Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T13:02:50.289Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Narrative as an index of communicative competence in mildly mentally retarded children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Lowry Hemphill
Affiliation:
Harvard University
Nancy Picardi
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts at Boston
Helen Tager-Flusberg*
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts at Boston
*
Helen Tager-Flusberg, Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts, Boston, MA 02125-3393

Abstract

This study compared the narrative abilities of mildly mentally retarded and nonretarded children. Twenty mildly mentally retarded children and 20 nonretarded children, matched on mental age, PPVT-R scores, and SES were audiotaped while narrating a wordless picture book story. Results showed no differences between the groups in narrative length, use of tense and conjunctions, and use of narrative devices. However, there were significant differences in use of reference, with the mildly retarded children using more definite article + noun character introductions, showing more pronoun confusion, and more often pronominalizing all references to the story protagonist. Control of reference in narrative is discussed as presenting a particularly challenging set of discourse abilities because it requires the child to integrate knowledge across a number of linguistic and nonlinguistic domains.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1991

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abbeduto, L., & Rosenberg, S. (1987). Linguistic communication and mental retardation. In Rosenberg, S. (Ed.), Advances in applied psycholinguistics: Vol. 1. Disorders of first language development (pp. 76123). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Astington, J. (1990). Narrative and the child’s theory of mind. In Britton, B. & Pellegrini, A. (Eds.), Narrative thought and narrative language (pp. 151171). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bamberg, M. (1987). The acquisition of narratives: Learning to use language. Berlin: de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L., Lahey, M., Hood, L., Lifter, K., & Fiess, K. (1980). Complex sentences: Acquisition of syntactic connectives and the semantic relations they encode. Journal of Child Language, 7, 235256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braunwald, S. R. (1985). The development of connectives. Journal of Pragmatics, 9, 513525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, C. A., Hunt, A. K., & Linton, M. J. (1988). Medium effects on children’s story rewriting and story retelling. First Language, 8, 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, J. (1985). Some problems and purposes in narrative analysis in educational research. Journal of Education, 167, 5770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (1981). The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – Revised: Manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Edwards, A. D. (1976). Speech codes and speech variants: Social class and task differences in children’s speech. Journal of Child Language, 3, 247265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feagans, L., & Applebaum, M. I. (1986). Validation of language subtypes in learning disabled children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 358364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feagans, L., & Short, E. J. (1984). Developmental differences in the comprehension and production of narratives by reading-disabled and normally achieving children. Child Development, 55, 17271736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fine, J. (1985). Cohesion as an index of social-cognitive factors: Oral language of the reading disabled. Discourse Processes, 8, 91112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, A. (1988). Language acquisition in Down’s syndrome children: Syntax and morphology. In Cicchetti, D. & Beeghly, M. (Eds.), Down syndrome: The developmental perspective. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gee, J. P. (1989). Two styles of narrative construction and their linguistic and educational implications. Discourse Processes, 12, 287307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, M. H. (1982). “Instigating”: Storytelling as social process. American Ethnologist, 9, 799819.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1981). The grammatical marking of thematic structure in the development of language production. In Deutsch, W. (Ed.), The child’s construction of language (pp. 121147). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1985). Language and cognitive processes from a developmental perspective. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1, 6185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemper, S. (1984). The development of narrative skills: Explanations and entertainments. In Kuczaj, S. A. (Ed.), Discourse development (pp. 99124). New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemper, S., & Edwards, L. L. (1986). Children’s expression of causality and their construction of narratives. Topics in Language Disorders, 7, 1120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kernan, K. T., & Sabsay, S. (1982). Semantic deficiencies in the narratives of mildly retarded speakers. Semiotica, 42, 169193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kernan, K. T., & Sabsay, S. (1987). Referential first mention in narratives by mildly mentally retarded adults. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 8, 361369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacWhinney, B. (1989). CHAT manual. Pittsburgh: Carnegie-Mellon University.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B., & Snow, C. (1985). The child language data exchange system. Journal of Child Language, 12, 271296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mandler, J. (1984). Stories, scripts, and scenes: Aspects of schema theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? New York: Dial.Google Scholar
McGann, W., & Schwartz, A. (1988). Main character in children’s narratives. Linguistics, 26, 215233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menig-Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1978). Children’s orientation of a listener to the context of their narratives. Developmental Psychology, 14, 582592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merritt, D. D., & Liles, B. Z. (1989). Narrative analysis: Clinical applications of story generation and story retelling. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 438447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, P. J., & Sperry, L. L. (1988). Early talk about the past: The origins of conversational stories of personal experience. Journal of Child Language, 15, 293315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, K., & Gruendel, J. (1981). Generalized event representations: Basic building blocks of cognitive development. In Brown, A. L. & Lamb, M. E. (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 131158). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nwokah, E. E. (1982). “Once upon a time” – Aspects of storytelling in normal and retarded children. Language and Speech, 25, 293298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, D., & Terranee, N. (1981). Learning to meet the requirements of written text: Language development in the school years. In Fredericksen, C. & Dominic, J. (Eds.), Writing: The nature, development, and teaching of written communication: Vol. 2. Writing: Process, development, and communications (pp. 235255). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child’s narrative. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rondal, J. (1988). Down’s syndrome. In Bishop, D. & Mogford, K. (Eds.), Language development in exceptional circumstances (pp. 165176). Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone.Google Scholar
Roth, F. P., & Spekman, J. J. (1986). Narrative discourse: Spontaneously generated stories of learning disabled and normally achieving students. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 51, 823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scollon, R. (1976). Conversations with a one year old. Honolulu: University Press of Hawaii.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, C. M. (1984). Adverbial connectivity in conversations of children six to twelve. Journal of Child Language, 11, 423452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sleight, C. C. & Prinz, P. M. (1985). Use of abstracts, orientations, and codas in narration by language-disordered and nondisordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 50, 361371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E. (1983). Literacy and language: Relationships during the preschool years. Harvard Educational Review, 53, 165189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snyder, L. S. (1984). Communicative competence in children with delayed language development. In Schiefelbusch, R. L. & Pickar, J. (Eds.), The acquisition of communicative competence (Language Intervention Series: Vol. 8, pp. 423478). Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Stenning, K., & Michell, L. (1985). Learning how to tell a good story: The development of content and language in children’s telling of one tale. Discourse Processes, 8, 261279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Umiker-Sebeok, D. J. (1979). Preschool children’s intraconversational narratives. Child Language, 6, 91109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Villaume, S. K. (1988). Creating context within text: An investigation of primary-grade children’s character introductions in original stories. Research in the Teaching of English, 22, 161182.Google Scholar
Weaver, P. A., & Dickinson, D. K. (1979). Story comprehension and recall in dyslexic students. Bulletin of the Orion Society, 29, 157171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolf, D., & Hicks, D. (1989). The voices within narratives: The development of intertextuality. Discourse Processes, 12, 329351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar