Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T22:40:59.981Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Majority language vocabulary and nonword repetition skills in children attending minority language immersion education

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 October 2022

Vasiliki Chondrogianni*
Affiliation:
School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, Scotland, United Kingdom
Florence Judge-Clayden
Affiliation:
School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, Scotland, United Kingdom
Morna Butcher
Affiliation:
NHS Ayrshire and Arran, Glasgow, Scotland, United Kingdom
*
*Corresponding author. Email: v.chondrogianni@ed.ac.uk

Abstract

The present study examined nonword repetition (NWR) and comprehension/production of single-word vocabulary in the majority language (English) in six- to eight-year-old English-Gaelic emergent bilingual children attending Gaelic-medium primary education (GMPE) (primary years 2 and 3). GMPE is an immersion education model found in Scotland where the minority language, Gaelic, is the language of instruction, whereas English is the majority community language, not supported in school in the first three years of primary schooling. All children spoke English as their first language (L1) and had varying exposure to the second language (L2) Gaelic outside the school setting. We investigated how individual factors, such as age and frequency of exposure, and psycholinguistic factors, such as task modality, word class (for vocabulary), frequency, length, and prosody (for NWR), influenced performance. Later exposure to Gaelic and higher nonverbal IQ were contributing predictors of performance for English vocabulary. For NWR, participants with earlier and longer exposure to Gaelic who also used both languages at home more performed significantly better on this task. These results suggest that minority language immersion education differentially affects majority language abilities; later L2 onset benefited L1 vocabulary learning, whereas earlier L2 onset boosted L1 NWR skills in these bilinguals.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Altman, C., Goldstein, T., & Armon-Lotem, S. (2017). Quantitative and qualitative differences in the lexical knowledge of monolingual and bilingual children on the LITMUS-CLT task. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 31(11–12), 931954. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699206.2017.1312533 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Antonijevic-Elliott, S., Lyons, R., O’ Malley, M. P., Meir, N., Haman, E., Banasik, N., Carroll, C., McMenamin, R., Rodden, M., & Fitzmaurice, Y. (2020). Language assessment of monolingual and multilingual children using non-word and sentence repetition tasks. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 34(4), 293311. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699206.2019.1637458 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Archibald, L. M. D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Short-term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41(6), 675693. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820500442602 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Archibald, L. M. D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2007). Nonword repetition in specific language impairment: More than a phonological short-term memory deficit. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(5), 919924.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Archibald, L. M. D., Gathercole, S. E., & Joanisse, M. F. (2009). Multisyllabic nonwords: More than a string of syllables. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125(3), 17121722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Armon-Lotem, S., & Ohana, O. (2017). A CDI study of bilingual English-Hebrew children – Frequency of exposure as a major source of variation. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 20(2), 201217. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2016.1179257 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, C. (2010). Bilingual education. In Kaplan, R. B. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of applied linguistics. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195384253.013.0020 Google Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 1(1). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01 Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2018). Bilingual education for young children: Review of the effects and consequences. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 21(6), 666679. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2016.1203859 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boerma, T., Chiat, S., Leseman, P., Timmermeister, M., Wijnen, F., & Blom, E. (2015). A Quasi-Universal Nonword repetition task as a diagnostic tool for bilingual children learning Dutch as a second language. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58(6), 17471760. https://doi.org/10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-15-0058 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boerma, T., Leseman, P., Wijnen, F., & Blom, E. (2017). Language proficiency and sustained attention in monolingual and bilingual children with and without language impairment. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiat, S. (2015). Non-word repetition. In Armon-Lotem, S., de Jong, J., & Meir, N. (Eds.), Assessing multilingual children (pp. 125150). Multilingual Matters. https://doi.org/doi:10.21832/9781783093137-008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chiat, S., & Polišenská, K. (2016). A framework for Crosslinguistic Nonword repetition tests: Effects of Bilingualism and socioeconomic status on children’s performance. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59(5), 11791189. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0293 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiat, S., Polišenská, K., Yanushevskaya, I., & Antonijevic, S. (2020). Language neutral Crosslinguistic Nonword Repetition Test (CL-NWR). https://www.bi-sli.org/cl-nonword-repetiti Google Scholar
Chondrogianni, V. (2018). Child L2 acquisition. In Miller, D., Bayram, F., Rothman, J., & Serratrice, L. (Eds.), Studies in bilingualism (Vol. 54, pp. 103126). John Benjamins Pub Co. https://doi.org/10.1075/sibil.54.06cho Google Scholar
Chondrogianni, V., & Marinis, T. (2011). Differential effects of internal and external factors on the development of vocabulary, tense morphology and morpho-syntax in successive bilingual children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1(3), 318345. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.1.3.05cho CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Comajoan-Colomé, L., & Coronel-Molina, S. M. (2021). What does language revitalisation in the twenty-first century look like? New trends and frameworks. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 42(10), 897904. https://doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2020.1827643 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B. (2001). Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(6), 741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cummins, J. (2000). Language, power, and pedagogy: Bilingual children in the crossfire. Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., & Carter, D. M. (1987). The predominance of strong initial syllables in the English vocabulary. Computer Speech & Language, 2(3), 133142. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-2308(87)90004-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daller, M., & Ongun, Z. (2017). The threshold hypothesis revisited: Bilingual lexical knowledge and non-verbal IQ development. International Journal of Bilingualism, 22(6), 675694. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006917690835 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, J., Kuiken, F., Jorna, R. J., & Klinkerberg, E. L. (2016). The role of majority and minority language input in the early development of a bilingual vocabulary. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(1), 191205. https://doi.org/DOI:10.1017/S1366728915000012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dollaghan, C., Biber, M., & Campbell, T. (1993). Constituent syllable effects in a nonsense-word repetition task. In Journal of Speech & Hearing Research (Vol. 36, Issue 5, pp. 1051–1054). American Speech-Language-Hearing Assn. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3605.1051 Google Scholar
dos Santos, C., & Ferré, S. (2018). A Nonword repetition task to assess bilingual children’s phonology. Language Acquisition, 25(1), 5871. https://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2016.1243692 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duff, F. J., Reen, G., Plunkett, K., & Nation, K. (2015). Do infant vocabulary skills predict school-age language and literacy outcomes? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(8), 848856. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12378 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engel de Abreu, P. M. J., Cruz-Santos, A., Tourinho, C. J., Martin, R., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingualism enriches the poor: Enhanced cognitive control in low-income minority children. Psychological Science, 23(11), 13641371. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612443836 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29(3), 336360. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(90)90004-J CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Frankish, C. R., Pickering, S. J., & Peaker, S. (1999). Phonotactic in- fluences on short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(1), 8495. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.25.1.84 Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28(5), 887898. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.28.5.887 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, V. C., & Thomas, E. (2009). Bilingual first-language development: Dominant language takeover, threatened minority language take-up. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(2), 213237. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909004015 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, V. C., Thomas, E., & Hughes, E. (2008). Designing a normed receptive vocabulary test for bilingual populations: A model from Welsh. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 11(6), 678720. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050802149283 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, V. C., Thomas, E. M., Kennedy, I., Prys, C., Young, N., Viñas Guasch, N., Roberts, E. J., Hughes, E. K., & Jones, L. (2014). Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from preschoolers through older adults on card sorting, Simon, and metalinguistic tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, V. C., Thomas, E. M., Roberts, E., Hughes, C., & Hughes, E. K. (2013). Why assessment needs to take exposure into account: Vocabulary and grammatical abilities in bilingual children. In Gathercole, V. C. M. (Ed.), Issues in the assessment of bilinguals (pp. 2055). Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F. (2006). What do we know about bilingual education for majority-language students? The Handbook of Bilingualism, pp. 547576. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470756997.ch21 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, T. A., Jarmulowicz, L., & Oller, D. K. (2018). Difficulties using standardized tests to identify the receptive expressive gap in bilingual children’s vocabularies. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(2), 328339. https://doi.org/DOI:10.1017/S1366728917000074 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, T. A., Oller, D. K., Jarmulonowicz, L., & Ethington, C. A. (2012). The receptive–expressive gap in the vocabulary of young second-language learners: Robustness and possible mechanisms. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(01), 102116. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728910000490 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, T. A., Peña, E. D., & Bedore, L. M. (2014). The relation between language experience and receptive-expressive semantic gaps in bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 17(1), 90110. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2012.743960 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, T. A., Peña, E. D., Bedore, L. M., & McCarter, K. S. (2020). A longitudinal investigation of the semantic receptive-expressive gap in Spanish-English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 115. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2020.1721427 Google ScholarPubMed
Golberg, H., Paradis, J., & Crago, M. (2008). Lexical acquisition over time in minority first language children learning English as a second language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29(1), 4165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T. C. (2008). More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(3), 787814. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.07.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haman, E., Łuniewska, M., Hansen, P., Simonsen, H. G., Chiat, S., Bjekić, J., Blažienė, A., Chyl, K., Dabašinskienė, I., Engel de Abreu, P., Gagarina, N., Gavarró, A., Håkansson, G., Harel, E., Holm, E., Kapalková, S., Kunnari, S., Levorato, C., Lindgren, J., … Armon-Lotem, S. (2017). Noun and verb knowledge in monolingual preschool children across 17 languages: Data from Cross-linguistic Lexical Tasks (LITMUS-CLT). Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 31(11–12), 818843. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699206.2017.1308553 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haman, E., Łuniewska, M., & Pomiechowska, B. (2015). Designing cross-linguistic lexical tasks (CLTs) for bilingual preschool children (Armon-Lotem, S., de Jong, J., & Meir, N. eds., pp. 196240). Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Haman, E., Wodniecka, Z., Marecka, M., Szewczyk, J., Białecka-Pikul, M., Otwinowska, A., Mieszkowska, K., Łuniewska, M., Kołak, J., Miękisz, A., Kacprzak, A., Banasik, N., & Foryś-Nogala, M. (2017). How does L1 and L2 exposure impact L1 performance in bilingual children? Evidence from Polish-English migrants to the United Kingdom. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hansen, P., Simonsen, H. G., Łuniewska, M., & Haman, E. (2017). Validating the psycholinguistic aspects of LITMUS-CLT: Evidence from Polish and Norwegian. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 31(11–12), 910930. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699206.2017.1307455 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrell, F. Jr., & Dupont, Ch. (2020). Hmisc: Harrell Miscellaneous. R Package Version 4.2-0. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=Hmisc Google Scholar
Hoff, E., Rumiche, R., Burridge, A., Ribot, K. M., & Welsh, S. N. (2014). Expressive vocabulary development in children from bilingual and monolingual homes: A longitudinal study from two to four years. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 433444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.04.012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnstone, R., Harlen, W., MacNeil, M., Stradling, R., & Thorpe, G. (1999). The attainments of pupils receiving Gaelic medium education in Scotland. University of Stirling, Scottish CILT for the Scottish Executive Education Department.Google Scholar
Jones, G. (2016). The influence of children’s exposure to language from two to six years: The case of nonword repetition. Cognition, 153, 7988. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.04.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, G., Gobet, F., & Pine, J. M. (2007). Linking working memory and long-term memory: A computational model of the learning of new words. Developmental Science, 10(6), 853873. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00638.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juhasz, B. J. (2005). Age-of-acquisition effects in word and picture identification. Psychological Bulletin, 131(5), 684712. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.684 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kan, P. F., & Kohnert, K. (2012). A growth curve analysis of novel word learning by sequential bilingual preschool children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(3), 452469. https://doi.org/DOI:10.1017/S1366728911000356 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M., & Marian, V. (2009). The bilingual advantage in novel word learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(4), 705710. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.4.705 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamb, W. (2001). Scottish Gaelic. Lincom Europa.Google Scholar
Lamb, W. (2002). Scottish Gaelic (2nd ed.). Lincom Europa.Google Scholar
Lervåg, A., Dolean, D., Tincas, I., & Melby-Lervåg, M. (2019). Socioeconomic background, nonverbal IQ and school absence affects the development of vocabulary and reading comprehension in children living in severe poverty. Developmental Science, 22(5), e12858. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12858 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levelt, W. J. M. (2001). Relations between speech production and speech perception: Some behavioral and neurological observations. In E. Dupoux (Ed.), Language, brain and cognitive development: Essays in honour of Jacques Mehler (pp. 241256). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Łuniewska, M., Haman, E., Armon-Lotem, S., Etenkowski, B., Southwood, F., Anđelković, D., Blom, E., Boerma, T., Chiat, S., de Abreu, P. E., Gagarina, N., Gavarró, A., Håkansson, G., Hickey, T., de López, K. J., Marinis, T., Popović, M., Thordardottir, E., Blažienė, A., … Ünal-Logacev, Ö. (2016). Ratings of age of acquisition of 299 words across 25 languages: Is there a cross-linguistic order of words? Behavior Research Methods, 48(3), 11541177. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0636-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Łuniewska, M., Wodniecka, Z., Miller, C. A., Smolík, F., Butcher, M., Chondrogianni, V., Hreich, E. K., Messarra, C. A., Razak, R., Treffers-Daller, J., Yap, N. T., Abboud, L., Talebi, A., Gureghian, M., Tuller, L., & Haman, E. (2019). Age of acquisition of 299 words in seven languages: American English, Czech, Gaelic, Lebanese Arabic, Malay, Persian and Western Armenian. PLOS ONE, 14(8), e0220611. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0220611 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacNeilage, P. F., & Davis, B. L. (2000). On the origin of internal structure of word forms. Science, 288(5465), 527531. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.288.5465.527 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, C. R., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2009). Effects of word position and stress on onset cluster production: Evidence from typical development, specific language impairment, and dyslexia. Language, 85(1), 3957. https://doi.org/http://doi.org/10.1353/lan.0.0081 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masoura, E. V, & Gathercole, S. E. (1999). Phonological short-term memory and foreign language learning. International Journal of Psychology, 34(5–6), 383388. https://doi.org/10.1080/002075999399738 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonough, C., Song, L., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Lannon, R. (2011). An image is worth a thousand words: Why nouns tend to dominate verbs in early word learning. Developmental Science, 14(2), 181189. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00968.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meir, N., & Armon-Lotem, S. (2017). Independent and combined effects of socioeconomic status (SES) and bilingualism on children’s vocabulary and verbal short-term memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1442. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01442 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metsala, J. L. (1999). Young children’s phonological awareness and nonword repetition as a function of vocabulary development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(1), 319. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.1.3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montanari, S. (2014). A case study of bi-literacy development among children enrolled in an Italian–English dual language program in Southern California. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 17(5), 509525. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2013.833892 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 187202. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrison, D. A. (2019). Metrical structure in Scottish Gaelic: Tonal accent, glottalisation and overlength. Phonology, 36(3), 391432. https://doi.org/DOI:10.1017/S0952675719000204 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munson, B., Kurtz, B. A., & Windsor, J. (2005). The influence of vocabulary size, Phonotactic probability, and wordlikeness on nonword repetitions of children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(5), 10331047. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/072) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nance, C., & Maolalaigh, R. Ó. (2019). Scottish Gaelic. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 115. https://doi.org/DOI:10.1017/S002510031900015X Google Scholar
National Records of Scotland. (2015). Scotland’s census 2011: Gaelic report (part 1). National Records of Scotland.Google Scholar
Nicolay, A.-C., & Poncelet, M. (2013). Cognitive abilities underlying second-language vocabulary acquisition in an early second-language immersion education context: A longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(4), 655671. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.04.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ó Muircheartaigh, J., & Hickey, T. (2008). Academic outcome, anxiety and attitudes in early and late immersion in Ireland. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 11(5), 558576. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050802149184 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Hanlon, F., Paterson, L., & McLeod, W. (2012). Language models in Gaelic medium pre-school, primary and secondary education: Vol. null (null (ed.)). Soillse.Google Scholar
O’Hanlon, F., Paterson, L., & McLeod, W. (2013). The attainment of pupils in Gaelic-medium primary education in Scotland. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 16(6), 707729. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2012.711807 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Toole, C., Ní Shíthigh, D., Molamphy, A., & Walsh, E. (2019). Findings from the first phase of developing a receptive vocabulary test for the Irish language. International Journal of Bilingualism, 24(4), 572587. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006919848142 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oller, D. K., Pearson, B. Z., & Cobo-Lewis, A. B. (2007). Profile effects in early bilingual language and literacy. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(2), 191230. https://doi.org/DOI:10.1017/S0142716407070117 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Padilla, A. M., Fan, L., Xu, X., & Silva, D. (2013). A Mandarin/English two-way immersion program: Language proficiency and academic achievement. Foreign Language Annals, 46(4), 661679. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/flan.12060 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. (2011). Individual differences in child English second language acquisition comparing child-internal and child-external factors. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1(3), 213237. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.1.3.01par CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Genesee, F., & Crago, M. B. (2011). Dual language development and disorders: A handbook on bilingualism and second language learning (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Pub. Co.Google Scholar
Raven, J. C. (2003). Raven’s coloured progressive matrices. Pearson Assessment.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Rhys, M., & Thomas, E. M. (2013). Bilingual Welsh–English children’s acquisition of vocabulary and reading: implications for bilingual education. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 16(6), 633656. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2012.706248 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rispens, J., & Baker, A. (2012). Nonword repetition: The relative contributions of phonological short-term memory and phonological representations in children with language and reading impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55(3), 683694. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0263)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roy, P., & Chiat, S. (2004). A prosodically controlled word and nonword repetition task for 2- to 4-year-olds. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(1), 223234. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/019)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saffran, J. R., & Wilson, D. P. (2003). From syllables to syntax: Multilevel statistical learning by 12-month-old infants. Infancy, 4(2), 273284. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0402_07 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, M., Moin, V., & Leikin, M. (2012). Lexical knowledge development in the first and second languages among language-minority children: The role of bilingual versus monolingual preschool education. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 15(5), 549571. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2011.650332 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Semel, E., & Wiig, E. H. (2017). Clinical evaluation for language fundamentals 5 (CELF-5). Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
Sheng, L., & McGregor, K. K. (2010). Lexical–semantic organization in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(1), 146159. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0160)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smithson, L., Paradis, J., & Nicoladis, E. (2014). Bilingualism and receptive vocabulary achievement: Could sociocultural context make a difference? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(4), 810821. https://doi.org/DOI:10.1017/S1366728913000813 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorenson Duncan, T., & Paradis, J. (2016). English language learners’ nonword repetition performance: The influence of age, L2 vocabulary size, length of L2 exposure, and L1 phonology. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59(1), 3948. https://doi.org/10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-14-0020 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephen, C., McPake, J., McLeod, W., Pollock, I., & Carroll, T. (2010). Review of Gaelic medium early education and childcare. Retrieved form http://strathprints.strath.ac.uk/26270/1/0100403.pdf Google Scholar
Sundara, M., Polka, L., & Molnar, M. (2008). Development of coronal stop perception: Bilingual infants keep pace with their monolingual peers. Cognition, 108(1), 232242. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szewczyk, J. M., Marecka, M., Chiat, S., & Wodniecka, Z. (2018). Nonword repetition depends on the frequency of sublexical representations at different grain sizes: Evidence from a multi-factorial analysis. Cognition, 179(June), 2336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.06.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thiessen, E. D., & Saffran, J. R. (2003). When cues collide: Use of stress and statistical cues to word boundaries by 7- to 9-month-old infants. In Developmental psychology (Vol. 39, Issue 4, pp. 706716). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.4.706 Google Scholar
Thordardottir, E., & Brandeker, M. (2013). The effect of bilingual exposure versus language impairment on nonword repetition and sentence imitation scores. Journal of Communication Disorders, 46(1), 1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuller, L. (2015). Clinical use of parental questionnaires in multilingual contexts. In Armon-Lotem, S., de Jong, J., & Meir, N. (Eds.), Assessing mutlilingual children (pp. 301330). Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, S. (2013). Current issues in multilingual first language acquisition. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 33, 2150. https://doi.org/DOI:10.1017/S0267190513000044 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Wonderen, E., & Unsworth, S. (2020). Testing the validity of the Cross-Linguistic Lexical Task as a measure of language proficiency in bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 125. https://doi.org/DOI:10.1017/S030500092000063X Google ScholarPubMed
Verhagen, J., Boom, J., Mulder, H., de Bree, E., & Leseman, P. (2019). Reciprocal relationships between nonword repetition and vocabulary during the preschool years. In Developmental Psychology (Vol. 55, Issue 6, pp. 11251137). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000702 Google Scholar
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 21432152. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613488145 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yan, S., & Nicoladis, E. (2009). Finding le mot juste: Differences between bilingual and monolingual children’s lexical access in comprehension and production*. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(03), 323. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990101 CrossRefGoogle Scholar