Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T12:24:32.122Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Initial voicing contrast acquisition in normal and phonologically disordered children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Ann A. Tyler*
Affiliation:
University of Nevada, Reno
John H. Saxman
Affiliation:
Syracuse University
*
Ann A. Tyler, University of Nevada, Reno School of Medicine, Mackay Science Building, Dept. of Speech Pathology & Audiology, Reno, NV 89557-0046

Abstract

Speech sound production changes that occurred during acquisition of the initial voicing contrast induced through treatment in phonologically disordered children were compared to those that occurred during normal acquisition of the contrast. Target stop productions from three normally developing children and six phonologically disordered children were analyzed acoustically along the temporal measure of VOT. The phonologically disordered subjects displayed different patterns of voicing contrast acquisition, none of which replicated the normally developing children's pattern. The phonologically disordered children's patterns of acquisition were characterized by larger and more variable VOT values in comparison to those of the normally developing children. Different patterns of voicing contrast acquisition observed in VOTs are discussed with regard to the nature of linguistic change and the source of breakdown in children's speech sound errors.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1991

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baltes, P. B., Reese, H. W., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1977). Life-span developmental psychology: Introduction to research methods. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
Barton, D., & Macken, M. A. (1980). An instrumental analysis of the voicing contrast in word-initial stops in the speech offour-year-old English-speaking children. Language and Speech, 23, 159169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carrow-Woolfolk, E. (1985). Test of Auditory Comprehension of Language-Revised. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources.Google Scholar
Catts, H. W., & Jensen, P. J. (1983). Speech timing of phonologically disordered children: Voicing contrasts of initial and final stop consonants. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 26, 501510.Google Scholar
Chen, M., & Wang, W. S.-Y. (1975). Sound change: Actuation and implementation. Language, 51, 255281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, M. L. (1986, November). Comprehensive procedures for phonological assessment. Poster session presented at the Annual Convention of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, Detroit, MI.Google Scholar
Eguchi, S., & Hirsch, I. J. (1969). Development of speech sounds in children. Ada Otolaryngologica Supplementum, 257, 151.Google ScholarPubMed
Eilers, R. E., Oiler, D. K., & Benito-Garcia, C. R. (1984). The acquisition of voicing contrasts in Spanish and English learning infants and children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Language, 11, 313336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forrest, K., & Rockman, B. K. (1988). Acoustic and perceptual analysis of word-initial stop consonants in phonologically disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 31, 449459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forrest, K., Weismer, G., Dinnsen, D., & Elbert, M. (1990, November). Spectral analysis of /k/-/t/ in normal and phonologically disordered children. Paper presented at the Annual American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Convention, Seattle, WA.Google Scholar
Gierut, J. A., & Dinnsen, D. A. (1986). On word-initial voicing: Converging sources of evidence in phonologically disordered speech. Language and Speech, 29, 97114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, J. H. V. (1977). A voice onset time analysis of apical stop production in 3-year-olds. Journal of Child Language, 4, 103110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, R., & Fristoe, M. (1972). Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation. Circle Pines, MI: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (1976). Phonological disability in children. New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Janson, T. (1983). Sound change in perception and production. Language, 59, 1834.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kent, R. D., & Forner, L. L. (1980). Speech segment durations in sentence recitations by children and adults. Journal of Phonetics, 8, 157168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kewley-Port, D., & Preston, M. S. (1974). Early apical stop production: A voice-onset time analysis. Journal of Phonetics, 8, 195210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klatt, D. H. (1975). Voice onset time, frication and aspiration in word-initial consonant clusters. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 18, 686706.Google Scholar
Kuhn, D. (1974). Inducing development experimentally: Comments on a research paradigm. Developmental Psychology, 10, 590600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, W. (1981). Resolving the Neogrammarian controversy. Language, 57, 267308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word, 20, 384422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macken, M. A. (1980). The child's lexical representation: The “puzzle-puddle-pickle” evidence. Journal of Linguistics, 16, 117.Google Scholar
Macken, M. A., & Barton, D. (1980). A longitudinal study of the acquisition of the voicing contrast in American English word-initial stops, as measured by voice-onset time. Journal of Child Language, 7, 4174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maxwell, E. (1984). On determining child underlying phonological representations: A critique of the current theories. In Elbert, M., Dinnsen, D. A., & Weismer, G. (Eds.), Phonological theory and the misarticulating child (ASHA Monographs No. 22, pp. 1829). Rock-ville, MD: ASHA.Google Scholar
Maxwell, E., & Weismer, G. (1982). The contribution of phonological, acoustic, and perceptual techniques to the characterization of a misarticulating child's voice contrast for stops. Applied Psycholinguistics, 3, 2943.Google Scholar
Metz, D. E., Conture, E. G., & Caruso, A. (1979). Voice onset time, frication, and aspiration during stutterers' fluent speech. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 22, 649656.Google Scholar
Miller, J. F., & Chapman, R. (1981). The relation between age and mean length of utterance in morphemes. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 24, 154161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preston, M. S., Yeni-Komshian, G., & Stark, R. E. (1967). Voicing in initial stop consonants produced by children in the prelinguistic period from different language communities. Annual report: Neurocommunications laboratory, 2, 305323. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine.Google Scholar
Sharkey, S. G., & Folkins, J. W. (1985). Variability of lip and jaw movements in children and adults: Implications for the development of speech motor control. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, 815.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smit, A. B., & Bernthal, J. E. (1983). Voicing contrasts and their phonological implications in the speech of articulation-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 26, 486500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, N. V. (1973). The acquisition of phonology: A case study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C., & Cooper, J. A. (1984). Patterns of early lexical and phonological development. Journal of Child Language, 11, 247271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyler, A. A., Edwards, M. L., & Saxman, J. H. (1987). Clinical application of two phonologically based treatment procedures. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52, 393409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyler, A. A., Edwards, M. L., & Saxman, J. H. (1990). Acoustic validation of phonological knowledge and its relationship to treatment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 251261.Google Scholar
Velleman, S. L. (1988). The role of linguistic perception in later phonological development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 221236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. M., & Greenlee, M. (1987). Individual differences in phonological development: Ages one and three years. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 30, 503521.Google Scholar
Wang, W. S.-Y. (1969). Competing changes as a cause of residue. Language, 45, 925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, C. E. (1978). Weiss Comprehensive Articulation Test. Boston: Teaching Resources Corporation.Google Scholar
Zebrowski, P. M., Conture, E. G., & Cudahy, E. A. (1985). Acoustic analysis of young stutterers' fluency: Preliminary observations. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 10, 173192.Google Scholar
Zlatin, M. A. (1974). Voicing contrast: Perceptual and productive voice onset time characteristics of adults. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 56, 981994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zlatin, M. A., & Koenigsknecht, R. A. (1976). Development of the voicing contrast: A comparison of voice onset time in stop perception and production. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 19, 93111.Google Scholar