. J. (1964). The analysis of word sounds by young children. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 34, 158170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J., & Slobin, D. I. (1982). Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past. Language, 58, 265289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. (1987). The use of morphological knowledge in spelling derived forms by learning-disabled and normal students. Annals of Dyslexia, 37, 90108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlisle, J. (1988). Knowledge of derivational morphology and spelling ability in fourth, sixth, and eighth graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 247266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, C. (1970). Reading, writing and phonology. Harvard Educational Review, 40, 287309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1970). Phonology and reading. In Levin, H. & Williams, J. P. (Eds.), Basic studies in reading. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Derwing, B., & Baker, W. (1979). Recent research on the acquisition of English morphology. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (Eds.), Language acquisition (1st ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Derwing, B., & Baker, W. (1986). Assessing morphological development. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (Eds.), Language acquisition (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ehri, L. (1989). Movement into word reading and spelling: How spelling contributes to reading. In Mason, J. (Ed.), Reading and writing connections. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Ehri, L., & Wilce, L. (1980). The influence of orthography on readers: Conceptualizations of the phonemic structure of words. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, 371385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, C. A., & Farwell, C. B. (1975). Words and sounds in early language acquisition. Language, 51, 419439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, C. A., & Ganica, O. K. (1975). Theories of phonological development. In Lenneberg, E. H. & Lenneberg, E. (Eds.), Foundations of language development: A multidisciplinary approach (Vol. 1). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Fox, B., & Routh, D. (1975). Analyzing spoken language into words, syllables, and phonemes: A developmental study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4, 331342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, B., & Routh, D. (1976). Phonemic analysis and synthesis as word-attack skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 7074.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, B., & Routh, D. (1984). Phonemic analysis and synthesis as word attack skills: Revisited. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 10591064.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fromkin, V. (1971). The non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances. Language, 47, 2752.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halle, M. (1962). Phonology in a generative grammar. Word, 18, 5472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, A., & Jacobson, M. (1972). Basic elementary reading vocabularies. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Hyman, L. M. (1975). Phonology: Theory and analysis. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Jones, N. K. (1979). Development of linguistic awareness of phonemic segments and the acquisition of reading in first grade and kindergarten children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, P. (1968). Linguistic universals and linguistic change. In Bach, E. & Harms, R. T. (Eds.), Universals in linguistic theory. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, P., & Menn, L. (1977). On the acquisition of phonology. In Macnamara, J. (Ed.), Language, learning, and thought. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Liberman, I. Y. (1973). Segmentation of the spoken word and reading acquisition. Bulletin of the Orlon Society, 8, 6577.Google Scholar
Liberman, I. Y., & Shankweiler, D. (1979). Speech, the alphabet, and teaching to read. In Resnick, L. & Weaver, P. (Eds.), Theory and practice of early reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Liberman, I. Y., & Shankweiler, D. (1985). Phonology and the problems of learning to read and write. Remedial and Special Education, 6, 817.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, A., Fowler, C., & Fischer, F. (1977). Phonetic segmentation and recoding in the beginning reader. In Reber, A. & Scarborough, D. (Eds.), Toward a psychology of reading: The proceedings of the CUNY Conferences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Locke, J. (1986). Speech perception and the emergent lexicon: An ethological approach. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (Eds.), Language acquisition (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Locke, J. (1988). The sound shape of early lexical representations. In Smith, M. & Locke, J. (Eds.), The emergent lexicon: The child’s development of a linguistic vocabulary. San Diego: Academic.Google Scholar
Macken, M. (1979). Developmental reorganization of phonology: A hierarchy of basic units of acquisition. Lingua, 49, 263, 265, 267–268, 314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macken, M. (1986). Phonological development: A crosslinguistic perspective. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (Eds.), Language acquisition (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mann, V. (1986). Phonological awareness: The role of reading experience. Cognition, 24, 6592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mattingly, I. (1984). Reading, linguistic awareness and language acquisition. In Downing, J. & Veltin, R. (Eds.), Linguistic awareness and learning to read. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Menn, L. (1978). Phonological units in beginning speech. In Bell, A. & Hooper, J. (Eds.), Syllables and segments. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Moskowitz, A. (1970). The acquisition of phonology. Berkeley: University of California Language & Behavior Research Lab.Google Scholar
Moskowitz, B. A. (1980). Idioms in phonology acquisition and phonological change. Journal of Phonetics, 8, 6983.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nearey, T. (1981). The psychological reality of phonological representations: Experimental evidence. In Myers, T., Laver, J., & Anderson, J. (Eds.), The cognitive representation of speech. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Olmsted, D. (1971). Out of the mouths of babes. The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olofsson, A., & Lundberg, I. (1983). Can phonemic awareness be trained in kindergarten? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 24, 3544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, A. (1983). The units of language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Read, C. (1975). Children’s categorization of speech sounds in English (Research Report No. 17). Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.Google Scholar
Rosner, J., & Simon, D. (1971). The Auditory Analysis Test: An initial report. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 4, 4048.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savin, H. (1972). What the child knows about speech when he starts to learn to read. In Kavanagh, J. & Mattingly, I. (Eds.), Language by ear and eye. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Schane, S. (1973). Generative phonology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Steinberg, D. (1973). Phonology, reading, and Chomsky and Halle’s optimal orthography. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2, 239258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teale, W., & Sulzby, E. (Eds.). (1986). Emergent literacy: Writing and reading. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Templeton, S., & Scarborough-Franks, L. (1985). The spelling’s the thing: Knowledge of derivational morphology in orthography and phonology among older students. Applied Psycholinguistics, 6, 371390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treiman, R. (1985). Spelling of stop consonants after /s/ by children and adults. Applied Psycholinguistics, 6, 261282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treiman, R. (1987). On the relationship between phonological awareness and literacy. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 7(5), 524529.Google Scholar
Waterson, N. (1971). Child phonology: A prosodie view. Journal of Linguistics, 7, 179211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wepman, J., & Hass, W. (1969). Spoken word count: Children ages 5, 6, and 7. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Wysocki, K., & Jenkins, J. (1987). Deriving word meanings through morphological generalization. Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 6681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zifcak, M. (1981). Phonological awareness and reading acquisition. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 6, 117126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar