Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T22:35:11.608Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Solitary sound play during acquisition of English vocalizations by an African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus): Possible parallels with children’s monologue speech

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Irene M. Pepperberg*
Affiliation:
Northwestern University
Katherine J. Brese
Affiliation:
Northwestern University
Barbara J. Harris
Affiliation:
Northwestern University
*
Irene M. Pepperberg, Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Biological Sciences Building West, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ 85721

Abstract

An African Grey parrot, Alex, who had learned to use English speech in studies on referential interspecies communication and animal cognition, produced English monologues in both the presence and absence of human receivers. This study examines one component of Alex’s monologue behavior, private speech, while he was being taught new vocalizations. His private speech during those time periods included a small percentage of novel utterances, not yet used in the presence of his caretakers, that were phonologically related to, but not exact reproductions of, the new vocalizations. His monologues also contained utterances that were part of the general daily routine as well as the specific training paradigm, but rarely included verbatim reproductions of the training scenario. Alex’s behaviors were comparable to those of children in the early stages of language acquisition. Because monologue behavior has been characterized as a form of practice that facilitates human language development, the data are discussed in terms of the possible functions of monologues during Alex’s acquisition of novel vocalizations.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1991

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baldwin, J. M. (1914). Deferred imitation in West African Gray parrots. Proceedings of the IXth International Congress of Zoology, Monaco (pp. 536537).Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1971). Analysis of modeling procedures. In Bandura, A. (Ed.), Psychological modeling (pp. 162). Chicago: Aldine-Atherton.Google Scholar
Baptista, L. F. (1983). Song learning. In Brush, A. H. & Clark, G. A. Jr, (Eds.), Perspectives in ornithology (pp. 500506). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Black, R. (1979). Crib talk and mother-child interaction: A comparison of form and function. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 17, 9097.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1970). Language development: Form and function in emerging grammars. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1982). Reorganizational processes in language development. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. (Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art (pp. 319346). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Britton, J. (1970). Language and learning. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1983). Child’s talk: Learning to use language. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Collis, G. M. (1981). Social interaction with objects: A perspective on human infancy. In Immelmami, K., Barlow, G. W., Petrinovich, L., & Main, M. (Eds.), Behavioral development (pp. 603620). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Craig, H. K., & Gallagher, T. M. (1979). The structural characteristics of monologues in the speech of normal children: Syntactic nonconversational aspects. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 22, 4662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cross, T. G. (1977). Mother’s speech adjustments: The contribution of selected child listener variables. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 151188). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Davis, H. (1984). Discrimination of the number three by a raccoon (Procyon lotor). Animal Learning and Behavior, 4, 121124.Google Scholar
Davis, H., & Bradford, S. A. (1986). Counting behavior in rats in a simulated natural environment. Ethology, 73, 265280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodd, B. J. (1972). Effects of social and vocal stimulation on infant babbling. Developmental Psychology, 7, 8083.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dore, J. (1987, 10). An analysis of crib monologues from 22 months to three years. Paper presented at the 12th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Dore, J. (1989). Monologue as reenvoicement of dialogue. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Narratives from the crib (pp. 231260). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Feldman, C. F. (1989). Monologue as problem-solving narrative. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Narratives from the crib (pp. 98119). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Fey, M. E. (1986). Language intervention with young children. San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.Google Scholar
Furrow, D. (1984). Social and private speech at two years. Child Development, 55, 355362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furrow, D., & Nelson, K. (1986). A further look at the motherese hypothesis: A reply to Gleitman, Newport, & Gleitman. Journal of Child Language, 13, 163176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuson, K. (1979). The development of self-regulating aspects of speech: A review. In Zivon, G. (Ed.), The development of self-regulation through private speech. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Gallagher, T. M., & Craig, H. K. (1978). Structural characteristics of monologues in the speech of normal children: Semantics and conversational aspects. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 21, 103117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garvey, C. (1977a). Play. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Garvey, C. (1977b). Play with language and speech. In Ervin-Tripp, S. & Mitchel-Keenan, R. (Eds.), Child discourse. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gleitman, L. R., Newport, E. L., & Gleitman, H. (1984). The current status of the motherese hypothesis. Journal of Child Language, 11, 4379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, J., Jones, D., Brookes, S., & Grant, J. (1986). Relations between the non-verbal context of maternal speech and rate of language development. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 261268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurlock, E. (1934). Experimental investigations of childhood play. Psychological Bulletin, 31, 4766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocker, J. (1974). The origins of language comprehension. In Solso, R. L. (Ed.), Theories in cognitive psychology: The Loyola symposium (pp. 331368). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Koegel, R. L., Dyer, K., & Bell, L. K. (1987). The influence of child-preferred activities on autistic children’s social behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 243252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krashen, S. D. (1976). Formal and informal linguistic environments in language learning and language acquisition. TESOL Quarterly, 10, 157168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krashen, S. D. (1980). The input hypothesis. In Alatis, J. E. (Ed.), Current issues in bilingual education (pp. 168180). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Krashen, S. D. (1982). Principles and practices in second language acquisition. Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Kroodsma, D. E. (1974). Song learning, dialects, and dispersal in the Bewick’s wren. Zeit-schrift für Tierpsychologie, 35, 352380.Google ScholarPubMed
Krashen, S. D. (1986). Design of song playback experiments. Auk, 103, 640642.Google Scholar
Krashen, S. D. (1988). Song types and their use: Developmental flexibility of the blue-winged warbler. Ethology, 79, 235247.Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A. (1977). The acquisition of regular and irregular past tense forms. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 589600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A. (1982a). Language play and language acquisition. In Reese, H. (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A. (1982b). On the nature of syntactic development. In Kuczaj, S. A. (Ed.), Language development: Syntax and semantics. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A. (1983). Crib speech and language play. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A., & Bean, A. (1982). The development of non-communicative speech systems. In Kuczaj, S. (Ed.), Language development: Language, thought, and culture. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lambert, W. E. (1981). Bilingualism and language acquisition. In Winitz, H. (Ed.), Native language and foreign language acquisition (Vol. 379, pp. 922). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Lock, A. (1980). The guided reinvention of language. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Marler, P. (1970). A comparative approach to vocal learning: Song development in white-crowned sparrows. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 71, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P., & Peters, S. (1982a). Developmental overproduction and selective attrition: New processes in the epigenesis of birdsong. Developmental Psychobiology, 15, 369378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marler, P., & Peters, S. (1982b). Subsong and plastic song: Their role in the vocal learning process. In Kroodsma, D. E. & Miller, E. H. (Eds.), Acoustic communication in birds: Vol. 2. Song learning and its consequences (pp. 2550). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Marler, P., & Pickert, R. (1984). Species-universal microstructure in the learned song of the swamp sparrow (Melospiza georgiana). Animal Behaviour, 32, 673689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moran, G., Joch, E., & Sorenson, L. (1983, 06). The response of meerkats (Suricata suricatta) to changes in olfactory cues on established scent posts. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Animal Behavior Society, Lewisburg, PA.Google Scholar
Mowrer, O. H. (1960). Learning theory and symbolic processes. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1981). Individual differences in language development: Implications for development and language. Developmental Psychology, 17, 170187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1989). Monologues in the crib. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Narratives from the crib (pp. 123). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. E. (1978, 08). Toward a rare-event cognitive comparison theory of syntax acquisition. Paper presented at the 1st International Congress for the Study of Child Language, Tokyo; cited in Kuczaj, 1983.Google Scholar
Nottebohm, F. (1970). Ontogeny of bird song. Science, 167, 950956.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nottebohm, F., & Nottebohm, M. (1969). The parrots of Bush Bush. Animal Kingdom, 72, 1923.Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. S. (1981). The development of preschoolers’ private speech. Journal of Pragmatics, 5, 445458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1981). Functional vocalizations by an African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 55, 139160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1983). Cognition in the African Grey parrot: Preliminary evidence for auditory/vocal comprehension of the class concept. Animal Learning and Behavior, 11, 179185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1987a). Acquisition of the same/different concept by an African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus): Learning with respect to categories of color, shape, and material. Animal Learning and Behavior, 15, 423432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1987b). Evidence for conceptual quantitative abilities in the African Grey parrot: Labeling of cardinal sets. Ethology, 75, 3761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1987c). Interspecies communication: A tool for assessing conceptual abilities in the African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). In Greenberg, G. & Tobach, E. (Eds.), Language, cognition, and consciousness: Integrative levels (pp. 3156). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1988a). An interactive modeling technique for acquisition of communication skills: Separation of ‘labeling’ and ‘requesting’ in a psittacine subject. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 5976.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1988b). Comprehension of “absence” by an African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus): Learning with respect to questions of same/different. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 553564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1988c). The importance of social interaction and observation in the acquisition of communicative competence: Possible parallels between avian and human learning. In Zentall, T. R. & Galef, B. G. (Eds.), Social learning: A comparative approach (pp. 279299). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1990a). A communicative approach to animal cognition: Some conceptual abilities of an African Grey parrot. In Ristau, C. (Ed.), The minds of other animals (pp. 156187). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1990b). Referential mapping: Attaching functional significance to the innovative utterances of an African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 2344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M., & Kozak, F. A. (1986). Object permanence in the African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Animal Learning & Behavior, 14, 322330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M., & Matias, D. (1983). Transcripts of tapes of Alex. Unpublished data.Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M., Matias, D., Brese, K. J., & Conn, D. (1988). Transcripts of tapes of Alex. Unpublished data.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1951). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1954). Language and thought from the genetic point of view. Ada Psychologie, 10, 5160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1966). Psychology of intelligence. Totawa, NJ: Littlefield, Adams.Google Scholar
Pickert, S. (1981). Imaginative dialogues in children’s private speech. First Language, 2, 520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putney, R. T. (1985). Do willful apes know what they are aiming at? Psychological Record, 35, 4962.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynolds, P. (1976). Play, language, and human evolution. In Bruner, J., Jolly, A., & Sylva, K. (Eds.), Play: Its role in development and evolution. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rummelhart, D., & Norman, D. (1978). Accretion, tuning, and restructuring: Three modes of learning. In Cooton, J. & Klatzky, R. (Eds.), Semantic factors in cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ryan, J. (1973). Interpretation and imitation in early language development. In Hinde, R. A. & Stephenson-Hinde, J. (Eds.), Constraints on learning. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Scollon, R. (1976). Conversation with a one-year old. Honolulu: University Press of Hawaii.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1969). Speech acts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shatz, M. (1982). On mechanisms of language acquisition: Can features of the communicative environment account for development? In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R. (Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art (pp. 102127). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1979). The role of social interaction in language acquisition. In Collins, W. A.(Ed.), Children’s language and communication (pp. 157182). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stern, C., & Stern, W. (1922). Die Kindersprache. Leipzig: Barth.Google Scholar
Todt, D. (1975). Social learning of vocal patterns and modes of their applications in Grey parrots. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 39, 178188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velleman, S. (1987, 10). Mother-child interactions: A longitudinal microanalysis. Paper presented at the 12th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1966). Play and its role in the mental development of the child. Soviet Psychology, 12, 6276.Google Scholar
Weeks, T. E. (1979). Born to talk. Rowley, MA: Newbury.Google Scholar
Weir, R. H. (1962). Language in the crib. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
West, M. J., & King, A. P. (1985). Social guidance of vocal learning by female cowbirds: Validating its functional significance. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 70, 225235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoder, P. J., & Kaiser, A. P. (1987, 10). Exploring the indirect routes by which maternal speech predicts later child language development. Paper presented at the 12th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, MA.Google Scholar