Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T12:04:05.295Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Some influences on the grammar of English- and Italian-speaking children with specific language impairment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Laurence B. Leonard*
Affiliation:
Purdue University
Letizia Sabbadini
Affiliation:
USL RMI, Rome
Virginia Volterra
Affiliation:
Istituto di Psicologia, CNR, Rome
Jeanette S. Leonard
Affiliation:
Purdue University
*
Laurence B. Leonard Audiology & Speech Sciences, Heavilon Hall, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN 47907, U.S.A.

Abstract

The spontaneous speech of both English-speaking (E) and Italian-speaking (I) children with specific language impairment (SLI) was examined to determine (a) whether phonological factors influence the grammatical morpheme use of ISLI children, as has been found for ESLI children, and (b) whether ESLI and ISLI children show similar syntactic abilities at the same level of mean utterance length as measured in words. The results indicated that word-final consonants adversely influenced the ISLI children's tendency to use articles – the only Italian grammatical morphemes in which word-final consonants are required. There was no evidence of syntactic differences between the ESLI and ISLI children. However, both groups of children seemed to have a problem using morphemes that constituted unstressed elements in a sentence even though the grammatical and semantic function of these elements varied across the two languages. The findings suggest that a speech production or perception component may be playing a greater role than previously believed in contributing to SLI children's well-documented expressive grammatical difficulties, though the specific effects of this factor will vary as a function of the surface characteristics of the language being acquired.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1988

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Antinucci, F., & Parisi, D. (1973). Early language acquisition: A model and some data. In Ferguson, C. & Slobin, D. (Eds.), Studies of child language development (pp. 607619). New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.Google Scholar
Arthur, G. (1952). The Arthur Adaptation of the Leiter International Performance Scale. Chicago: Stoelting.Google Scholar
Bates, E. (1976). Language and context. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Boncori, A., & Pizzamiglio, L. (1980). Valutazione delle capacità cognitive in bambini normali e handicappati. Unpublished manuscript, E.I.S.S.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calleri, D. (1986). L'acquisizione dei pronomi clitici: Verifica in testi prodotti da bambini in età prescolare. Unpublished paper, Università di Torino.Google Scholar
Carrow, E. (1973). Test for Auditory Comprehension of Language. Boston, MA: Teaching Resources.Google Scholar
Cousins, A. (1979). Grammatical morpheme development in an aphasie child: A longitudinal study. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University.Google Scholar
Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – Revised.Google Scholar
Fletcher, P., & Peters, J. (1981). Verb-forms in normal and language-impaired children. Paper presented at the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Convention, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
French, J. (1964). Pictorial Test of Intelligence. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (1972). The acquisition of the English verbal auxiliary and copula in normal and linguistically deviant children. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 4, 7991.Google Scholar
Johnston, J. (1982). The language disordered child. In Lass, N., Northern, J., Yoder, D., & McReynolds, L. (Eds.), Speech, language, and hearing (pp. 780801). Philadelphia: Saunders.Google Scholar
Johnston, J., & Kamhi, A. (1984). Syntactic and semantic aspects of the utterances of language-impaired children: The same can be less. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 30, 6585.Google Scholar
Johnston, J., & Schery, T. (1976). The use of grammatical morphemes by children with communication disorders. In Morehead, D. & Morehead, A. (Eds.), Normal and deficient child language (pp. 239258). Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Kaplan, R., & Bresnan, J. (1982). Lexical-functional grammar: A formal system for grammatical representation. In Bresnan, J. (Ed.), The mental representation of grammatical relations (pp. 173281). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Laicardi, C. (1984). Validazione degli items di una prova di comprensione sintattica per bambini da 4 a 6 anni. Psicologia Clinica, 2, 247257.Google Scholar
Lee, L. (1966). Developmental sentence types: A method for comparing normal and deviant syntactic development. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 31, 311330.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. (1972). What is deviant language? Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 37, 427446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. (1982). The nature of specific language impairment. In Rosenberg, S. (Ed.), Handbook of applied psycholinguistics (pp. 295327). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Leonard, L., Sabbadini, L., Leonard, J., & Volterra, V. (1987). Specific language impairment in children: A cross-linguistic study. Brain and Language, 32, 233252.Google Scholar
Leonard, L., Schwartz, R., Chapman, K., Rowan, L., Prelock, P., Terrell, B., Weiss, A., & Messick, C. (1982). Early lexical acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 25, 554564.Google Scholar
Maratsos, M. (in press). Cross-linguistic analysis, universals, and language acquisition. In Essays in honor of Roger Brown.Google Scholar
Menyuk, P. (1964). Comparison of grammar of children with functionally deviant and normal speech. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 7, 109121.Google Scholar
Miller, J., & Chapman, R. (1983). Systematic analysis of language transcripts. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin.Google Scholar
Morehead, D., & Ingram, D. (1973). The development of base syntax in normal and linguistically deviant children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 16, 330352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newcomer, P., & Hammill, D. (1977). The Test of Language Development-Primary. Austin, TX: Empiric Press.Google Scholar
Parisi, D., & Antinucci, F. (1976). Essentials of grammar. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Paul, R., & Shriberg, L. (1982). Associations between phonology and syntax in speech-delayed children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 25, 536547.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In Ferguson, C. & Slobin, D. (Eds.), Studies of child language development (pp. 175208). New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. (1985). Crosslinguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. In Slobin, D. (Ed.), The crosstinguistic study of language acquisition. Volume 2: Theoretical issues (pp. 11571249). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale. (1960). Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Templin, M. (1957). Certain language skills in children. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Valian, V. (1986). Syntactic categories in the speech of young children. Developmental Psychology, 22, 562579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volterra, V. (1972). II “no.” Prime fasi di sviluppo della negazione nel linguaggio infantile. Archivio di Psicologia, Neurologia e Psichiatria, 33, 1653.Google Scholar
Volterra, V., Leonard, L., Campagnoli, M., Leonard, J., & Sabbadini, L. (forthcoming). The acquisition of morphology in Italian.Google Scholar
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Revised. (1974). New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence. (1967). New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Zardini, A. (1985). Lo sviluppo della comprensione verbale in età prescolare: Una prova di identificazione di figure nella valutazione di strutture grammaticali. Paper presented at the Simposio di Neurologia dello Sviluppo, Milano.Google Scholar