Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T04:27:58.008Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Contextual effects on the conversations of mothers and their children with language impairment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 April 2013

MELANIE STICH
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
LUIGI GIROLAMETTO*
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
CARLA J. JOHNSON
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
PATRICIA L. CLEAVE
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University
XI CHEN
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Luigi Girolametto, Department of Speech–Language Pathology, Rehabilitation Sciences Building, Faculty of Medicine, University of Toronto, 160–500 University Avenue, Toronto, ON M5G 1V7, Canada. E-mail: l.girolametto@utoronto.ca

Abstract

Twenty-four mothers and their preschool children with language impairment participated in two 12-min sessions of toy play and book reading that were transcribed to yield maternal mean length of utterance in morphemes (MLU-m), type token ratio (TTR), and maternal interaction style (directive vs. responsive). Maternal MLU-m was significantly longer during book reading than during toy play, whereas TTR was similar across contexts. In contrast, children's MLU-m was similar across contexts, whereas TTR was higher during book reading. Mothers used an eliciting style characterized by more commands and questions during toy play than during book reading. Only maternal MLU-m predicted children's expressive language skills (i.e., a composite score of two standardized language tests). The implications include sampling both book reading and play interactions because they provide differential opportunities for conversation and language productivity.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bernicot, J., Comeau, J., & Feider, H. (1994). Dialogues between French-speaking mothers and daughters in two cultures: France and Quebec. Discourse Processes, 18, 1934.Google Scholar
Bradshaw, M., Hoffman, P., & Norris, J. (1998). Efficacy of expansions and cloze procedures in the development of interpretations by preschool children exhibiting delayed language development. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 29, 8595.Google Scholar
Burgemeister, B., Hollander Blum, L., & Lorge, I. (1972). Columbia Mental Maturity Scale. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Coorporation.Google Scholar
Bus, A., van IJzendoorn, M., & Pellegrini, A. (1995). Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read: A meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. Review of Educational Research, 65, 121.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G. (1990). Maternal recasts and other contingent replies to language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 262274.Google Scholar
Cowley, J., & Glasgow, C. (1994). The Renfrew Bus Story: Language screening by narrative recall. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services.Google Scholar
Crain-Thoreson, C., Dahlin, M., & Powell, T. (2001). Parent–child interaction in three conversational contexts: Variations in style and strategy. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 92, 2337.Google Scholar
Crain-Thoreson, C., & Dale, P. (1992). Do early talkers become early readers? Linguistic precocity, preschool language, and emergent literacy. Developmental Psychology, 28, 421429.Google Scholar
Crain-Thoreson, C., & Dale, P. (1999). Enhancing linguistic performance: Parents and teachers as book reading partners for children with language delays. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education. Special Issue: 62, 19, 2839.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crawley, S. B., & Spiker, D. (1983). Mother–child interactions involving two-year-olds with Down syndrome: A look at individual differences. Child Development, 54, 13121323.Google Scholar
Cross, T. (1977). Mother's speech adjustments: The contributions of selected child listener variables. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 157188). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Crowe, L., Norris, J., & Hoffman, P. (2004). Training caregivers to facilitate communicative participation of preschool children with language impairment during storybook reading. Journal of Communication Disorders, 37, 177196.Google Scholar
Dale, P., Crain-Thoreson, C., Notari-Syverson, A., & Cole, K. (1996). Parent–child book reading as an intervention technique for young children with language delays. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 16, 213235.Google Scholar
Dawson, J., Stout, C., Eyer, J., Tattersall, P., Fonkalsrud, J., & Croley, K. (2005). Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test–Preschool (2nd ed.). DeKalb, IL: Janelle Publications.Google Scholar
Evans, M., & Schmidt, F. (1991). Repeated maternal book reading with two children: Language-normal and language-impaired. First Language, 11, 269287.Google Scholar
Field, A. (2005). Discovering statistics using SPSS (2nd ed.). London: Sage.Google Scholar
Fromberg, D., & Bergen, D. (2006). Play from birth to twelve: Contexts, perspectives, and meaning. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Girolametto, L., Johnson, C. J., Cleave, P. L., Chen, X., & Truxler, J. (2012). Emergent literacy intervention for preschoolers with language disorders: A controlled feasibility study. Unpublished manuscript, University of Toronto, Department of Speech–Language Pathology.Google Scholar
Girolametto, L., & Weitzman, E. (2002). Responsiveness of child care providers in interactions with toddlers and preschoolers. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in the Schools, 33, 268282.Google Scholar
Girolametto, L., & Weitzman, E. (2006). It Takes Two to Talk—The Hanen Program for Parents: Early language intervention through caregiver training. In McCauley, R. & Fey, M. (Eds.), Treatment of language disorders in children (pp. 77103). New York: Brookes.Google Scholar
Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., van Lieshout, R., & Duff, D. (2000). Directiveness in teachers’ language input to toddlers and preschoolers in day care. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 11011114.Google Scholar
Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., Wiigs, M., & Pearce, P. (2000). The relationship between maternal language measures and language development. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 8, 364374.Google Scholar
Hargrave, A., & Senechal, M. (2000). A book reading intervention with preschool children who have limited vocabularies: The benefits of regular reading and dialogic reading. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 15, 7590.Google Scholar
Harris, M., Jones, D., Brookes, S., & Grant, J. (1986). Relations between the non-verbal context of maternal speech and rate of language development. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 261268.Google Scholar
Harris, R. (1978). Don't forget to come back. Cambridge, MA: Candlewick Press.Google Scholar
Hebert, H., Swank, P., Smith, K., & Landry, S. (2004). Maternal support for play and language across early childhood. Early Education and Development, 15, 93116.Google Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1990). Maternal speech and the child's development of syntax: A further look. Journal of Child Language, 17, 8599.Google Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1991). Conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Development, 62, 782796.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Levine, S., & Vevea, J. (1998). Environmental input and cognitive growth: A study using time-period comparisons. Child Development, 69, 10121029.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S. (2002). Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 337374.Google Scholar
Jeffers, O. (2004). How to catch a star. London: HarperCollins Children's Books.Google Scholar
Kaderavek, J., & Justice, L. M. (2002). Shared storybook reading as an intervention context: Practices and potential pitfalls. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 11, 395406.Google Scholar
Kaderavek, J., & Sulzby, E. (1998). Parent–child joint book reading: An observational protocol for young children. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 7, 3347.Google Scholar
Kaderavek, J., & Sulzby, E. (2000). Issues in emergent literacy for children with language impairments. In Watson, L., Crais, E., & Layton, T. L. (Eds.), Handbook of early language impariment in children (pp. 199244). Albany, NY: Delmar Thomson Learning.Google Scholar
Marvin, C., & Wright, D. (1997). Literacy socialization in the homes of preschool children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 28, 154163.Google Scholar
Miller, J. (1981). Assessing language production in children: Experimental procedures. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Miller, J., & Chapman, R. (2002). Systematic analysis of language transcripts. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin–Madison.Google Scholar
Norris, J., & Hoffman, P. (1990). Language intervention within naturalistic environments. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 21, 7284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rabidoux, P., & MacDonald, J. (2000). An interactive taxonomy of mothers and children during storybook interactions. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 9, 331344.Google Scholar
Richards, B. (1987). Type/Token Ratios: What do they really tell us? Journal of Child Language, 14, 201209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sackett, G. (1978). Observing behavior. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Senechal, M. (1997). The differential effect of storybook reading on preschoolers’ acquisition of expressive and receptive vocabulary. Journal of Child Language, 24, 123138.Google Scholar
Simon, F. (2003). Little yellow dog gets a shock. London: Orion Children's Books.Google Scholar
Skibbe, L., Mooney, A., Justice, L. M., & McGinty, A. S. (2010). Socio-emotional climate of storybook reading interactions for mothers and preschoolers with language impairment. Reading and Writing, 23, 5371.Google Scholar
Sulzby, E., & Kaderavek, J. (1996). Parent–child language during storybook reading and toy play contexts: Case studies of normally developing and specific language impaired (SLI) children. National Reading Conference Yearbook, 45, 257269.Google Scholar
Tannock, R. (1988). Mothers’ directiveness in their interactions with their children with and without Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 93, 154165.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 14541463.Google Scholar
Vandermaas-Peeler, M., Nelson, J., Bumpass, C., & Sassine, B. (2009). Social contexts of development: Parent–child interactions during reading and play. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 9, 295317.Google Scholar
Weistuch, L., & Lewis, M. (1986). Effect of maternal language intervention strategies on the language abilities of delayed two- to four-year-olds. New York: Eastern Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Whitehurst, G., Arnold, D., Epstein, J., Angell, M., & Fischel, J. (1994). A picture book reading intervention in day care and home for children from low-income families. Developmental Psychology, 30, 679689.Google Scholar
Whitehurst, G., Falco, F., Lonigan, C., Fischel, J., De Baryshe, B., Valdez-Menchaca, M., et al. (1988). Accelerating language development through picture book reading. Developmental Psychology, 24, 552559.Google Scholar
Whitehurst, G. J., Zevenbergen, A. A., Crone, D. A., Schultz, M. D., Velting, O. N., & Fischel, J. E. (1999). Outcomes of an emergent literacy intervention from Head Start through second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 261272.Google Scholar
Wiig, E., Secord, W., & Semel, E. (2004). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals—Preschool (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Yoder, P., Kaiser, A., Alpert, C., & Fischer, R. (1993). Following the child's lead when teaching nouns to preschoolers with mental retardation. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 158167.Google Scholar
Yoder, P., Spruytenburg, H., Edwards, A., & Davies, B. (1995). Effect of verbal routine contexts and expansions on gains in the mean length of utterance in children with developmental delays. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 26, 2132.Google Scholar