Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T23:11:14.223Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Assessing the productive vocabulary of Spanish–English bilingual toddlers from low-income families

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 March 2011

JEANNETTE MANCILLA-MARTINEZ*
Affiliation:
University of Illinois at Chicago
BARBARA ALEXANDER PAN
Affiliation:
Harvard Graduate School of Education
SHAHER BANU VAGH
Affiliation:
Harvard Graduate School of Education
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Jeannette Mancilla-Martinez, University of Illinois at Chicago, College of Education, 1040 West Harrison Street (MC 147), Room 3042, Chicago, IL 60607. E-mail: jmm25@uic.edu

Abstract

This study investigates the utility and validity of the MacArthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventory (CDI) for use with low-income parents and their 24- to 36-month-old Spanish–English bilingual children (n = 79). Issues in the interpretation of the integrated CDI/Inventarios del Desarrollo de Habilidades Comunicativas (IDHC) score to index bilingual children's overall conceptual knowledge are also considered. Results indicate that the CDI/IDHC can be used with this population through at least age 36 months and parents are accurate reporters of their children's Spanish and English vocabulary. The value of the integrated score was confirmed. However, given the lack of norms associated with the integrated score, the complexity of determining how best to interpret this score was underscored.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, R. (2004). First language loss in Spanish-speaking children: Patterns of loss and implications for clinical practice. In Goldstein, B. (Ed.) Bilingual language development and disorders in Spanish–English speakers (pp. 187212). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
August, D., & Shanahan, T. (2006) (Eds.), Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language-Minority Children and Youth. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bedore, L. M., & Peña, E. D. (2008). Assessment of bilingual children for identification of language impairment: Current findings and implications for practice. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 11, 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bedore, L. M., Peña, E. D., García, M., & Cortez, C. (2005). Conceptual versus monolingual scoring: When does it make a difference? Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 188200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyce, L. K., Akers, J. F., Innocenti, M. S., Ortiz, E., & Cook, G. A. (2008). Expressive vocabulary development of young Spanish–English bilingual children living in poverty. Manuscript submitted for publication, Utah State University.Google Scholar
Caesar, L. G., & Kohler, P. D. (2007). The state of school-based bilingual assessment: Actual practice versus recommended guidelines. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38, 190200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, P. S., Fenson, L., & Thal, D. (1993). Some suggestions for the adaptation of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories to additional languages. Unpublished manuscript. Retrieved from http://www.sci.sdsu.edu/cdi/suggestions_adapatations.htmGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A. (1999). Environmental factors in early bilingual development: The role of parental beliefs and attitudes. In Extra, G. & Verhoeven, L. (Eds.), Bilingualism and migration (pp. 7596). New York: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A., Bornstein, M. H., & De Coster, S. (2006). Early understanding of two words for the same thing: a CDI study of lexical comprehension in infant bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 10, 331347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A., Bornstein, M. H., & Leach, D. B. (2005). Assessing early communicative ability: A cross-reporter cumulative score for the MacArthur CDI. Journal of Child Language, 32, 735758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd ed.). Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Padilla, E. R., Lugo, D. E., & Dunn, L. M. (1986). Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody. Circle Pine, MN: American Guidance Services.Google Scholar
Federal Register. (2008). The 2008 Health & Human Services Poverty Guidelines. Retrieved March 14, 2008, from http://aspe.hhs.gov/poverty/08poverty.shtmlGoogle Scholar
Feldman, H. M., Dollaghan, C. A., Campbell, T. F., Kurs-Lasky, M., Janosky, J. E., & Paradise, J. L. (2000). Measurement properties of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories at ages one and two years. Child Development, 71, 310322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User's guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. (1982). Life with two languages: An introduction to bilingualism. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. (1989). Neurolinguists, beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals in one person. Brain and Language, 36, 315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F. (2008). Studying bilinguals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Harwood, R. L., Leyendecker, B., Carlson, V. J., Asencio, M., & Miller, A. (2002). Parenting among Latino families in the U.S. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 4. Social conditions and applied parenting (2nd ed., pp. 2146). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hernandez, D. J., Denton, N. A., & Macartney, S. E. (2008). Children in immigrant families: Looking into America's future. Social Policy Report, 3, 322.Google Scholar
Jackson-Maldonado, D., Thal, D. J., Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Newton, T., & Conboy, B. (2003). MacArthur Inventarios del Desarollo de Habilidades Comunicativas: User's guide and technical manual. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Leopold, W. (1939). Speech development of a bilingual child: A linguist's record: Vol. 1. Vocabulary growth in the first two years. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Lesaux, N., Koda, K., Seigel, L., & Shanahan, T. (2006). Development of literacy. In August, D. & Shanahan, T. (Eds.), Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language-Minority Children and Youth. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A. (1999). Scoring program for the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories. Retrieved from http://www.sci.sdsu.edu/cdi/scoringdb_p.htmGoogle Scholar
Marchman, V. A., & Martinez-Sussmann, C. (2002). Concurrent validity of caregiver/parent report measures of language for children who are learning both English and Spanish. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 45, 983997.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchman, V. A., Martinez-Sussmann, C., & Dale, P. S. (2004). The language-specific nature of grammatical development: Evidence from bilingual language learners. Developmental Science, 7, 212224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mueller Gathercole, V. C., Thomas, E. M., & Hughes, E. (2008). Designing a normed receptive vocabulary test for bilingual populations: A model from Welsh. The International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 11, 678720.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, NIH DHHS. (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel: Teaching children to read (Report No. 00-4769). Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office.Google Scholar
Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Spier, E., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. (2004). Measuring productive vocabulary of toddlers in low-income families: Concurrent and predictive validity of three sources of data. Journal of Child Language, 31, 587608.Google ScholarPubMed
Pearson, B. Z., & Fernández, S. C. (1994). Patterns of interaction in the lexical growth in two languages of bilingual infants and toddlers. Language Learning, 44, 617653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S. C., Lewedeg, V., & Oller, D. K. (1997). The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 4158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S. C., & Oller, D. K. (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: Comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning, 43, 93120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S. C., & Oller, D. K. (1995). Cross-language synonyms in the lexicons of bilingual infants: One language or two? Journal of Child Language, 22, 345368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petitto, L. A., & Holowka, S. (2002). Evaluating attributions of delay and confusion in young bilinguals: Special insights from infants acquiring a signed and a spoken language. Sign Language Studies, 3, 433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pew Hispanic Center. (2006). Statistical portrait of Hispanics in the United States, 2006 (table 22). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Rescorla, L., & Achenbach, T. M. (2002). Use of the Language Development Survey (LDS) in a national probability sample of children 18 to 35 months old. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 733743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, J. E., Burchinal, M., & Durham, M. (1999). Parents’ report of vocabulary and grammatical development of African American preschoolers: Child and environmental associations. Child Development, 70, 92106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (1998) (Eds.), Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Suarez-Orozco, M. M., & Paez, M. M. (2002). Latinos: Remaking America. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Thal, D., Jackson-Maldonado, D., & Acosta, D. (2000). Validity of a parent-report measure of vocabulary and grammar for Spanish-speaking toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 43, 10871100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
US Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families. (2007). Head Start Program Fact Sheet, fiscal year 2007. Retrieved from http://www.acf.hhs.gov/programs/hsb/about/index.html#factsheetGoogle Scholar
Vagh, S. B., Pan, B. A., & Mancilla-Martinez, J. (2009). Measuring growth in bilingual and monolingual children's English productive vocabulary development: The utility of combining parent and teacher report. Child Development, 80, 15451563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodcock, R. W. (1991). Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery—Revised. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.Google Scholar
Woodcock, R. W., & Muñoz-Sandoval, A. F. (1995). Bateria Woodcock–Muñoz Pruebas de Aprovechamieto—Evisada. Chicago: Riverside.Google Scholar