Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-11T04:30:03.426Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Are schizophrenics more religious? Do they have more daughters?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 June 2008

Satoshi Kanazawa
Affiliation:
Interdisciplinary Institute of Management, London School of Economics and Political Science, Houghton Street, London WC2A 2AE Department of Psychology, University College London, London WC1E 6BT Department of Psychology, Birkbeck College, University of London, London WC1E 7HX, United Kingdom. S.Kanazawa@lse.ac.ukhttp://www.lse.ac.uk/collections/MES/people/Kanazawa.htm

Abstract

Combined with recent evolutionary psychological theories, Crespi & Badcock's (C&B's) intragenomic conflict theory of the social brain suggests that schizophrenics are more religious, and autistics are less religious, than the normal population. Combined with the generalized Trivers-Willard hypothesis (gTWH), it suggests that schizophrenics have more daughters, and autistics have more sons, than expected.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Atran, S. (2002) In gods we trust: The evolutionary landscape of religion. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (2003) The essential difference: The truth about the male and female brain. Basic Books/Penguin.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Bolton, P., Wheelwright, S., Scahill, V., Short, L., Mead, G. & Smith, A. (1998) Autism occurs more often in families of physicists, engineers, and mathematicians. Autism 2:296301.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2001) Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Guthrie, S. E. (1993) Faces in the clouds: A new theory of religion. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Haselton, M. G. & Nettle, D. (2006) The paranoid optimist: An integrative evolutionary model of cognitive biases. Personality and Social Psychology Review 10:4766.Google Scholar
Kanazawa, S. (2005) Big and tall parents have more sons: Further generalizations of the Trivers-Willard hypothesis. Journal of Theoretical Biology 235:583–90.Google Scholar
Kanazawa, S. (2006) Violent men have more sons: Further evidence for the generalized Trivers-Willard hypothesis (gTWH). Journal of Theoretical Biology 239:450–59.Google Scholar
Kanazawa, S. (2007a) Beautiful parents have more daughters: A further implication of the generalized Trivers-Willard hypothesis (gTWH). Journal of Theoretical Biology 244:133–40.Google Scholar
Kanazawa, S. (2007b) Big and tall soldiers are more likely to survive battle: A possible explanation for the “returning soldier effect” on the secondary sex ratio. Human Reproduction 22:30023008.Google Scholar
Kanazawa, S. & Vandermassen, G. (2005) Engineers have more sons, nurses have more daughters: An evolutionary psychological extension of Baron-Cohen's extreme male brain theory of autism and its empirical implications. Journal of Theoretical Biology 233:589–99.Google Scholar
Kirkpatrick, L. A. (2005) Attachment, evolution, and the psychology of religion. Guilford.Google Scholar
McNamara, P. (2001) Religion and the frontal lobes. In: Religion in mind: Cognitive perspectives on religious belief, ritual, and experience, ed. Andresen, J., pp. 237–56. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Miller, A. S. & Kanazawa, S. (2007) Why beautiful people have more daughters. Perigee.Google Scholar
Miller, A. S. & Stark, R. (2002) Gender and religiousness: Can socialization explanations be saved? American Journal of Sociology 107:13991423.Google Scholar
Tallal, P., Ross, R. & Curtiss, S. (1989) Unexpected sex-ratios in families of language/learning-impaired children. Neuropsychologia 27:987–98.Google Scholar